Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 10 May 2021
Sec. Social Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Hallucinations from the Perspective of Altered Experiences of Self: A Multidisciplinary Approach View all 9 articles

The Sensory and Perceptual Scaffolding of Absorption, Inner Speech, and Self in Psychosis

\nCherise Rosen
Cherise Rosen1*Michele TufanoMichele Tufano1Clara S. HumpstonClara S. Humpston2Kayla A. ChaseKayla A. Chase1Nev JonesNev Jones3Amy C. AbramowitzAmy C. Abramowitz1Ann Franco ChakkalakalAnn Franco Chakkalakal1Rajiv P. SharmaRajiv P. Sharma1
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 2School of Psychology, Institute for Mental Health, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, United States

This study examines the interconnectedness between absorption, inner speech, self, and psychopathology. Absorption involves an intense focus and immersion in mental imagery, sensory/perceptual stimuli, or vivid imagination that involves decreased self-awareness and alterations in consciousness. In psychosis, the dissolution and permeability in the demarcation between self and one's sensory experiences and perceptions, and also between self-other and/or inter-object boundaries alter one's sense of self. Thus, as the individual integrates these changes new “meaning making” or understanding evolves as part of an ongoing inner dialogue and dialogue with others. This study consisted of 117 participants: 81 participants with psychosis and 36 controls. We first conducted a bivariate correlation to elucidate the relationship between absorption and inner speech. We next conducted hierarchical multiple regressions to examine the effect of absorption and inner speech to predict psychopathology. Lastly, we conducted a network analysis and applied extended Bayesian Information Criterion to select the best model. We showed that in both the control and psychosis group dialogic and emotional/motivational types of inner speech were strongly associated with absorption subscales, apart from the aesthetic subscale in the control group which was not significant, while in psychosis, condensed inner speech was uniquely associated with increased imaginative involvement. In psychosis, we also demonstrated that altered consciousness, dialogic, and emotional/motivational inner speech all predicted positive symptoms. In terms of network associations, imaginative involvement was the most central, influential, and most highly predictive node in the model from which all other nodes related to inner speech and psychopathology are connected. This study shows a strong interrelatedness between absorption, inner speech and psychosis thus identifying potentially fertile ground for future research and directions, particularly in the exploration into the underlying construct of imaginative involvement in psychotic symptoms.

Introduction

There is no immaculate perception” Nitetzche (1)

The interior world is a dynamic and complex source of sensory and perceptual experiences in which the individual constructs and deconstructs the ontological dimensions of self and environment. Touch (tactile systems), sight (visual systems), hearing (auditory systems), smell (olfactory systems), and taste (gustatory systems) are sensory experiences that do not exist in isolation, but rather are a multisensory construct that are influenced by multiple factors. For example, color perception is shaped by multiple determinants that include biological factors (2), cultural factors (3), emotional factors (4), environmental factors, as shown in the case of the blue-black or white-gold dress (5), and priors (6). Color, for e.g., can also influence tactile (7), auditory (8), olfactory (9, 10), and gustatory sensory systems (11). Perception is the subjective process by which individuals interpret these sensory inputs based on prior knowledge. Consequently, when all influencing factors are considered, subjective sensory experience is different between individuals, resulting in unique perceptual worlds (12).

For many, during the psychosis prodrome, alterations in the alignment of sensory and perceptual experiences emerge. It is within this attenuated state that the primary dimensions of psychosis assemble themselves. Phenomenological descriptions of early psychosis often focus on changes in sensory experiences that are heightened, making it difficult for the individual to filter out irrelevant and unwanted stimuli, while at the same time simultaneously drawing the person into an immersive fractal experience of multisensory and perceptual changes (13, 14). As defined in our earlier work, the construct of absorption “describes a state of immersion in (or capture by) mental imagery or perceptual stimuli and correlates with vivid imagination or fantasy. During a period of intense absorption ‘objects of attention' are hypothesized to ‘acquire an importance and intimacy that are normally reserved for the self and may therefore acquire a self-like quality' (14, 15).” In the early stages of self-disturbance, the pre-reflective and uncanny feeling is interwoven with a new reality yet to be revealed; “….it is ‘as if' I am looking at reality with other eyes, it almost seems as if I am awakening.” (16).

As the sensory and perceptual scaffolding is fundamentally altered, the architectural boundaries of the interior and exterior distinctions deconstruct. The dissolution and permeability of the demarcation between self and one's sensory experiences and perceptions, between self-other and/or inter-object boundaries alter one's sense of self. Thus, as the individual integrates these changes new ‘meaning making' or understanding evolves: “Everything I see is split up. It's like a photograph that's torn in bits and put together again. If somebody moves or speaks, everything I see disappears quickly and I have to put it together” (1720). These early and complex phenomenological alterations in sensory and perceptual inputs as well as cognitive, bodily, and social experiences are associated with altered self-experience as a core element from which the emergence of psychosis extends (21, 22).

