Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 15 June 2018
Sec. Health Economics

Self-Control in Intertemporal Choice and Mediterranean Dietary Pattern

  • 1Department of Economics and Business, Universidad de Almería, Almería, Spain
  • 2Department of Behavioral Science, Center for Experimental Research in Social Sciences, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan

Background: The Mediterranean Diet (hereinafter MD) is considered a healthy dietary pattern. Adherence to this pattern can be assessed by means of the KIDMED test by which individuals are assigned an index and classified into three groups of adherence to MD: high, medium, and low. In addition, impulsivity or impatience in intertemporal choice has been defined as a strong preference for small immediate rewards over large delayed ones.

Objective: This study examines the relationship between dietary habits, specifically Mediterranean dietary pattern, measured by the KIDMED index, and the exhibited impatience in intertemporal choices, by means of the parameter k (discount rate of the hyperbolic discount function).

Methods: A sample of 207 university students answered a questionnaire based on two tests: the KIDMED test, to assess the degree of adherence to MD, and an intertemporal choice questionnaire, to assess impatience or impulsivity. Individuals were grouped depending on their KIDMED score and then the discount rate or impulsivity parameter was calculated for each group.

Results: Discount rates were inversely related to the degree of adherence to MD. The values of overall k were 1.53, 1.91, and 3.71% for the groups exhibiting high, medium and low adherence to MD, respectively. We also found higher k-values for larger rewards (magnitude effect) in the three groups.

Conclusion: High adherence to MD is related to less steep time discounting, which implies less impulsivity (more self-control) or lower discount rates. Conversely, low adherence to MD is related to steeper time discounting, which implies impulsivity or higher discount rates. These findings could be used to identify the target population where policy interventions are needed in order to promote healthier diet habits.

Introduction

The traditional Mediterranean diet is characterized by a high consumption of plant foods (vegetables, fruits, legumes, and cereals), of olive oil as the principal source of mono-unsaturated fat (low intake of saturated fat), intake of fish, low-to-moderate intake of dairy products, low consumption of meat and poultry, and wine consumed in low-to-moderate amounts, normally with meals (1). This dietary pattern is considered a balanced and varied diet which provides most of the recommended macronutrients in their correct proportion (2).

Adherence to the Mediterranean dietary pattern has been associated with a longer life-span, reduced risk of cardiovascular and/or cancer mortality and of neurodegenerative disease (3, 4). It is therefore important, from the point of view of health economics, to examine the relationship between adherence to the Mediterranean diet, one of the most typical healthy eating habits, and economic decision-making over time. Specifically, the main objective of this paper is to examine the relationship between the Mediterranean dietary pattern and the exhibited impatience in intertemporal choices, by means of the parameter k of the hyperbolic discount function.

Adherence to MD in Spain, Greece, Cyprus, and Italy among children and adolescents was surveyed by Grosso and Galvano (5). A survey in Italy was carried out by Cavaliere et al. (6), where a significant part of the analyzed population followed dietary patterns alien to the MD pyramid guidelines. This may be due to the increasing cost of many key foods of the Mediterranean diet, which has led people to westernize their diet in favor of less expensive, often unhealthy products (7). A historical review of the adherence to MD worldwide can be found in da Silva et al. (8), and a similar review, in Spain, in Bach-Faig et al. (9).

There are several studies which show that unhealthy eating habits are related to impatience in intertemporal choice [for a systematic review, see (10)]. From this, we can deduce the opposite scenario. Since the Mediterranean diet is a model of healthy eating (2, 1114), it can be predicted that the Mediterranean diet habit is related to self-control in intertemporal choice. This is the hypothesis we are going to test in the empirical study described in detail in section Materials and Methods. One of the advantages of the present study is that the data was collected in the Spanish regions close to the Mediterranean Sea. Contrarily, if this type of investigation were conducted in other areas such as North America, it would be virtually impossible to collect data on genuine Mediterranean dietary habits.

It is often suggested that the methodologies developed to measure the impatience in intertemporal choice could be applied to other domains such as health states or dieting (15). In effect, the existing literature on this topic has been focused on the use of discounting tools when analyzing the decisions about smoking, exercising, dieting, saving, etc. at different points in time [see (1618)]. In this context, our paper aims to analyze the influence of the score obtained in the questionnaires administered to measure the aforementioned individual characteristics (more specifically, dieting) on the intertemporal choice of monetary rewards. Specifically, we take into account the impatience or impulsivity shown by individuals in their choices by means of the k-parameter (discount rate) of a discount function. The higher the value of k, the more impatient the individual is said to be. On the other hand, the opposite behavior to impulsivity is self-control.

