Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 23 April 2021
Sec. Public Mental Health

mHealth: Potentials and Risks for Addressing Mental Health and Well-Being Issues Among Nepali Adolescents

\nSiobhan K. Yilmaz
Siobhan K. Yilmaz*Alok K. BoharaAlok K. Bohara
  • Department of Economics, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, United States

Adolescents are slowly being recognized as a generation, worldwide, that may require different policy approaches to improve staggering statistics on their failing well-being, including mental health. By providing the support to allow the next generation to achieve better mental health outcomes, they are going to be more economically successful and the future economic growth of nations can be better assured. Adoption of mobile-based health interventions (e.g., mHealth) has garnered a lot of attention toward this end. While mHealth interventions are growing in popularity, many researchers/policy-makers appear to have neglected assessing potential (indirect) costs/negative consequences from their use. Evidence from the developed world shows strong associations between extensive cell phone use and negative mental health outcomes, but similar research is minimal in developing world contexts. Additionally, the bulk of work on the outcomes of mobile phone use is studied using a unidirectional approach with blinders to front-end motivations. Using primary data from a large-scale, school-based survey of older adolescents in southwestern Nepal (N = 539), this work investigates such a tension between mobile/smartphone usage as a true mobile health (mHealth) opportunity in Nepal or as a potential problem, introducing additional deleterious well-being effects from over-use. Founded in Basic Psychological Needs Theory (BPNT), robust results of analyses using full structural modeling approaches (and traditional regression-based sensitivity analyses) indicate support for the BPNT framework in explaining statistically significant positive associations between bullying and anxiety, as well as, negative associations between bullying and grit, including evidence to support the mediating role of problematic mobile phone use in these relationships. More than 56% of the sample showed indicators of mild to moderate anxiety and over 10% claim experiences of bullying, coupled with over 75% of the sample scoring above the midline of a problematic mobile phone use scale, all of which motivates the relevance of our findings. Potential policy implications of these findings, and mention of other intriguing avenues for future work are further discussed.

Introduction and Background

Of the 1.2 billion people under age 18 across the globe, 90% of them live in the developing world (1). The economic future of the world will depend on this next generation and improvements to their current well-being trajectories (2, 3). Estimates indicate 20–50% of young people in the Southeast Asia (SEA) region report being bullied, 7.1% are using alcohol, 9.7% are smoking, 8.4% report being lonely most or all of the time, 6.9% report being so anxious they lose sleep, and 6.8% have admitted to seriously considering suicide within the prior year (4). Such behavioral and mental health/well-being concerns are exacerbated by governments and healthcare systems which either do not acknowledge the importance of mental health, fail to address adolescents differently than younger children or adults, or both.

As chronic conditions, such as mental health, continue to gain attention, mobile-based health initiatives (or mHealth) are one potential solution to address the lack of attention, cost, and availability of healthcare professionals, particularly within developing world contexts (5, 6). Although limited in scale, most existing evidence of the acceptability and efficacy of mHealth approaches to change health-related behaviors has been well-received (7, 8), although thorough assessment of true economic benefits is still lacking (9). Due to their existing high use of mobile-based technologies, the use of mHealth approaches in adolescent populations may be additionally promising (1012). However, while use of mobile technology is prolific among adolescent populations, there is also evidence of lack of sufficient research fully accounting for the effectiveness and potential consequences of pursuing mHealth initiatives (13, 14).

Overuse/maladaptive uses of mobile/smartphones in the developed world has been strongly associated with numerous negative health/well-being outcomes (15, 16). However, there is limited work in the developing world regarding such impacts, with most focused on India (17, 18). Initial findings, however, do appear consistent with those from the developed world, which implies a very bleak picture for adolescent well-being if mobile phones are seen as the “solution” to an already suffering demographic.

When examined with empirical rigor, though, this outlook may not be so bleak. There is other work indicating that it is only those adolescents most prone to suffer from behavioral, mental, and emotional struggles who are going to be drawn to overuse mobile phones. However, determination of directionality of such relationships is fraught with a lack of clarity. Lack of focus on motivating factors may explain part of this. Incorporating theoretical frameworks from fields such as Positive Psychology and Sociology may allow for a clearer understanding, while also analyzing data with the empirical rigor associated with a field such as Economics. In that vein, this work uses primary data from secondary school students in urban and rural Nepal to assess the connections between life burdens, mobile phone overuse/maladaptive use, and overall well-being of adolescents through estimating a full structural model based on the assumptions of Basic Psychological Needs Theory (BPNT).

Research Context: Nepal

Nepal is one of the poorest and neediest regions in terms of addressing adolescent well-being. Of the 11.8% of the population estimated to be between 13 and 17, over 50% of adolescents report being bullied, making this the highest rate in the SEA region (4). Additional burdens adolescents, and in particular females, face in this country include high levels of gender-based violence/inequity (e.g., domestic violence, menstrual taboos, child marriage), which have damaging mental and physical health consequences (1921). A healthcare treatment system that faces infrastructure and treatment malfunction, with limited access to mental health care, only exacerbates such situations. Nepal only has 2.1 physicians per 10,000 people (the United States has 24.5) (22), and stigma surrounding mental health problems is high.