Sense of self and “meaning making” of alterations in sensory and perceptual experiences in psychosis are “dialogical” processes consisting of ongoing inner dialogue/speech and dialogue with others. Inner-speech is defined as a conversation that occurs internally. Inner-speech in participants with psychosis has been shown to be differentially expressed in the presence of sensory and perceptual experiences, when compared to non-clinical controls (23, 24). Theoretically, Dialogical Self Theory (DST), posits self as the narrator in which the “I-position” bridges both inner dialogue/speech with oneself and outer dialogue with others (25, 26). There are multiple forms of inner speech. For example, dialogic inner speech involves an internal conversation with self; condensed inner speech the dialogue is abbreviated; other's voices that occur in inner speech are thoughts heard in the voice of another person; and the evaluative/motivational characteristics of inner speech (2730). It has been proposed that “the development of human existence is a dialectical process” that hinges on seemingly obvious assumptions or shared “common sense” which in the presence of anomalous experiences the obvious becomes questionable, leaving one's self to make inferences about the experience (3134). Common sense has been described as “the shared background that makes it possible to live in time without slipping into madness. This background in not so much a collection of statements of knowledge as it is a practice of habits, modes of behavior, and attitudes that together form the substructure, or the framework itself…(35).”

As with the experience of viewing color, the vast heterogeneity of psychosis is also influenced by biological, cultural, emotional, environmental factors, and priors. The primary dimensions of psychosis include the positive symptom dimension of hallucinations, delusions, formal thought disorder; the negative symptom dimension of affect flattening, avolition, and alogia; and the cognitive dimension of disorganization, abstract thinking and attention (3638). These heterogeneities in psychotic experience and dimensions (e.g., a manic psychotic experience being often markedly different in its phenomenology from a “bizarre delusional state” or traumatogenic voices) are critical to understanding associations between absorption, inner speech and psychosis –almost certainly they interact with (again just for example) a heightened manic-grandiose state in a different way from what would otherwise happen with bizarre delusions which is yet again different from ongoing trauma-related voices or intrusions. This study provides the groundwork by which future studies can further examine the nuances of these interactions.

Study Aims

The primary aims of this study were to examine the phenomenological construct of sensory and perceptual experiences within the framework of absorption and inner speech in participants experiencing present state psychosis compared to non-clinical controls. We investigated the following questions:

(1) What is the relationship between absorption and inner speech in participants with present-state psychosis compared to a non-clinical control group?

(2) Is there an association between alterations in sensory and perceptual experiences, absorption and inner speech in psychosis?

(3) Does absorption and/or inner speech predict positive, negative or cognitive disorganization in participants with psychosis?

(4) What are the interactions between network components of absorption, inner speech, and psychopathology?

Methods

Participants

The University of Illinois at Chicago Internal Review Board (IRB2012-0113) approved the study and all procedures were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (39). All participants gave signed written informed consent prior to the initiation of study procedures.

There were 117 participants in the study for whom all research measures were obtained. The clinical sample consisted of 81 participants, of whom 54 were diagnosed with schizophrenia and 27 were diagnosed with bipolar disorder with psychosis. The non-clinical control group (NCC; n = 36) did not meet the Structured Clinical Diagnostic Interview [SCID-IV; (40)] criteria for a current or past major psychiatric disorder. The study exclusion criteria included current substance dependence, seizure disorders, current pregnancy, and neurological conditions. Demographics were obtained at baseline. Duration of untreated psychosis (DUP) was defined as the number of years between onset of psychosis and initiation of antipsychotic medication. Duration of illness was defined as the number of years between onset of illness and current age. IQ was measured using the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) (41).

Measures Used to Evaluate the Relationship Between Sensory and Perceptual Alterations, Absorption, and Inner Speech

The primary measures of this study aimed to evaluate sensory and perceptual alterations, absorption, and inner speech that included the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS), Tellegen Absorption Scale (TAS), and the Varieties of Inner Speech Questionnaire (VISQ).

Measure Used to Evaluate Sensory and Perceptual Experiences

Psychopathology was assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (42, 43). PANSS items were scored along a continuum of severity between 1 (asymptomatic) to 7 (extreme symptom severity). Scores were calculated for three-factors: Positive symptoms (POS; “delusions, hallucinatory behavior, suspiciousness or persecution, unusual thought content”), Negative symptoms (NEG; “blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive/apathetic social withdrawal, lack of spontaneity and flow of conversation, and active social avoidance”), Cognitive Disorganization (COG; “conceptual disorganization, difficulty in abstract thinking, and poor attention”). Internal consistency for the PANSS subscales used in this study α's were between 0.84 and 0.93.

Measure Used to Evaluate Absorption

Absorption was measured using the Tellegen Absorption Scale (TAS). The TAS is a 34 item true/false questionnaire designed to measure levels of mental involvement with the object of experience (14, 44, 45). Absorption was formulated in terms of “total attention involving a full commitment of available perceptual, motoric, imaginative and ideational resources to a unified representation of attentional object.” The TAS consists of five subscales that include: synesthesia (TASSYN; “I find that different odors have different colors”), altered states of consciousness (TASACS; “I sometimes ‘step outside' my usual self and experience an entirely different state of being”), aesthetic involvement in nature (TASAN; “I like to watch cloud shapes change in the sky”), imaginative involvement (TASII; “I am able to wander off into my thoughts while doing a routine task and actually forget that I am doing the task, and then find a few minutes later that I have completed it”), and extrasensory perception or ESP (TASESP; “I can often somehow sense the presence of another person before I actually see her/him”). In the current study, internal consistency α's ranged from 0.63 to 0.91 for all TAS subscales and TAS total score.