Eating habits and dietary intake are a public health concern. In health economic studies, relationships between eating habits/disorders and self-control in intertemporal choices have been examined [see (19) for a review]. In Davis et al. (20) the roles of impulsivity in the Iowa gambling task and temporal discounting are reported. Among heavy drinkers, impulsivity in temporal discounting (i.e., preference for sooner smaller rewards over larger later ones) contributes to dysregulated eating (21). In both obese and non-obese women, impulsivity in intertemporal choices has been associated with problematic eating behavior (22, 23). Also in obese children, self-control in intertemporal choice predicted weight loss (24). In the context of behavioral interventions for problematic eating behavior, mindful eating training reduced preference for smaller and more immediate food rewards over larger later ones (25). Contrarily, among female students, self-control in intertemporal choice may be a risk factor for anorexia nervosa (26). In bulimia nervosa, in contrast, impulsivity in intertemporal choice was reportedly exacerbated (27). Drug misuse is also a public health issue. Thus, Mole et al. (28) reported that impulsivity in intertemporal choice is a common-core impairment across problematic eating and substance misuse. Among working adults, fast-food consumption (unhealthy eating habits) has been associated with greater temporal discounting (29). From certain perspectives in behavioral economics, it has been argued that higher future time preferences (i.e., self-control in intertemporal choice) are related to a lower frequency of fast-food consumption (30).

Nevertheless, to the extent of our knowledge, no previous paper has examined the relations between MD (a model of healthy dietary habits) and time discounting, and therefore our study is the first to examine this relationship.

Materials and Methods

To test our hypothesis, we have used a questionnaire based on two different tests. First, the KIDMED test (11) has been used to evaluate the adherence to the Mediterranean Diet (MD), and second the questionnaire devised by Kirby et al. (31) has been implemented to obtain appropriate information about intertemporal choice. We also requested some demographic data (such as sex, age, education, and employment status).

KIDMED Test and Intertemporal Choice Questionnaire

In order to assess the degree of adherence to MD, we have used the KIDMED test developed by Serra-Majem et al. (11). This test is a 16-item questionnaire regarding food habits (see Appendix 1, Part 1) in which the answers can be either affirmative or negative. The answers with a positive connotation in relation to the principles of the MD are assigned a value of +1, and those with a negative connotation, a value of −1. We thereby obtain the KIDMED index with a value within the range 0 to 12. On this basis, each participant is classified in one of the following groups according to his/her score in the test (2):

1. KIDMED index ≥ 8 means high adherence to MD (optimal Mediterranean diet).

2. KIDMED index from 4 to 7 implies medium adherence to MD (improvement needed to adjust intake to Mediterranean patterns).

3. KIDMED index ≤ 3 means poor adherence to MD (very low diet quality).

To assess impulsivity, we have taken into account the level of impatience exhibited in intertemporal choices, by means of the k-parameter (discount rate) of a hyperbolic discount function (32):

SIR=LDR1+kd,k>0,    (1)

where SIR is the smaller, immediate reward, LDR is the larger, delayed reward, and d is the delay associated with LDR.

For this purpose, we have used the 27-item monetary choice questionnaire developed by Kirby et al. (31) (see Appendix 1, Part 2 in the Supplementary Materials). This questionnaire contains a set of 27 specific choices between an SIR and a LDR. There are three levels of reward size: small (from $25 to $35), medium (from $50 to $60), and large (from $75 to $85). It is thereby possible to calculate for each participant a separate k value for small, medium, and large delayed rewards. Therefore, the magnitude effect, which implies higher discount rates for smaller than for larger amounts (3335), can be assessed.

To derive the discount rates from the responses of the individual questionnaires, we have used the automated scores for an Excel spreadsheet as obtained from Kaplan et al. (36) which applies and facilitates the scoring procedure to estimate the discount rate, and which is fully described in Kirby et al. (31) and Kaplan et al. (37).

Sample

A total of 207 students at the Business School of the University of Almería (Spain) participated by answering the questionnaires. The participation was completely voluntary. Eleven questionnaires were discarded: seven were incomplete and four were rejected for reasons of lack of consistency which will be explained in the following subsection. Our final sample therefore consisted of 196 subjects. Regarding the composition of our sample, 55% of the participants were men and 45% women, and the mean age was 22 years. Only 11% of the participants smoked, and only 15% combined their studies with paid employment.