Indicative of the cumulative effect of such burdens is that Nepal has the second highest adolescent suicide rate regionally. A recent WHO Report (23) found 14% of surveyed adolescents thought about/planned suicide and 10% attempted suicide within the prior 12 months. Such risks also appear to continue on into adulthood, with Nepal having one of the top five (ten) suicide rates globally for women (men) (22). The need is real for interventions aimed at reducing such well-being-damaging burdens and providing treatment/support for them, particularly among adolescent populations.

Unfortunately, most of the existing research/interventions addressing these areas in Nepal has primarily focused on the plight of former child soldiers and conflict/displaced youth (24, 25). Furthermore, the use of mHealth interventions, while growing, is still in its infancy in Nepal, primarily focusing on more standard telehealth and tracking functionalities (26, 27). At the same time, cell phone/smartphone adoption is growing rapidly in Nepal, with 70–90% of households owning mobile phones (20). The World Bank (23) also reports that mobile-cellular telephone subscriptions per 100 people in Nepal jumped from 34.4 to 96.7% between 2010 and 2015.

Prior Literature on Mobile Phones and Negative Well-Being

With the growth in development, innovation, and adoption of mobile/smartphone technology, there is also a growth in research surrounding the consequences of its overuse. Along with findings supporting that high mobile phone use is linked with lost sleep (2830), there is consistent evidence of higher associations to anxiety, stress, and depression (3133).

The empirical limitation in these analyses is that direction of causality has yet to be well-established. While there is work that has focused on theoretical explanations of the motivation for mobile phone use (34), such work is not supported with empirical validation. The general assumption in empirical work is that cell phone use is the predecessor of negative mental health/well-being, supported by laboratory experiments (35, 36). Several long-term studies have also confirmed this (37, 38), along with strong indications of bi-directionality (39, 40), which may imply: the rich are getting richer (or in this case, the depressed are getting more depressed, due to their over-use of mobile phones).

To bring the literature forward with respect to further clarity about the relationships indicated, while also accounting for motivational elements that partially explain the findings already seen, this work examines the tension between mobile phone/smart phone usage as a true mHealth opportunity in Nepal and its potential for introducing deleterious effects on well-being from over-use of mobile phones. The impacts of technology on health deserve to be analyzed with the empirical rigor associated with Economics, given that the impacts of health on micro and macro-level economic problems is becoming ever more apparent. Therefore, by using robust empirical techniques we are able to provide additional validity to the conclusions and policy recommendations we make.

The Model: Framework, Specification, and Hypotheses

Conceptual Framework

Determining what motivates behavior is among the most central of all social science questions. Within sociology, consideration of social contexts has often served as the jumping-off point to answer this question. Within the study of Self-Determination Theory (SDT), there is the sub-theory of BPNT, which claims that satisfaction and/or frustration of the three needs of autonomy, competence, and relatedness serve as strong motivating factors in many behavioral relationships (41), many of which have well-being outcomes (42, 43).

As shown in Figure 1, low need satisfaction over time can result in costs, but this deterioration process will be exacerbated when needs are actively “frustrated” (or thwarted). When needs are frustrated, there are two consequences: immediate costs of reduced well-being and chronic need-thwarting leading to the development of coping strategies such as “Need Substitutes” and compensatory behaviors. However, most evidence indicates that the latter may ultimately be short-lived in producing feelings of need satisfaction, and can lead to (or include) further negative outcomes such as anxiety and substance abuse (44, 45).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Framework of basic psychological needs theory (BPNT).

We believe the relationship between problematic mobile phone use (PMPU) and well-being outcomes may follow this theoretical pathway, as seen in Figure 2. The social/cultural context adolescents face includes both protective and adverse environments/pressures which either meet or frustrate the three needs. When needs are frustrated, BPNT indicates that adolescents may seek out remedies to alleviate their sense of loss, which we postulate is facilitated through mobile phones. Use of technology that (attempts) to fulfill psychological voids, however, may be more prone to addiction (46, 47) and associated poorer well-being. Thus, if mobile phones are a medium through which the types of coping strategies BPNT mentions are facilitated, then they have the potential to exacerbate existing states of negative well-being, in congruence with existing literature.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Research application of BPNT framework.

Empirical Specification

In accordance with the framework and pathways above, we identify the empirical specification below:

Anxi=α0+α1PMPUi+α2Bullyi+α3APi*+α4FEi*+α5SSi*+α6X1i+α7X2i+α8X3i+u1i    (1)
Griti=β0+β1PMPUi+β2Bullyi+β3APi*+β4FEi*+β5SSi*+β6X1i+β7X2i+ β8X3i+u2i    (2)
PMPUi=λ0+λ1Bullyi+λ2APi*+λ3FEi*+λ4SSi*+λ5PCi+λ6PC2i+λ7FPMPUi+ λ8X1i+λ9X2i + X3i+u3i    (3)

Anxi and Griti represent the well-being outcomes of anxiety and grit1 for individual i. PMPUi is a measure of mobile phone overuse/maladaptive use, and Bullyi represents our key need-thwarting factor of bullying experiences. The remaining socio-cultural environmental pressures we consider/control for are academic pressure (APi*), family environment (FEi*), and social support (SSi*), all latent constructs. In Equation (3), PCi is the monetary cost of individual i's mobile phone, with PC2i its square. Friend's PMPU is represented by FPMPUi. Demographic controls for being female, age, and rural area residence are accounted for by X1i-X3i, respectively. White noise error terms for each equation are denoted by ui, with the appropriate equation referenced in subscript.