Measure Used to Evaluate Varieties of Inner Speech

Varieties of Inner Speech Questionnaire [VISQ; (30)] is an 18-item self-report measure designed to evaluate the experience of inner speech. It measures four varieties of inner speech: dialogic (VISQDIS; “When I am talking to myself about things in my mind, it is like I am going back and forward asking myself questions and then answering them”); condensed (VISQCIS; “I think to myself in words using brief phrases and single words rather than full sentences”); other people (VISQOIS; “I hear other people's actual voices in my head, saying things that they actually once said to me”); and evaluative and motivational characteristics (VISQEIS; “I talk silently to myself telling myself not to do things”). Participants rate applicability of each item using on a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from “Certainly does not apply to me” (1) to “Certainly applies to me” (6). In the current study, internal consistency α's ranged from 0.55 to 0.90 for all VISQ subscales.

Data Analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS version 24.0 and all network analyses were conducting using the statistical programming language R in RStudio (Version 1.2.5033), using the mgm package (version 1.2–7), qgraph package (version 1.6.5), and bootnet package (version 1.4.3), as reported in Pappa et al. (46). Demographic data were analyzed using Chi-square tests to measure differences between groups in sex and race, analyses of variance (ANOVAs) was applied to measure differences in age and IQ between groups and independent sample t-tests were conducted to measure differences between the onset of illness, duration of untreated psychosis and duration of illness between schizophrenia and bipolar with psychosis groups. ANOVAs that yielded significant results were followed up with Bonferroni post-hoc tests to identify significant pair-wise group differences. Pearson's bivariate correlations with bootstrapping at 1,000 iterations were conducted to determine associations between TAS and VISQ subscales in the clinical and non-clinical groups. Hierarchical multiple linear regressions were used to examine the unique contribution of absorption and inner speech in the prediction of positive, negative, and cognitive disorganization in participants with present state psychosis. A two-step hierarchical multiple linear regression model was performed to predict the presence and severity of PANSS positive, negative, and cognitive disorganization subscale scores examining the relative contribution of the TAS scores (synesthesia, altered states of consciousness, aesthetic, imaginative, and ESP), and VISQ subscale scores (condensed inner speech, evaluative and motivational inner speech, and dialogic inner speech). There were a total of 81 subjects included in the analysis. The relevant assumptions of this statistical analysis were tested (47). The collinearity statistics were within accepted limits (48). Mahalanobis distance scores indicated no multivariate outliers. Residual and scatter plots indicated that the assumptions of normality, linearity and homoscedasticity were all satisfied (48).

Lastly, for the network analysis, we used the mgm package (version 1.2–7) to estimate the network model (49). The Mixed Graphical Models (MGMs) allow modeling of cross-sectional data with ℓ1-regularized (LASSO) neighborhood regression, which facilitates model interpretation by attributing the zero value to artificial edge-parameters (50, 51). We used Extended Bayesian Information Criterion to select the best model (52). The stability and reliability of the network model and its centrality indices were assessed with bootstrap method using the bootnet package. The network model and centrality indices were displayed using the qgraph package (53, 54).

Results

Descriptive Characteristics

Group comparisons of demographics revealed no statistically significant difference between sex, race, age and IQ between the clinical and NCC groups (See Table 1). There was no significant difference between clinical groups regarding age of onset of illness, duration of untreated psychosis and duration of illness. Thus, the clinical sample was combined for the remaining analysis.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of sample (n = 117).

Relationship Between Absorption and Inner Speech in Participants With Present-State Psychosis and Non-clinical Control Group

To elucidate the relationship between absorption and inner speech, we conducted a bivariate correlation with confidence intervals calculated at 1,000 iterations bootstrapped and found multiple positive associations. As reported in Supplementary Table 1, the combined sample associations were found in all domains between absorption and inner speech with the exception of VISQCIS which was only associated with TASII.

As shown in Figure 1A, the non-clinical sample shows a positive association between VISQDIS and TASSYN, TASACS, TASAN, TASII, and TASESP. Likewise, VISQEIS also showed a positive correlation with TASSYN, TASACS, TASAN, TASII, and TASESP. However, there was no association between VISQCIS and any of the TAS subscales.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Association between inner speech and absorption. Pearson's r correlations (95% confidence intervals) between Absorption and VISQ subscales. Confidence intervals calculated using 1,000 iterations bootstrapped sample. *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 (Blue bars indicate statistical significance). (A) The association between inner speech and absorption control sample. (B) The association between inner speech and absorption psychosis sample.

The clinical sample shared a similar pattern, as shown in Figure 1B, in that there was a positive association between VISQDIS and TASSYN, TASACS, TASAN, TASII, and TASESP. VISQEIS also showed a positive association with TASSYN, TASACS, TASAN, TASII, and TASESP. Uniquely, VISQCIS in the clinical sample was associated with TASII.

Predictive Dimensions of Absorption and Inner Speech With Positive, Negative, and Cognitive Disorganization in Participants With Psychosis

We conducted a hierarchical multiple regression to examine the effect of absorption and varieties of inner speech to predict increased levels of positive, negative, and cognitive disorganization symptoms in participants with psychosis (n = 81). For the purposes of this analysis, other people inner speech was removed from the analysis due to the potential confound between other people inner speech and internal auditory verbal hallucinations.