Procedure

We contacted the students at the Business School in their classes, using the last minutes of them, and prior agreement with their professors.

Before completing the questionnaire, students were informed that these would be completely anonymous and that we asked for their voluntary participation. Therefore, students who did not want to collaborate could leave the classroom or stay there and not to complete the questionnaires. Moreover, we asked them to respond with absolute sincerity and we gratefully acknowledged their participation in this research.

After collecting the data, we started by evaluating the KIDMED test and calculating the KIDMED index derived from the responses. According to their scores in the test, we classified the subjects in three groups: high, medium, and low adherence to MD, as explained in section KIDMED Test and Intertemporal Choice Questionnaire.

We subsequently introduced the intertemporal choices of each individual in the first group (high adherence to the MD) in the automated scores for an Excel spreadsheet using (36), obtaining the discount rates (k-values) for each individual and for the whole group. We proceeded in the same way with the other two groups (medium and low adherence to MD). This spreadsheet estimated for each individual the overall k value based on the answers to the 27 intertemporal choice questions. Moreover, according to the response patterns, a consistency score was determined for each participant. The recommendation of Kaplan et al. (36) is to examine more closely the individual-level patterns in cases where consistency scores are less than 75%, because it may indicate a lack of attention to the questionnaire. As we had four cases where individual consistency scores were less than 75%, we opted for their exclusion from the sample.

Results

We had a final sample of 196 subjects divided into three groups depending on their adherence to MD: 29% of the subjects were included in the high-adherence group, 58% in the medium adherence group, and 13% in the low adherence group, according to their KIDMED index. These figures make sense if we take into account the place where the data were collected: Almería, a province in the south-east of Spain, on the Mediterranean coast. We have few people with low adherence to MD and a majority with medium and high adherence to MD. The composition of our sample, in terms of adherence to MD, is in line with other studies conducted among university students. Specifically Durà and Castroviejo (38) analyzed a sample of 570 university students with regard to the adherence to MD, using the KIDMED test. They had the following results: 28.4% of the students had high adherence to MD, 62.1% medium adherence to MD, and 9.5% low adherence to MD. As can be observed, the composition regarding adherence to MD in both samples is very similar.

Table 1 shows the mean, standard deviation (SD) and standard error of the mean (SEM) of the k-values for non-transformed, log-transformed and ln-transformed data corresponding to the three groups under study (high, medium, and low adherence to MD). These measures have been also discriminated by overall k-values, and k-values for the small, medium, and large magnitude sizes.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary statistics for k-values in the three groups under study (high, medium and low adherence to MD).

The obtained results reveal the existence of the magnitude effect in high, medium and low adherence to MD groups. As we can observe in Figure 1, higher discount rates (k) are applied to smaller delayed rewards, and vice versa (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. k-values for small, medium, and large delayed rewards in the three groups under study.

Table 1 also includes a measure of consistency which is a score which takes into account the instances of 0s (i.e., the choice of the SIR) prior to the given k-value, and instances of 1s (i.e., LDR) at and following the given k-value. This amount is then divided by 27 (in the case of the overall k) or 9 (in the case of a particular k-value).

Table 2 displays the Pearson correlations between the three possible pairs of transformed and non-transformed k-values. Likewise, it shows the intervals at a 95% confidence level.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Pearson correlations for the three groups under study between small, medium and large k.

In Table 3, we have summarized the results for the three separate groups, taking into account only the overall k.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Overall discount rates by groups and consistency of the results.

These results confirm our initial hypothesis: high adherence to MD is related to less steep time-discounting, or in other words, high adherence to MD implies lower discount rates, in this case 1.53% (the smallest discount rate of the three groups). Moreover, the group with low adherence to MD presents the highest discount rate: 3.71%.

As the rate of discount is commonly taken to indicate the level of impulsivity or impatience in intertemporal choices (39), we found that people with low adherence to MD exhibit more impatience than people with high adherence to MD. Moreover, a general deterioration of the MD among young people has been observed (5). In line with these observations, 58% of the subjects surveyed in our sample presented medium adherence to the MD, which means that efforts are needed to adjust their food consumption to Mediterranean patterns.

Discussion

Our results are in line with those of other researchers [see (10)] who found higher discount rates in persons consuming unhealthy diets, and those who are overweight and obese.

Therefore, impatience could be an early indication of a problem of self-control which could result in overweigh, obesity or even other diseases (e.g., addiction), given that excessive discounting may be considered as a trans-disease process (40, 41). This finding could be used to identify the target population where policy interventions are needed in order to promote healthier diet habits.