Hypotheses

Bullying can be staggering and have dramatic impacts on mental/physical health (53, 54), whereas individuals with greater support across environments (including school) show a number of positive outcomes including subjective well-being and lower stress levels (55, 56). In the absence of real-life support, many adolescents may turn to other means, such as media entertainment, to try and meet their needs. Given associations between psychological need frustration and dependency in adolescents (44), adolescents using media (often accessed via mobile phones) for such means would be expected to then have more problematic use of digital technology. This supports the “needs-as-motive” hypothesis among young adults, wherein the trio of needs not only sustains well-being, but also motivates remedial behavior when missing (57). Given strong evidence that coping strategies are ultimately ineffective/maladaptive (45, 58), this would imply further reductions in overall well-being.

Given such evidence, we formally hypothesize:

Hypothesis #1: Those adolescents bullied more will have worse well-being outcomes – higher anxiety (Anx) and lower grit (Grit).

Hypothesis #2: Those adolescents who are bullied more (Bully) will have more problematic mobile phone use (PMPU).

Hypothesis #3: There will be a significant indirect effect of bullying (Bully) on well-being outcomes (Anx and Grit), acting through problematic mobile phone use (PMPU).

Additionally, it is also important to establish any potential relationship to be had between our well-being outcomes. The small literature addressing anxiety and grit consistently indicate a negative relationship (59, 60), including in developing world contexts (61, 62). Thus, we finally hypothesize:

Hypothesis #4: There will be a negative correlation between anxiety (Anx) and grit (Grit).

Data and Variables

Data

Data comes from a self-report survey administered to 11 and 12th grade-equivalent adolescents in the urban city of Siddharthanagar and surrounding areas of Pulpa, Gulma, and Argankhachi, in southwestern Nepal. Following an exploratory focus group assessment conducted in the same area prior to this study, which addressed mental health knowledge/awareness, current mobile phone practices/uses, and cultural/developmental burdens seen as most detrimental, the survey used in this study was translated by a professional translator, certified by the local Notary Public Council. The translated survey was further read through by several of the Nepali enumerators and support staff running the survey collection on the ground, prior to its implementation, to ensure that it was conceptually sound in its translation. The survey/data collection was performed on-sight at schools during school time. We a priori aimed to collect data from around 600 observations and were only limited by the fixed amount of sampling provided by headmasters at the schools. These goals were met and once restricting our sample to only those adolescents who owned a phone, our final sample size was 539 adolescents. This study was approved by the institutional review board (IRB) of our institution and the executive management group of the Lumbini Center for Sustainability at its Pratiman Neema College venue in Siddharthanagar, Rupandehi district, who also oversaw the collection and supervision of the data collection.

Key Variables

Well-being outcomes were measured using previously validated survey instruments. Anx is based on a modified version of the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (63), assessing the extent of anxiety-related symptoms such as dizziness and breathing difficulties over the prior 2 weeks. Adjustments were made to adhere to recommendations from a previous validation in a Nepali context (64), which indicated a specificity and sensitivity around 0.90. The final (continuous) measure is composed of the sum of seventeen, 4-point scale items, where a higher score indicates poorer well-being. Summary statistics of this and all other variables can be found in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary statistics of variables.

Grit is the Duckworth Grit Scale (65), which has an internal consistency around 0.80. Grit has previously served as a proxy for positive well-being, including among adolescent/young adult populations (67, 68). The sum of ten, 5-point Likert-scale items is divided by five to obtain the final (continuous) measure used in estimation, where a higher score indicates more grit.

PMPU was operationalized through the use of Bianchi and Phillip's (66) Problematic Mobile Phone Use survey. This 27 item, 5-point Likert-scale instrument assesses dimensions of problematic phone use. It has been validated in terms of internal reliability (0.93) and validity in capturing the addictive tendencies associated with maladaptive use. The final (continuous) measure used in estimation is the sum of the 27 items, where a higher score is indicative of more (problematic) use.

To allow for proper identification of our empirical model, we also include the use of three variables believed to influence respondent's PMPU, but not expected to directly influence well-being. In traditional econometric approaches, these would be deemed the “instrumental variables.” PC is the cost of the respondent's mobile phone in thousands of Nepali rupees (NR), ascertained from an opened ended question. PC2 is simply the square of this measure. Use of these variables stems from the assumption that amount of phone usage depends on its cost, with more usage at lower cost levels (69, 70). FPMPU is a modified version of (66) survey instrument, rephrasing six of the questions to reflect respondent's perceptions of their closest friend's interactions with his/her mobile phone, where the assumption is that the behaviors of a close friend in relation to his/her phone usage will correlate with those of the respondent's own usage. Summation of these six, 5-point Likert-scale questions produces the continuous measure used in estimation.