We first examined the predictive power of absorption and inner speech with positive symptoms as the dependent variable. In step one of the model, we entered aspects of absorption, as measured by TAS subscales; in step two of the model, we entered the three VISQ subscale scores. Hierarchical multiple regression indicated that at step one, the TASASC subscale scores contributed significantly to the regression model [F(5,75) = 2.69, p = 0.03, R2 = 0.152]. In step two of the model we entered the three VISQ subscale scores, which further significantly contributed to the model [ΔF(3,72) = 4.27, p ≤ 0.008, ΔR2 = 0.13]. The overall model was significant [F(8,72) = 3.504, p =0.002, R2 = 0.28]. When all independent variables were included in the regression model, TASACS (p = 0.03), VISQDIS (p = 0.01), and VISQCIS (p = 0.05) scores significantly predicted increased psychotic symptoms and this accounted for 28% of the variance in positive symptoms scores (See Supplementary Table 2).

We next examined the predictive power of absorption and inner speech with negative symptoms as the dependent variable. In step one, we entered subscale scores of absorption; in step two, we entered the VISQ subscale scores. The hierarchical multiple regression showed that at step one, absorption scores did not contribute significantly to the regression model [F(5,75) = 1.27, p = 0.29, R2 = 0.078]. In step two of the model we entered the three VISQ subscale scores, which also did not significantly contribute to the model [ΔF(3,72) = 2.31, p = 0.08, ΔR2 = 0.08]. The overall model was not significant [F(8,72) = 1.70, p = 0.011, R2 = 0.16].

Lastly, we examined the power of absorption and inner speech in predicting increased cognitive disorganization. In step one, we entered aspects of absorption as measured by TAS subscales; in step two, we entered the three VISQ subscale scores. The hierarchical multiple regression revealed that at step-one, TASESP (p = 0.008) scores contributed significantly to the regression model [F(5,75) = 2.5, p = 0.04, R2 = 0.14]. In step two of the model we entered the three VISQ subscale scores, which significantly contributed to the model [ΔF(3,73) = 4.06, p =0.01, ΔR2 = 0.12]. The overall model was significant [F(8,72) = 3.28, p = 0.003, R2 = 0.27]. When all independent variables were included in the regression model, TASESP (p = 0.01), VISQDIS (p = 0.001), and VISQEIS (p = 0.01) scores significantly predicted increased absorption and this model accounted for 27% of the variation in cognitive disorganization scores (See Supplementary Table 3).

Interactive Network Components of Absorption, Inner Speech, and Psychopathology

The main goal of this network analysis was to explore the centrality of the constructs of absorption, inner speech and psychopathology to potentially identify core features of interrelatedness. Network analysis uses the graph theory to analyze individual symptoms (paranoia, hallucinations, or depression) or constructs such as absorption, inner speech and psychopathology to reveal the most influential underlying structure (46, 5557). The network structure of absorption, inner speech, and psychopathology are presented in Figure 2. In the network model, each variable is represented by a node (illustrated as a dot), while each interaction among the nodes is represented by an edge (illustrated as a line). The probability of each node is calculated by estimating the nodes connecting to it (nodewise predictability). Additionally, the predictability of each node within the network is calculated. In this way, this method augments the reliability of the network structure (50). Nodes within the networks were determined using the force-directed Fruchterman-Reingold algorithm, which tends to group stronger nodes connections at the center of the network, in order to avoid edge crossing (58). Positive and negative interactions among nodes are displayed using the qgraph package and are represented in green and red edges, respectively, with the width of the edges proportional to the strength of each interaction. However, all the interactions among the nodes of the network are positive, thus no red edges/negative interactions are represented.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Interactive network for the components of absorption, inner speech, and psychopathology. MGM predictability interactive network for the components of absorption, inner speech, and psychopathology. Each node represents a variable, including aesthetic nature (AN), synesthesia (SYN), imaginative involvement (II), altered states of consciousness (ASC), extra sensory perception (ESP), negative symptoms (NEG), cognitive disorganization (COG), positive symptoms (POS), condensed inner speech (CIS), evaluative and motivational characteristics (EIS), and dialogic inner speech (DIS). Green lines connecting nodes symbolizes positive interactions, with the width of the edges proportional to the strength of each interaction. The proportion of each node's variance explained by all the other nodes in the network is shown by blue rings surrounding each node.

All nodes are included in the overall network structure. The contribution of condensed inner speech to the network structure is minimal. Psychopathology (cognitive disorganization, positive symptoms and negative symptoms) was most predictive of the network model of absorption (TASII, TASACS); absorption and inner speech (VISQEIS,VISQDIS) were also closely related with each other (Supplementary Table 4).

Indices included in the centrality analysis as noted in Figure 3 were: closeness, which quantifies the indirect strength of connection between a node to other nodes; betweenness, which quantifies the importance of each node for the interaction between two adjacent nodes; node strength, which is the sum of weighted correlation coefficient of all the interactions connected to a single node (54); and expected influence, which takes into account the negative edges to evaluate the strength and influence of each node within a network (59).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Centrality metrics intervals for the interactive network components of absorption, inner dialogue, and psychopathology. Centrality metrics intervals for the interactive network components of absorption, inner speech, and psychopathology, ordered according to the Expected Influence. II, Imaginative involvement; COG, cognitive disorganization; POS, positive symptoms; ASC, altered states of consciousness; NEG, negative symptoms; DIS, dialogic inner speech; EIS, evaluative and motivational characteristics; SYN, synesthesia; AN, aesthetic nature; ESP, extra sensory perception; CIS, condensed inner speech.