Additionally, MD has been associated with a number of beneficial effects on cognitive functions. Among French people, Féart et al. (42) reported that MD has been associated with slower cognitive decline in elderly people (aged 65+). Recently, Luciano et al. (43) observed that MD adherence was associated with slower brain atrophy among a group of people in Scotland aged 73–76. This indicates that several components of MD may even protect neuronal structures in elderly people, which could help this sector of the population make better decisions. A recent meta-analysis study reported that MD adherence is related to several types of good memory and executive functions (44). For instance, episodic memory and global cognition were positively associated with MD adherence.

According to studies in cognitive psychology, self-control in time discounting is associated with working memory (45), attention (46), executive functions (47), episodic future thinking (48), and time perception (49). Future studies should examine which cognitive components are modulated by MD adherence.

As limitations of our study, we could include the size of the sample used to test our hypothesis or the fact that it includes only university students. Future research could replicate this study using a bigger and randomly selected sample. Nevertheless, our sample is in line with similar studies: in the 34 studies reviewed by McClelland et al. (19), the mean sample was 137, and only six papers out of 34 used a sample with more than 200 subjects.

Despite these possible limitations, the main strength of our study is the originality of the hypothesis submitted to test. To date, the relationship between healthy eating habits and intertemporal choice has not been extensively investigated, and specifically and to the extent of our knowledge, no previous paper has examined the relations between MD and time discounting.

More research is needed to support the relation between MD and time discounting. We aim to further investigate the relations between MD, BMI (body mass index) and time discounting.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for this study in accordance with the national and institutional requirements. The University of Almería approved the collection of data among students who voluntarily agreed to answer the anonymous questionnaire.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

SCR and MJMT acknowledge the financial support from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, and the European Regional Development Fund ERDF/FEDER-UE (National R&D Project ECO2015-66504 and National R&D Project DER2016-76053-R). We are also very grateful for the comments and suggestions offered by three reviewers.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2018.00176/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Willett WC, Sacks F, Trichopoulou A, Drescher G, Ferro-Luzzi A, Helsing E, et al. Mediterranean diet pyramid: a cultural model for healthy eating. Am J Clin Nutr. (1995) 61(6 Suppl.): 1402S–6S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/61.6.1402S

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. García Cabrera S, Herrera Fernández N, Rodríguez Hernández C, Nissensohn M, Román-Viñas B, Serra-Majem L. KIDMED test; prevalence of low adherence to the Mediterranean Diet in children and young: a systematic review. Nutr Hosp. (2015) 32:2390–9. doi: 10.3305/nh.2015.32.6.9828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Trichopoulou A, Costacou T, Bamia C, Trichopoulos D. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet and survival in a Greek population. N Engl J Med. (2003) 348:2599–608. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa025039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Sofi F, Cesari F, Abbate R, Gensini GF, Casini A. Adherence to Mediterranean diet and health status: meta-analysis. BMJ (2008) 337:a1344. doi: 10.1136/bmj.a1344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Grosso G, Galvano F. Mediterranean diet adherence in children and adolescents in southern European countries. NFS J. (2016) 3:13–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nfs.2016.02.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Cavaliere A, De Marchi E, Banterle A. Exploring the adherence to the Mediterranean diet and its relationship with individual lifestyle: the role of healthy behaviors, pro-environmental behaviors, income, and education. Nutrients (2018) 10:E141. doi: 10.3390/nu10020141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bonaccio M, Donati MB, Iacoviello L, de Gaetano G. Socioeconomic determinants of the adherence to the Mediterranean diet at a time of economic crisis: the experience of the MOLI-SANI study. Agric Agric Sci Procedia (2016) 8:741–7. doi: 10.1016/j.aaspro.2016.02.059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. da Silva R, Bach-Faig A, Raidó Quintana B, Buckland G, Vaz de Almeida MD, Serra-Majem L. Worldwide variation of adherence to the Mediterranean diet, in 1961–1965 and 2000–2003. Public Health Nutr. (2009) 12:1676–84. doi: 10.1017/S1368980009990541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bach-Faig A, Fuentes-Bol C, Ramos D, Carrasco JL, Roman B, Bertomeu IF, et al. The Mediterranean diet in Spain: adherence trends during the past two decades using the Mediterranean Adequacy Index. Public Health Nutr. (2011) 14:622–8. doi: 10.1017/S1368980010002752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Barlow P, Reeves A, McKee M, Galea G, Suckler D. Unhealthy diets, obesity and time discounting: a systematic literature review and network analysis. Obesity Rev. (2016) 17:810–9. doi: 10.1111/obr.12431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Serra-Majem L, Ribas L, García A, Pérez-Rodrigo C, Aranceta J. Nutrient adequacy and Mediterranean Diet in Spanish school children and adolescents. Eur J Clin Nutr. (2003) 57:S35–9. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Ayechu A, Durá T. Quality of dietary habits (adherence to a mediterranean diet) in pupils of compulsory secondary education. An Sist Sanit Navar. (2010) 33:35–42.