Bully is the summation of three binary indicators for having been bullied in the prior month outside of school, inside of school, and having been physically hurt by someone at school. Framing and use of these measures is consistent with large-scale national and international work examining adolescent life to determine overall well-being (71).

While bullying is clearly indicated as a potential problem among Nepali youth (72), we also acknowledge the existence of other socio-cultural environments in adolescent lives which are likely to also influence well-being outcomes. Adolescent health and development is strongly affected by social factors including family (7375) and academic pressure/competition (76). Furthermore, there is also evidence of how social support can buffer against stressful situations (7678). We control for each of these elements through FE*, AP*, and SS*, respectively. SS* is measured based on six related variables, while the other latent construct have four each, in a manner consistent with prior work on adolescent well-being [such as (20, 79)]. Additional details on these constructs and all other variables, including descriptive statistics according to gender-grouping are presented in the Appendix in Supplementary Material, along with a correlation matrix of the key scale variables to provide preliminary insights on the hypothesized relationships.

Estimation Strategy

While a reduced form system of equations may be a natural way to illustrate the model and causal channels laid out in Equations (1–3) above, doing so may mask underlying channels/complexities central to our narrative. The presence of three latent structures which play important controlling roles in our structural model requires a different estimation strategy allowing the flexibility to account for the measurement error associated with these constructs. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was chosen as the primary estimation strategy, given its application in prior work on health issues, family/peer dynamics, self-efficacy, depression, and psychotherapy (80).

Through maximum likelihood estimation, with iterative computation, SEM allows us to combine measurement models, which involve the relationships between observed (i.e., exogenous) measurements and latent constructs, with path analysis models that relate all variables. SEM allows one to model the measurement error (as opposed to the traditional statistical approach of assuming no error). SEM also eliminates problems with multicollinearity and reduces random error through utilizing multiple measures to describe one latent construct. Finally, use of SEM allows one to specify the appropriate variance-covariance structure for the system of equations based on prior knowledge and assumptions. In straight forward terms, the goal in SEM is to summarize this variance-covariance matrix and compare the estimated/implied variance-covariance matrix from the proposed structural model to the observed matrix from the data to explain as much of the variance as possible (81).

With these capabilities in mind, our structural model is enhanced with the addition of a measurement model representing the latent variables. Within each equation, j is the number of measurement variables associated with the respective latent construct.

APji=ϕ0+ϕj1APi*+u4j    j=1,,4    (4)
FEji=Υ0+Υj1FEi*+u5j   j=1,,4    (5)
SSji=δ0+δj1SSi*+u6j  j=1,,6    (6)

We undertook two estimation methods of our full system: linear probability and generalized linear approaches. The key difference, as it pertains to our work, is the treatment of the measurement components for our latent constructs. Likert-scale survey items are often treated as interval; however, it is often more accurate to treat them as ordinal2. A generalized linear approach can be used under such scenarios where the estimation involves non-continuous variables which cannot be estimated under the typical assumptions of linear regression (81) 3. Under both approaches, we assume block-independence between the structural (Equations, 1-3) and the measurement (Equations, 4-6) systems. Additionally, we note that the parameter estimate of the respective latent variable's impact on the first measured indicator for each latent variable (e.g., SS1i, FE1i, AP1i) is normalized to 1 so that the magnitude of the latent constructs can be pegged against that measure.

With the potential for simultaneity between PMPU and the well-being outcomes (particularly A) implied by literature, and in the absence of over-identification testing typically associated with traditional regression techniques, we explored the strength of the model using three different covariance and model specification structures for the exogenous/observed variables. These results are presented in Tables 3, 4. Our full model specified covariances be estimated between Anx and Grit, PMPU and Anx, and PMPU and Grit, with no demographic controls included in the equation for PMPU. The second structure constrained the covariance between PMPU and Anx and SS* and FE* to be zero, while maintaining no demographic controls in the equation for PMPU. The third model allowed for demographic controls in the PMPU equation. Goodness of fit diagnostics are reported for those results estimated under linear probability assumptions.

We determined the best fit model between the three models based on lowest Akaike's Information Criteria (AIC), and the remainder of analyses were performed based on this choice, for each estimation method. Assessment of the construct validity of the measurement models was performed, and order and rank conditions confirmed for model identification. Estimation of indirect and total effects for the various pressure points (most notably for B) was also performed, with results presented in Tables 4, 5.

Various sensitivity analyses were performed to test the strength of our findings, including the use of traditional econometric techniques which are often conventionally employed to manage various forms of endogeneity including measurement error and simultaneity. Detailed information on such approaches and their results can be found in the Appendix in Supplementary Material.

Results

Basic Statistics

Summary statistics of all variables used are presented in Table 1. The study sample had an average age of 17.6, with 51% of them being female. Among the sample, 48% came from rural secondary schools. The average cost of mobile phones was 19.35 (thousand) NR.

The mean score of Anx was 13.38, which according to previous validation of the BAI (64) indicate at least mild levels of anxiety, on average, in the study sample4. Grit showed an average score of 3.26, and the average PMPU score was 86.65. This indicates more than half of the population have indicators of addictive mobile phone use, placing them above the midline of the scale. FPMPU had an average score of 20.09, indicating respondents perceive on average, even higher levels of problematic use among their closest friend than when responding to assessments of their own use.