Interestingly imaginative involvement (TASII) was found to be the most central, influential, and most highly predictive node in the model from which all other nodes are connected. The proximity pathways of the nodes most closely related are TASII (1.38), altered states of consciousness (TASACS = 1.14), and positive symptoms (POS = 0.36). The nodes that predict the network model the most within the expected influence pathway are TASII (1.64), COG (0.72), POS (0.74), and TASACS (0.62).

Discussion

Think back to a time when you heard your name being called from across the room when no one was there, caught a fleeting image or shadow in the corner of your vision when nothing was there, felt a light sensation brush across your skin in the absence of external stimuli, or even briefly experienced a scent that could not be associated with the environment. Think about the shifting of salience you gave the experience, the feelings that accompanied the experience, the inner speech as your mind stacked up all the priors to make sense of the experience before you categorized it. Now imagine that the frequency, duration, and intensity of the experience increase, and thoughts turn inward in the search to reconstruct equilibrium. Alterations in the process of integrating sensory and perceptual experiences in psychosis are thought to be closely associated to the phenomenon of absorption or aberrant salience (60). The etiology of alterations in sensory and perceptual experiences fundamentally consist of an association between absorption, inner speech and dimensions of psychopathology that are not under direct or voluntary control, all of which are rooted in interactions between complex cultural, environmental, biological, and psychological substrates (33, 61, 62). In this paper we examined the associations between absorption, inner speech, and psychopathology in relation to self within the psychotic experience.

Our previous research has shown that absorption is more prevalent and intense in participants with psychosis when compared to non-clinical controls, and was significantly associated with hallucinations and delusions (15). We have also previously shown that dialogic inner speech is associated with psychosis (24). In this study we report that in participants with psychosis, dialogic inner speech and evaluative and motivational inner speech were associated with all factors of absorption whereas condensed inner speech was associated with imaginative involvement. We also report that altered states of consciousness, dialogic inner speech, and condensed inner speech predict increased positive symptoms; and that extrasensory perception, dialogic inner speech and evaluative and motivational inner speech predict both increased cognitive disorganization and negative symptoms. Other studies have shown that TAS and nonclinical psychotic assessments were structurally distinct (63). In terms of network associations and pathways, imaginative involvement was found to be the most central, influential, and most highly predictive node in the model from which all other nodes related to psychopathology and inner speech are connected.

Absorption is defined as a process by which the individual is immersed in mental imagery or perceptual stimuli, vivid imagination or fantasy. During a period of intense absorption “objects of […] attention” are believed to “acquire an importance and intimacy that are normally reserved for the self and may therefore acquire a self-like quality” (14, 64). Absorption is also associated with decreased self-awareness and increased alteration in state of consciousness (65). Intense absorption is closely related to basic self-disturbances, which are expressed in the dissolution of the boundaries between self and the interior and external worlds (6668). Alterations in sensory and perceptual experiences related to changes in self-experience are supported by phenomenological, experimental and neurophysiological data that suggest possible neurobiological underpinnings of a core disturbance (22, 69, 70). For example, the core network node, imaginative involvement, includes the endorsement of items along themes of losing connections to self and others while becoming internally immersed where one becomes oblivious to one's environment, experiencing thoughts as visual images, and attributing meaning to something that is otherwise meaningless. Imaginative involvement has been described as an element of absorption where the individual may become vividly immersed in their imagination that at times it can become difficult to distinguish imagination from reality (71). Additionally, imaginative involvement can include the experience of “loss of time” or the feeling of performing in “auto-pilot,” which have been linked to automatic features of dissociation that can also be experienced in psychosis: “I am able to wander off into my thoughts while doing a routine task and actually forget that I am doing the task, and them find a few minutes later that I have completed it” (72, 73). The nodes most closely related to imaginative involvement are altered states of consciousness and positive symptoms. This central pathway further supports the blueprint of interconnection between alterations in sense of self, as is seen in basic symptoms, increased absorption and psychosis.

The network node imaginative involvement is also loosely associated to dialogic inner speech, evaluative and motivational inner speech, and condensed inner speech. Yet the association between absorption and inner speech in psychosis demonstrates that all factors of absorption, including imaginative involvement, were significantly associated with dialogic inner speech, evaluative and motivational inner speech. Imaginative involvement was also significantly associated with condensed inner speech. Inner speech appears to be connected to positive symptoms via the imaginative involvement/altered states of consciousness pathway, suggesting interconnectedness between inner speech, particularly dialogic inner speech when strongly associated with evaluative and motivational inner speech, and positive symptoms as part of the imaginative involvement and altered states of consciousness pathway. Kusters (35) eloquently describes the experience by which thoughts within the imaginative process are given life: “You think about something, and at the same time you think about everything that surrounds what you're thinking about. You think under it, over it, and behind it. With paranoia, thinking is the same as creating. A theoretical point of concentration expands into a practical creative field. By thinking somewhere behind it, you even change the character of the object of the thought itself. By imagining something, you bring it to life (35).”