PubMed Abstract

13. Dernini S, Berry EM. Mediterranean diet: from a healthy diet to a sustainable dietary pattern. Front Nutr. (2015) 2:15. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2015.00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Donini LM, Dernini S, Lairon D, Serra-Majem L, Amiot MJ, del Balzo V, et al. A Consensus proposal for nutritional indicators to assess the sustainability of a healthy diet: the Mediterranean diet as a case study. Front Nutr. (2016) 3:37. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2016.00037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Rohde KIM The hyperbolic factor: a measure of time inconsistency. J Risk Uncertain (2010) 41:125–40. doi: 10.1007/s11166-010-9100-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Bickel WK, Odum AL, Madden GJ. Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology (1999) 146:447–54. doi: 10.1007/PL00005490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Ohmura Y, Takahashi T, Kitamura N. Discounting delayed and probabilistic monetary gains and losses by smokers of cigarettes. Psychopharmacology (2005) 182:508–815. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0110-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Chabris CF, Laibson DI, Schuldt JP. Intertemporal choice. In: Durlauf SN, Blume LE, editors. Behavioural and Experimental Economics. The New Palgrave Economics Collection. London: Palgrave Macmillan (2010). p. 168–77.

Google Scholar

19. McClelland J, Dalton B, Kekic M, Bartholdy S, Campbell IC, Schmidt U. A Systematic review of temporal discounting in eating disorders and obesity: behavioural and neuroimaging findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 71:506–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Davis C, Patte K, Curtis C, Reid C. Immediate pleasures and future consequences. A neuropsychological study of binge eating and obesity. Appetite (2010) 54:208–13. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Stojek MM, Fischer S, Murphy CM, MacKillop J. The role of impulsivity traits and delayed reward discounting in dysregulated eating and drinking among heavy drinkers. Appetite (2014) 80:81–8. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Appelhans BM, Woolf K, Pagoto SL, Schneider KL, Whited MC, Liebman R. Inhibiting food reward: delay discounting, food reward sensitivity, and palatable food intake in overweight and obese women. Obesity (2011) 19:2175–82. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Rollins BY, Dearing KK, Epstein LH. Delay discounting moderates the effect of food reinforcement on energy intake among non-obese women. Appetite (2010) 55:420–5. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Best JR, Theim KR, Gredysa DM, Stein RI, Welch RR, Saelens BE, et al. Behavioral economic predictors of overweight children's weight loss. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2012) 80:1086–96. doi: 10.1037/a0029827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Hendrickson KL, Rasmussen EB. Effects of mindful eating training on delay and probability discounting for food and money in obese and healthy-weight individuals. Behav Res Ther. (2013) 51:399–409. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lilienthal KR, Weatherly JN. Understanding the relationships between body esteem, risk for anorexia nervosa, and domain-dependent decision-making impulsivity in a college sample. Body Image (2013) 10:558–65. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Kekic M, Bartholdy S, Cheng J, McClelland J, Boysen E, Musiat P, et al. Increased temporal discounting in bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. (2016) 49:1077–81. doi: 10.1002/eat.22571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Mole TB, Irvine MA, Worbe Y, Collins P, Mitchell SP, Bolton S, et al. Impulsivity in disorders of food and drug misuse. Psychol Med. (2015) 45:771–82. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714001834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Garza KB, Ding M, Owensby JK, Zizza CA. Impulsivity and fast-food consumption: a cross-sectional study among working adults. J Acad Nutr Diet. (2016) 116:61–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2015.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Shuval K, Stoklosa M, Pachucki MC, Yaroch AL, Drope J, Harding M. Economic preferences and fast food consumption in US adults: insights from behavioral economics. Prev Med. (2016) 93:204–10. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen. (1999) 128:78–87. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Mazur JE. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In: Commons ML; Mazur JE, Nevin J, Rachlin H, editors. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior, Vol. 5. The Effect of Delay and of Intervening Events on Reinforcement Value. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates (1987). p. 55–73.