There are overall very high indications of pressure in the academic environment to perform well in school, along with perceptions of the environment as being highly controlling and competitive (average scores around 4 on a 1/5 agreement scale). There is also a measurable presence of violence and strict oversight within the family/cultural environment, as indicated by average scores between 2.5 and 3.5 on a 1/5 agreement scale. With regard to social support measures, more than a quarter of those adolescents in the sample do not have someone from whom they could borrow money from or stay with in times of trouble. Further, more than one-third of the sample claim they have no one to confide in about violence or to deal with harassing situations, and less than half of the adolescents surveyed have a place to meet same sex friends or are a member of a social club/youth group.

Estimation Results

In Table 2 are the results of estimating our full structural model with the latent variables measured according to linear probability estimation. Across all models, the fit statistics meet standard criterion/practices for assessing goodness of fit (81, 82). Parameter estimates indicate a statistically significant effect of PMPU on both well-being outcomes. Higher PMPU scores indicate lower Grit and higher Anx. Bully has a statistically significant impact on well-being measures, with the direction of association mirroring that of PMPU. There is no statistically significant effect of PMPU with Anx, but there is a positive covariance with Grit. There is a statistically significant, negative, covariance between the two well-being measures, across all models. These results are in-line with those indicated from preliminary Pearson correlations (found in the Appendix in Supplementary Material) indicating statistically significant inverse correlations between PMPU and both Grit and Bully, and a positive correlation between PMPU and Anx.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of linear probability estimation for structural model of adolescent life influences on well-being.

AP* has a positive significant effect on Grit and PMPU, but no effect on Anx. FE* appears to have a statistically significant positive effect on PMPU, only. Estimated covariances indicate significant positive associations between the latent variables representing the family and academic environments, and between the academic environment and social support.

All three measures used to allow for identification of the model (the “instruments”) and used to explain PMPU, are statistically significant. PC and FPMPU have a positive relationship with respondent's PMPU measure, and PC2 a negative association. While results do not indicate any significant effects of demographic controls on PMPU, females (X1) are predicted to have lower grit and higher anxiety. Rural adolescents (X3) appear to have lower scores on our measure of anxiety, as well.

The results based on generalized linear modeling map similarly to those results presented above and are presented in Table 3. Again, PMPU and Bully are shown to have statistically significant relationships with well-being outcomes (positive for Anx, negative for Grit). Here, we see significant positive effects of Bully on PMPU, along with those previously seen from the latent pressures in the academic and family environments. Females and rural adolescent continue to be indicated to have positive and negative effects, respectively, on Anx, with no significant effects on Grit. Older adolescents, are also seen to have higher A under this approach. The measures used for identification and the referenced key covariances from above continue to be statistically significant as before.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of generalized linear estimation for structural model of adolescent life influences on well-being.

Based on AIC values, the results presented in column (3) of both Tables 3, 4, indicate the best fit between the three model structures. Thus, all subsequent analyses were based on this approach. As previously indicated, full assessment of a structural model also requires assessment of the validity of the constructs used in the measurement model portion of the full model. Average variance extracted (AVE) and construct reliability (CR) values for our model are all above the 0.7 level indicated for good convergent validity (81). To assess discriminant validity, the AVE value should be larger than the squared correlations between two constructs, to reduce any lingering multicollinearity issues (81). For our model, this does not appear to be the case, regardless of estimation approach. Thus, the measurement model was also deemed to have divergent validity.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Mediation analysis for linear probability estimation of structural model of adolescent life influences on well-being.

Mediation analysis entails disaggregating the indirect and direct effects of the mediation pathway depicted in Panel C of Table 5. As visualized in the full structural model (approach number three) depicted in Figure 3, the treatment variables analyzed are those seven variables which have the potential to exhibit an indirect effect (e.g., they have a pathway which passes through PMPU).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Mediation analysis for generalized linear estimation of structural model of adolescent life influences on well-being.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Final structural model.

Looking at Panels A and B in Tables 4, 5, there are consistently significant direct and total effects of Bully on both well-being outcome measures, regardless of estimation approach. Additionally, there are consistently significant direct and total effects of being female on anxiety. Interpretation of indirect and mediation effects is slightly different between the two estimation approaches. So, for the sake of brevity, we detail the results from generalized linear modeling, given our belief that the assumptions underlying the measurement model with this approach may more accurately reflect the underlying processes and relationships our structural model attempts to capture, based on the data generating processes.

Referring to Table 5, one can see significant indirect effects of Bully, AP*, and FE* on both measures of well-being. This implies that there is a statistically significant impact on the well-being measures, which passes through the mediating variable of PMPU. FE* only has a significant indirect effect (indicative of full mediation), whereas Bully has significant indirect and direct effects, indicating only partial mediation. As it pertains to impacts on Grit, AP* appears to only be partially mediated, given significant indirect and direct effects. Interpretation of the mediation effects of PMPU on the relationship between AP* and Anx is less clear. Overall, there are no mediation effects of PMPU on the relationships between demographic controls and well-being outcome measures.