Limitations

This study has several limitations. First, the sample size is modest and there is only limited generalizability of these results as the data are largely based on self-report. Second, as culture significantly influences the subjective experience of psychosis, our results may not generalize to other regions and populations. Third, all measures were collected concurrently, limiting any causal inferences of these associations and pathways. For example, many people with chronic and persistent psychosis may become socially isolated, which could influence both inner speech and introspection and absorption. Also, if one is experiencing profound alterations of perception, and starts to look for reasons, this again may fuel introspection, immersive attention and inner speech. Thus, prospective longitudinal research would be critical to unpacking the complex ways in which inner speech might be a building block of psychosis, but could also go the reverse way, and likewise for absorption. Additionally, a general limitation of network analysis is the difficulty to rule out that the findings are not simply related to measurement-related issues, such as the way subscales of one instruments will tend to covey more with one another than with subscales of other instruments due to issues like scoring range similarities or conceptual similarities. Lastly, the dimensions of absorption in the TAS can directly overlap with psychosis, thus classic psychotic symptoms, for example, can be structurally similar to the TAS dimensions/sub-scales which can be seen as both a limitation (of measurement, primarily; of psychometric validity especially across populations) and strength as potentially fertile ground for future research directions.

Conclusion

This study examined the relationship between absorption, inner speech, and psychopathology. In psychosis, dialogic inner speech and emotional/motivational inner speech are strongly associated with all dimensions/subscales of absorption while condensed inner speech is singularly associated with increased imaginative involvement. We also demonstrated that altered states of consciousness, dialogic inner speech, and emotional/motivational inner speech all predicted positive symptoms. In terms of network associations and pathways within the scaffolding of absorption and inner speech in psychosis, imaginative involvement is the most central, influential, and most highly predictive node in the model from which all other nodes related to psychopathology and inner speech are connected. This interconnectedness has been described as if you were trying to grasp water, as if you had burnt your tongue in a flame, as if time had evaporated and condensed into fantasy. Something strange occurred, but how it occurred, what it was, and whether it was real or not—that all remains vague. You walk through the mirror, stroll around behind it a bit and come back; “in front of” turns out to have been “behind” and vice versa…(35). Lastly, this study shows that within the network of absorption, inner speech and psychopathology, vivid sensory, and perceptual imaginative involvement that encompasses dialogic inner speech with evaluative and motivational characteristics as expressed in alterations of consciousness is an interconnected structure in psychotic experiences.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because individual raw data cannot be made publicly available as ethical consent was not provided by participants nor do we have IRB approval to do such. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to Cherise Rosen, ccrosen@uic.edu.

Ethics Statement

The study presented involved human participants and was reviewed and approved by the University of Illinois at Chicago Internal Review Board. All participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study prior to study initiation.

Author Contributions

CR and RS contributed to the conception and design of the study. CR and KC were involved in data collection, data analysis, and interpretation. MT conducted the network analysis and MT and CR contributed to the interpretation. CR, CH, KC, AA, and AF contributed to the drafting the article. CR, CH, KC, NJ, and RS contributed to the critical revision of the article. All authors have given final approval of the version to be published.

Funding

This work was supported in part by PHS grant (NIH) R01MH094358 (RS).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all the individuals who participated in this study and contributed to the findings reported here by generously sharing their experiences of absorption, inner speech and symptoms.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.649808/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Nietzche F. Oktay rifat's blue freedom: mavi özgürlük. Transl Review. (1882) 2004:67–8. doi: 10.1080/07374836.2004.10523877

CrossRef Full Text

2. Longden KD. Central brain circuitry for color-vision-modulated behaviors. Curr Biol. (2016) 26:R981–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.07.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Al-Rasheed AS. An experimental study of gender and cultural differences in hue preference. Front Psychol. (2015) 6:30. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Nakajima K, Minami T, Nakauchi S. Interaction between facial expression and color. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:41019. doi: 10.1038/srep41019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Rabin J, Houser B, Talbert C, Patel R. Blue-black or white-gold? Early stage processing and the color of'the dress'. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0161090. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Longere P, Kraft JM, Brainard DH. Bayesian model of human color constancy. J Vision. (2001) 1:62. doi: 10.1167/1.3.62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bauer M, Oostenveld R, Fries P. Tactile stimulation accelerates behavioral responses to visual stimuli through enhancement of occipital gamma-band activity. Vis Res. (2009) 49:931–942. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Landgrebe M, Nyuyki K, Frank E, Steffens T, Hauser S, Eichhammer P, et al. Effects of colour exposure on auditory and somatosensory perception–hints for cross-modal plasticity. Neuroendocrinol Lett. (2008) 29:518–21.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

9. Barwich A. A critique of olfactory objects. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:1337. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Tamura K, Hamakawa M, Okamoto T. Olfactory modulation of colour working memory: How does citrus-like smell influence the memory of orange colour? PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0203876. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0203876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Spence C. Multisensory flavor perception. Cell. (2015) 161:24–35. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Emery KJ, Webster MA. Individual differences and their implications for color perception. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2019) 30:28–33. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Berrios GE. The clinical roots of the schizophrenia concept: translations of seminal european contributions to schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. (1987) 151:575–6. doi: 10.1192/S000712500021800X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Tellegen A, Atkinson G. Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. J Abnorm Psychol. (1974) 83:268–77. doi: 10.1037/h0036681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Rosen C, Jones N, Chase KA, Melbourne JK, Grossman LS, Sharma RP. Immersion in altered experience: an investigation of the relationship between absorption and psychopathology. Conscious Cogn. (2017) 49:215–26. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Van Duppen Z, Sips R. Understanding the blind spots of psychosis: a wittgensteinian and first-person approach. Psychopathology. (2018) 51:276–84. doi: 10.1159/000490257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Chapman J. The early symptoms of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. (1966) 112:225–51. doi: 10.1192/bjp.112.484.225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Geekie J, Read J. Making Sense of Madness: Contesting the Meaning of Schizophrenia. New York, NY: Routledge (2009). doi: 10.4324/9780203878422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Romme M, Escher S. Psychosis as a Personal Crisis: An Experience-Based Approach. East Sussex; New York, NY: Routledge (2013). doi: 10.4324/9780203696682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Rosen C, Jones N, Chase KA, Gin H, Grossman LS, Sharma RP. The intrasubjectivity of self, voices and delusions: a phenomenological analysis. Psychosis. (2016) 8:357–68. doi: 10.1080/17522439.2016.1162839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Mishara AL. Body self and its narrative representation in schizophrenia. Interdiscipli Perspect Body. (2005) 62:127–52. doi: 10.1075/aicr.62.09mis