Google Scholar

33. Thaler R. Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Econ Lett. (1981) 8:201–7. doi: 10.1016/0165-1765(81)90067-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Prelec D, Loewenstein G. Decision making over time and under uncertainty: a common approach. Manag Sci. (1991) 37:770–86. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.37.7.770

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Cruz Rambaud S, Muñoz Torrecillas MJ. An analysis of the anomalies in traditional discounting models. Intern Jour Psych Psychol Ther. (2004) 4:105–28.

Google Scholar

36. Kaplan BA, Lemley SM, Reed DD, Jarmolowicz DP. 21- and 27-Item Monetary Choice Questionnaire Automated Scorers [Software]. Center for Applied Neuroeconomics, University of Kansas (2014).

Google Scholar

37. Kaplan BA, Amlung M, Reed DD, Jarmolowicz DP, McKerchar TL, Lemley SM. Automating scoring of delay discounting for the 21- and 27-item monetary choice questionnaires. Behav Anal. (2016) 39:293–304. doi: 10.1007/s40614-016-0070-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Durá Travé T, Castroviejo Gandarias A. Adherencia a la dieta mediterránea en la población universitaria. Nutr Hosp. (2011) 26: 602–8. doi: 10.3305/nh.2011.26.3.4891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Cruz Rambaud S, Muñoz Torrecillas MJ. Measuring impatience in intertemporal choice. PLoS ONE (2016) 11:e0149256. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Bickel WK, Jarmolowicz DP, Mueller ET, Koffarnus MN, Gatchalian KM. Excessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a transdisease process contributing to addiction and other disease-related vulnerabilities: Emerging evidence. Pharmacol Ther. (2012) 134:287–97. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2012.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Cruz Rambaud S, Muñoz Torrecillas MJ, Takahashi T. Observed and normative discount functions in addiction and other diseases. Front Pharmacol. (2017) 8:416. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2017.00416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Féart C, Samieri C, Rondeau V, Amieva H, Portet F, Dartigues JF, et al. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia. JAMA (2009) 302:638–48. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Luciano M, Corley J, Cox SR, Valdés Hernández MC, Craig LC, Dickie DA, et al. Mediterranean-type diet and brain structural change from 73 to 76 years in a Scottish cohort. Neurology (2017) 88:449–55. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Loughrey DG, Lavecchia S, Brennan S, Lawlor BA, Kelly ME. The Impact of the Mediterranean diet on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Adv Nutr. (2017) 8:571–86. doi: 10.3945/an.117.015495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Bailey AJ, Gerst K, Finn PR. Delay discounting of losses and rewards in alcohol use disorder: the effect of working memory load. Psychol Addict Behav. (2018) 32:197–204. doi: 10.1037/adb0000341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Gunn RL, Gerst KR, Lake AJ, Finn PR. The effects of working memory load and attention refocusing on delay discounting rates in alcohol use disorder with comorbid antisocial personality disorder. Alcohol (2018) 66:9–14. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2017.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Esopo K, Mellow D, Thomas C, Uckat H, Abraham J, Jain P, et al. Measuring self-efficacy, executive function, and temporal discounting in Kenya. Behav Res Ther. (2018) 101:30–45. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Bromberg U, Lobatcheva M, Peters J. Episodic future thinking reduces temporal discounting in healthy adolescents. PLoS ONE (2017) 12:e0188079. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Takahashi T. Loss of self-control in intertemporal choice may be attributable to logarithmic time-perception. Med Hypotheses (2005) 65:691–3. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2005.04.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mediterranean diet, intertemporal choice, time discounting, impulsivity, self-control

Citation: Muñoz Torrecillas MJ, Cruz Rambaud S and Takahashi T (2018) Self-Control in Intertemporal Choice and Mediterranean Dietary Pattern. Front. Public Health 6:176. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2018.00176

Received: 31 January 2018; Accepted: 29 May 2018;
Published: 15 June 2018.

Edited by:

Sandra C. Buttigieg, University of Malta, Malta

Reviewed by:

Aleksandra Barac, University of Belgrade, Serbia
Aida Turrini, Consiglio per la Ricerca in Agricoltura e l'analisi dell'Economia Agraria (CREA), Italy

Copyright © 2018 Muñoz Torrecillas, Cruz Rambaud and Takahashi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: María J. Muñoz Torrecillas, mjmtorre@ual.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.