Discussion, Policy Implications and Conclusions

It is imperative that government and policy makers focus on ways to improve the well-being of adolescents, given current negative trajectories seen worldwide, and particularly in developing world nations such as Nepal. There is growing interest in using mobile-based (e.g., mHealth) interventions to combat such issues as high suicide and bullying rates (4). While adolescent use of mobile technology is believed to be high, existing literature focusing on mHealth has primarily focused on the believed cost and reach benefits potentially offered. To date, though, there has been a noted lack of economic data to support the use of digital health interventions (9), and coupled with that is a recognition of the importance of acknowledging potential unintended downsides to its implementation.

There is evidence from developed-world contexts of the negative mental health and well-being outcomes that are highly linked with overuse of mobile/smartphones (83, 84). Such findings are also being confirmed with initial studies of such issues in developing world contexts. However, clarity of the directionality of these relationships within the “dark side” of technology are proving hard to determine (28, 85). The small number of rigorous empirical examinations on these topics primarily neglect any underlying motivational framework to answer why adolescents are turning to their mobile phones. Or, if a motivational framework is emphasized, there is lack of strong statistical analyses to ensure validity of the theories postulated.

Thus, the overarching purpose of this study was to evaluate the presence and associations of PMPU in Nepali adolescents, using a strong analytic approach, framed by a well-supported conceptual theory. By incorporating assessment of both a positive and negative well-being outcome, we have attempted to capture a more holistic vision of the relationships underlying the connections between need thwarting contexts, PMPU, and well-being. We capture two measures of the potential for life success based on the downward trajectory chronic anxiety can provide, vs. the upward trajectory of grit. Using a full structural modeling approach, which accounts for the measurement error in socio-cultural environmental latent constructs and the covariance between key variables, we have been able to produce robust results which provide new insights and directions for future research into these topics.

As a consequence of this research, we have expanded mHealth literature by looking at the potential downsides of phone use which may come up as negative externalities to mHealth initiatives. In addition, by framing our analysis according to BPNT, we are able to capture motivational factors which may trigger turning to phones in the first place, and have moved beyond analyses based solely on personality traits [such as (86, 87)]5. Our application of BPNT has also moved that literature forward by applying it to technology use outside of video games/dating apps, and beyond applications in the classroom/academic functioning. We have also directly addressed simultaneity concerns in both our main modeling framework through accounting for covariances, and in our detailed sensitivity analyses using multiple econometric techniques and testing (summarized in Appendix in Supplementary Material).

Key results from our work indicate support for all four of our initial hypotheses. Those adolescents scoring higher on our measure of bullying are seen to exhibit higher levels of anxiety symptomology and lower on indicators of grit (H1). Those adolescent who report more bullying are seen to also have higher indicators for PMPU (H2). Beyond our hypotheses, literature which emphasizes the large portion of adolescents' time endowment spent at school (76, 91) allows for the significant direct effects of academic pressure on well-being outcomes to make sense. Similarly, the significant direct effects we see for females having higher anxiety measures is in congruence with existing research which indicates that negative mental health outcomes are often found to be more common among female (vs. male) adolescent populations (92, 93).

Significant indirect effects of bullying on well-being outcomes, mediated by our measure of PMPU, provide support for H3, and are the strongest indicators in support of BPNT being an appropriate framework through which to view the PMPU-well-being relationship. The additional significant indirect effects for pressure from the family environment and academic environment on well-being, mediated by PMPU, further provide support for the role that need thwarting contexts play in reducing overall well-being. These findings are in line with researchers who have pushed forward a “need-density” hypothesis, wherein those who have less basic needs met, may also be less able to autonomously regulate activities and make congruent decisions that match their aims and values (94, 95). They are therefore more vulnerable to technology overuse, and a vicious cycle can continue where seeking need fulfillment may ultimately further need frustration.

Our finding of a statistically significant covariance between our two outcomes measures, supports our final hypothesis (H4). Calculated as a negative correlation of −0.09, this finding supports our use of a full structural equation modeling approach which accounts for this interconnectedness. The ability to account for covariances between PMPU and the two well-being outcomes has also allowed us to realize a key insight into the lack of a significant covariance (i.e., simultaneous relationship) between A and PMPU. This finding appears contrary to much of the literature examining problematic phone use and mental health, which has pushed for understanding the relationship as bi-directional (28, 34). However, it is supported by findings by Pivetta et al. (96) who also did not find evidence of pre-existing psychopathology exerting a significant influence on average smartphone user's behavior. The stability of this finding is confirmed in our sensitivity analyses (see Appendix in Supplementary Material), where econometric tests for statistical exogeneity between our measures of PMPU and anxiety are not rejected.

Intriguingly, there are unexpected significant covariances (and concerns for endogeneity) indicated between PMPU and our measure of grit. Such a relationship has not been previously found or analyzed in work looking at mobile technology. Prior evidence of grit's influence on other maladaptive behaviors such as substance use (97, 98) and video game addiction (99) has been indicated in literature, but there is also literature which found no such associations (100, 101). The evidence supporting the important role that grit plays in the healthy development and success of individuals, and particularly adolescents is growing (102, 103). The connections between technology use and grit may offer a new and unique direction in which future research in these areas could focus.