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Uhlhaas PJ, Mishara AL. Perceptual anomalies in schizophrenia: integrating phenomenology and cognitive neuroscience. Schizophr Bull. (2007) 33:142–56. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Morin A, Everett J. Inner speech as a mediator of self-awareness, self-consciousness, and self-knowledge: an hypothesis. New Ideas Psychol. (1990) 8:337–56. doi: 10.1016/0732-118X(94)90020-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Rosen C, McCarthy-Jones S, Chase KA, Humpston CS, Melbourne JK, Kling L, et al. The tangled roots of inner speech, voices and delusions. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 264:281–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Hermans HJ, Kempen HJ, Van Loon RJ. The dialogical self: Beyond individualism and rationalism. Am Psychol. (1992) 47:23. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.47.1.23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lysaker PH, Lysaker JT. Schizophrenia and alterations in self-experience: a comparison of 6 perspectives. Schizophr Bull. (2010) 36:331–40. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Alderson-Day B, Fernyhough C. Inner speech: development, cognitive functions, phenomenology, and neurobiology. Psychol Bull. (2015) 141:931–65. doi: 10.1037/bul0000021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Fernyhough C. The dialogic mind: a dialogic approach to the higher mental functions. New Ideas Psychol. (1996) 14:47–62. doi: 10.1016/0732-118X(95)00024-B

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Fernyhough C. Alien voices and inner dialogue: towards a developmental account of auditory verbal hallucinations. New Ideas Psychol. (2004) 22:49–68. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2004.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. McCarthy-Jones S, Fernyhough C. The varieties of inner speech: links between quality of inner speech and psychopathological variables in a sample of young adults. Conscious Cogn. (2011) 20:1586–93. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Blankenburg W, Mishara AL. First steps toward a psychopathology of “common sense”. Philos Psychiatry Psychol. (2001) 8:303–15. doi: 10.1353/ppp.2002.0014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Giersch A, Mishara AL. Is schizophrenia a disorder of consciousness? Experimental and phenomenological support for anomalous unconscious processing. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:1659. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Humpston CS. Perplexity and meaning: toward a phenomenological “core” of psychotic experiences. Schizophr Bull. (2014) 40:240–43. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Humpston CS, Broome MR. Thinking, believing, and hallucinating self in schizophrenia. Lancet Psychiatry. (2020) 7:638–646. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30007-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Kusters W. A Philosophy of Madness: The Experience of Psychotic Thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2020). doi: 10.7551/mitpress/11573.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Cuesta MJ, de JalÃ3n EG, Campos MS, Ibáñez B, Sañchez-Torres AM, Peralta V. Duration of untreated negative and positive symptoms of psychosis and cognitive impairment in first episode psychosis. Schizophr Res. (2012) 141:222–7. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hempel CG. Philosophy of Natural Science 1966. Englewood cliffs, NY: Prentice-Hall (1966).

Google Scholar

38. Rosen C, Marvin R, Reilly JL, DeLeon O, Harris MS, Keedy SK, et al. Phenomenology of first-episode psychosis in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and unipolar depression: a comparative analysis. Clin Schizophr Relat Psycho. (2012) 6:145–51A. doi: 10.3371/CSRP.6.3.6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. General Assembly of the World Medical Association. World medical association declaration of helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. J Am Coll Dent. (2014) 81:14

Google Scholar

40. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JB. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition. New York, NY (2002). p. 94–101.

41. Wechsler D. Wechsler Test of Adult Reading: WTAR. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (2001).

PubMed Abstract

42. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (1987) 13:261–76. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Lindenmayer J, Bernstein-Hyman R, Grochowski S, Bark N. Psychopathology of schizophrenia: initial validation of a 5-factor model. Psychopathology. (1995) 28:22–31. doi: 10.1159/000284896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Jamieson GA. The modified tellegen absorption scale: a clearer window on the structure and meaning of absorption. Austral J Clin Exp Hypnosis. (2005) 33:119–9.

Google Scholar

45. Tellegen A. Content Categories: ABSORPTION Items (revised). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota (1982).

46. Pappa E, Peters E, Bell V. Insight-related beliefs and controllability appraisals contribute little to hallucinated voice networks in psychosis. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. (2019) doi: 10.31234/osf.io/9k6ty

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS, Ullman JB. Using Multivariate Statistics, Vol. 5. Boston, MA: Pearson (2007) 481–98p.

Google Scholar

48. Hair JF, Anderson RE, Tatham RL, Black WC. Multivariate Data Analysis With Readings, 5th ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall (1998)

Google Scholar

49. Haslbeck J, Waldorp LJ. Mgm: Estimating time-varying mixed graphical models in high-dimensional data. arXiv Preprint arXiv:1510.06871 (2015).

Google Scholar

50. Haslbeck JM, Waldorp LJ. How well do network models predict observations? On the importance of predictability in network models. Behav Res Methods. (2018) 50:853–61. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0910-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Yang E, Baker Y, Ravikumar P, Allen G, Liu Z. Mixed graphical models via exponential families. Art Intellig Stat. (2014) 33:1042–50.

Google Scholar

52. Friedman J, Hastie T, Tibshirani R. Regularization paths for generalized linear models via coordinate descent. J Stat Softw. (2010) 33:1. doi: 10.18637/jss.v033.i01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Epskamp S, Cramer AO, Waldorp LJ, Schmittmann VD, Borsboom D. Qgraph: network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. J Stat Softw. (2012) 48:1–18. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Epskamp S, Borsboom D, Fried EI. Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: a tutorial paper. Behav Res Methods. (2018) 50:195–212. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Bell V, O'Driscoll C. The network structure of paranoia in the general population. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2018) 53:737–44. doi: 10.1007/s00127-018-1487-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Fried EI, van Borkulo CD, Cramer AO, Boschloo L, Schoevers RA, Borsboom D. Mental disorders as networks of problems: a review of recent insights. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2017) 52:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s00127-016-1319-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Fried EI, Epskamp S, Nesse RM, Tuerlinckx F, Borsboom D. What are'good'depression symptoms? Comparing the centrality of DSM and non-DSM symptoms of depression in a network analysis. J Affect Disord. (2016) 189:314–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Fruchterman TM, Reingold EM. Graph drawing by force directed placement. Softw Pract Exper. (1991) 21:1129–64. doi: 10.1002/spe.4380211102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Robinaugh DJ, Millner AJ, McNally RJ. Identifying highly influential nodes in the complicated grief network. J Abnorm Psychol. (2016) 125:747–57. doi: 10.1037/abn0000181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Weilnhammer V, Röd L, Eckert A, Stuke H, Heinz A, Sterzer P. Psychotic experiences in schizophrenia and sensitivity to sensory evidence. Schizophr Bull. (2020) 46:927–36. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Luhrmann TM. Diversity within the psychotic continuum. Schizophr Bull. (2017) 43:27–31. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Nelson B, Thompson A, Yung AR. Basic self-disturbance predicts psychosis onset in the ultra high risk for psychosis “prodromal” population. Schizophr Bull. (2012) 38:1277–87. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Humpston CS, Walsh E, Oakley DA, Mehta MA, Bell V, Deeley Q. The relationship between different types of dissociation and psychosis-like experiences in a non-clinical sample. Conscious Cogn. (2016) 41:83–92. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Roche SM, McConkey KM. Absorption: nature, assessments and correlates. J Person Psicol Soc. (1990) 59:91–101. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.1.91

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Pekala RJ, Wenger CF, Levine RL. Individual differences in phenomenological experience: states of consciousness as a function of absorption. J Person Soc Psychol. (1985) 48:125–32. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.48.1.125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Nordgaard J, Nilsson LS, Sæbye D, Parnas J. Self-disorders in schizophrenia-spectrum disorders: a 5-year follow-up study. Euro Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2018) 268:713–8. doi: 10.1007/s00406-017-0837-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Raballo A, Parnas J. Examination of anomalous self-experience: initial study of the structure of self-disorders in schizophrenia spectrum. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2012) 200:577–83. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31825bfb41

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Raballo A, Preti A. Temporal stability of self-disorders and longitudinal unfolding of symptom dimensions: a complementary analysis. Schizophr Res. (2018) 195:78–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Mishara AL, Schwartz MA. Psychopathology in the light of emergent trends in the philosophy of consciousness, neuropsychiatry and phenomenology. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (1997) 10:383–9. doi: 10.1097/00001504-199709000-00011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Phillips W, Silverstein SM. Convergence of biological and psychological perspectives on cognitive coordination in schizophrenia. Behav Brain Sci. (2003) 26:65–82. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X03000025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Bregman-Hai N, Abitbul-Gordon S, Deutsch I, Garbi D, Shelef L, Soffer-Dudek N. Leave everything to the imagination: cognitive functioning of individuals high in dissociative absorption and imaginative involvemen. J Res Person. (2018) 76:129–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2018.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Hilgard ER. Divided Consciousness: Multiple Controls in Human Thought and Action. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Inc. (1977).

Google Scholar

73. Soffer-Dudek N, Lassri D, Soffer-Dudek N, Shahar G. Dissociative absorption: an empirically uniquem, clinically relevant, dissociative factor. Conscious Cogn. (2015) 36:338–51. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: psychosis, inner speech and thought, absorption, phenomenology, network analysis

Citation: Rosen C, Tufano M, Humpston CS, Chase KA, Jones N, Abramowitz AC, Franco Chakkalakal A and Sharma RP (2021) The Sensory and Perceptual Scaffolding of Absorption, Inner Speech, and Self in Psychosis. Front. Psychiatry 12:649808. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.649808

Received: 05 January 2021; Accepted: 09 April 2021;
Published: 10 May 2021.

Edited by:

Sören Krach, University of Lübeck, Germany

Reviewed by:

Sarah Keedy, University of Chicago, United States
Macià Buades-Rotger, University of Lübeck, Germany

Copyright © 2021 Rosen, Tufano, Humpston, Chase, Jones, Abramowitz, Franco Chakkalakal and Sharma. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cherise Rosen, ccrosen@uic.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.