In study of our mediation effects, there are instances in which indirect and/or direct effects are statistically significant, while total effects are not. It has been shown that it is possible to have such a situation, and this can be explained by the presence of several mediating paths that cancel each other out (104). This would be viewed as competitive mediation, as compared to complementary mediation where indirect and direct effects both exist and move in the same direction, or indirect-only mediation (105, 106). Should further exploration of each adolescent pressure/environmental factor accounted for in our model be pursued, such work may entail further breaking down analyses of such pathways.

Future work may also benefit from further exploration of additional unexpected outcomes in this study, including the lack of significance of SS*. While not part of our research hypotheses, the findings of complete lack of significance in our main estimation models for this variable is contrary to what is found in typical regression approaches to the system of equations we estimate (see Appendix in Supplementary Material), and what would be expected from literature (77, 107). Initial speculation for this outcome revolves around the possibility that this was a latent construct. AVE and CR values are the lowest for social support, which may indicate that the strength of our measures for this construct are weaker than the other two.

Additional limitations to our work include that while many studies listed previously have looked at impacts of smartphone use on adolescents' sleeping problems, we did not specifically measure this or focus on it, although there are references to sleep troubles within the PMPU survey instrument we used. We also could have used a different set of explanatory “instrumental variables” to ensure identification within our equation for PMPU. Previous authors who have used instrumental variable techniques similar to those used in our sensitivity analyses have captured phone use (not necessarily problematic use phone) with measures of Wi-Fi capabilities, phone download volume, and 4G streaming capabilities (69, 108). The limitations in internet coverage in the developing world and length limitations in our survey to avoid survey fatigue meant that we were limited on which elements of adolescents' technology use to capture. However, future empirical work could consider capturing such measures if additional survey development on this topic were to be pursued. In any future survey development on these topics, it might also be useful to consider inclusion of a specific need thwarting/need satisfaction scale (109) to delineate differences in the three needs of autonomy, relatedness, and competence and their unique roles in the relationships indicated by our model.

By incorporating key specific factors of life adolescents face which can satisfy/thwart needs in this study, and viewing need thwarting through a more all-encompassing lens [like (56, 110)], we are able to gain insights into ways that future interventions can be focused. Additionally, results of this study are confirmation of the universality of needs (111, 112), and additional confirmation that BPNT applies across cultures. This implies that the best way to focus any health/well-being interventions aimed at Nepali adolescents would be to seek out ways to improve their perceptions that their needs for autonomy, relatedness, and competence are being met.

Our results do support literature's findings of poorer well-being outcomes from maladaptive/problematic use of mobile phones. Consequently, care must be taken if mHealth interventions are the path that policymakers choose, given that many mental health apps have not been properly vetted/tested (113, 114). If such interventions are deployed in conjunction with, or subsequent to, non-technology based interventions which focus on meeting the three basic needs, such concerns may be dampened. If adolescents no longer see mobile phones as a compensatory means by which to fulfill their unmet or thwarted needs in life, then the prevalence of problematic phone use may go down, and any negative well-being outcomes from that maladaptive use would also fall.

Work in the developed world has focused on the roles of in-school health systems and counselors as a potential front-line approach to negative well-being outcomes (115, 116). However, in many developing world contexts, there is not even a school nurse, let alone a mental/emotional counselor. Among our sample, 62.4% report that there is a school counselor at their school, while 85.2% claim that they would like one present with whom they could discuss their worries/concerns. So, a first step in non-technology-based interventions would be to try to raise the perceived importance of having health professionals on staff at schools and reducing associated stigma. However, in the absence of financial support for such initiatives, or as the recent global pandemic which removed in-person schooling opportunities across the globe have shown, there may be times where society must rely on digital approaches to address the socio-emotional needs of youth (117, 118). It is in the face of such situations, that careful consideration must be given to proper form and approach in digitally-based approaches to healthcare, including mental/emotional support.

If mHealth interventions/approaches are properly focused, some studies have shown both passive and active entertainment media, including social media, can provide challenges, choices, and relational elements potentially conducive to need satisfaction (119). Apps such as the “Woebot” (120) and the “Youper” (121) have being deployed which, unlike traditional mHealth apps that focus on mind-calming exercises or simply providing resources, utilize an AI chatbot which chats with you like a friend, asking questions, and assessing your responses. This type of application genuinely may fulfill such needs as relatedness and autonomy.

Additionally, there needs to be buy-in and input from the end-users in the platform/app development, to ensure the highest likelihood and levels of engagement which match the needs and desires of the end-user patients (122, 123). It may be useful to create gender-targeted interventions, given evidence from literature and supported in this work that females exhibit higher anxiety symptomology (124). Specifically in the Nepali context, females face strong pressures for early marriage (19, 125) and menstrual taboos (126) which may exacerbate any lack of need fulfillment. Furthermore, in Nepal it is common for adolescents to share their phone with one or more other individuals. Consequently, privacy concerns may be a real barrier to full adoption/success of mobile-based interventions. Within our study, over 30% of adolescents report that they are not the only user of their phone. Close to 32% of the adolescents surveyed say that they worry about their privacy, consistent with 53% of them reporting that others take and go through their phones without permission.

Overall, though, investing in health systems and looking at minimizing the treatment gap is forecast to produce economic returns of 2.3 to 3.0 to 1 by 2030, indicating a long-term gain of $2.3–3.00 for every $1 invested in prevention and treatment (127). Such figures are based on not only reduction in long-term healthcare costs, but also increased economic productivity of healthier individuals contributing to society. In less than 10 years, today's adolescent populations will be those contributors to society. The “unhappiness” Inverted-U curve is real and ubiquitous across the world (128, 129), and if adolescents are already struggling, they are only going to get that much worse down the road. By finding ways to improve their positive well-being, now, there are enormous gains to be had long-term. Our work has shown that while PMPU does indeed appear to be a culprit of some of the negative well-being outcomes seen in a developing-world context such as Nepal, this connection can also be partially explained by the preexisting need thwarting contexts that these adolescent face. Contexts that drive them to phones in an attempt to meet their needs. Finding ways to meet those needs, whether through carefully targeted mHealth apps or non-technology-based approaches, may be a critical step toward having a next generation of world leaders who surpass the current in their levels of personal, emotional, and financial success.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board at the University of New Mexico. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

SY researched all background literatures, conceived of the primary research questions, generated the survey, led the IRB approval process, performed data analysis, drafted and edited the article. AB contributed to survey development, article formatting and editing, analysis guidance, and approval of all contents as appropriate to be listed as an author.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2021.563515/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^Anxiety is a measure of feelings of worry, nervousness, or unease, typically about an imminent event or something with an uncertain outcome, and is a typical measure of negative well-being, which hinders a multitude of long-term successes (4850). Grit, on the other hand, is a measure of perseverance and passion for achieving long-term goals, which serves as a useful measure of positive well-being potential and ability to succeed in life. Grittier people have been shown to have greater educational attainment/achievement, entrepreneurial potential, and earnings (51, 52).

2. ^The difference from an answer of “Strongly Agree” to “Agree” (coded as 1 and 2) is not treated as equivalent to that of between “Agree” and “Neutral” (coded as 2 and 3) in an intuitive sense under an ordinal treatment.

3. ^Through SEM based on a generalized linear model, the estimation is essentially a generalization of nonlinear least squares, where the likelihood is derived under the assumption that each observed variable is independent and identically distributed (iid) across the estimation sample and observed variables are also assumed to be independent of each other. These assumptions are conditional on the latent variables and observed exogenous variables. This is opposed to estimation with a linear probability treatment of the observed measures, where the likelihood is derived based on the means, variances, and covariances of the observed exogenous variables (not conditional upon). Additionally, estimation based on the generalized linear model removes any assumptions of normality of the observed exogenous variables, and instead uses a link function such as logit for distributional families such as ordinal or Bernoulli.

4. ^Note that use of this survey instrument was not meant to diagnose generalized anxiety disorder, but rather to serve as an indicator of overall experience of anxiety-related symptoms – indicative of negatively oriented emotional and mental well-being.

5. ^Unlike typical personality traits such as neuroticisms or introversion, grit has been viewed and confirmed as a learned characteristic, which develops over time and can be affected by life experiences (88, 89), much like anxiety has traditionally been recognized as situation/environmentally triggered/dependent (90).

References

1. UNICEF. Adolescents Overview. Unicef Data (2019). Available online at: https://data.unicef.org/topic/adolescents/overview/. (accessed March 1, 2020).

2. UN. Report of the United Nations Expert Group Meeting on Adolescents, Youth and Development. New York, NY (2011).

3. Patalay P, Fitzsimons E. Development and predictors of mental ill-health and wellbeing from childhood to adolescence. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2018) 53:1311–23. doi: 10.1007/s00127-018-1604-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. WHO-SEARO. Mental Health Status of Adolescents in South-East Asia: Evidence for Action. New Delhi (2017). Available online at: http://www.searo.who.int/entity/mental_health/documents/9789290225737/en/. (accessed March 1, 2020).

Google Scholar

5. Souraya S, Canning T, Farmer P. Addressing Anxiety and Depression: A Whole System Approach. Doha: WISH Anxiety and Depression Forum 2018 (2018). Available online at: https://www.wish.org.qa/wp-content/uploads/2018/11/IMPJ6078-WISH-2018-Anxiety-and-Depression-181026.pdf. (accessed March 1, 2020).

6. Naslund JA, Aschbrenner KA, Araya R, Marsch LA, Unützer J, Patel V, et al. Digital technology for treating and preventing mental disorders in low-income and middle-income countries: a narrative review of the literature. Lancet Psychiatry. (2017) 4:486–500. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30096-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Payne HE, Lister C, West JH, Bernhardt JM. Behavioral functionality of mobile apps in health interventions: a systematic review of the literature. JMIR MHealth UHealth. (2015) 3:e20. doi: 10.2196/mhealth.3335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Zhao J, Freeman B, Li M. Can mobile phone apps influence people's health behavior change? An evidence review. J Med Intern Res. (2016) 18:e287. doi: 10.2196/jmir.5692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Iribarren SJ, Cato K, Falzon L, Stone PW. What is the economic evidence for MHealth? A systematic review of economic evaluations of mhealth solutions edited by cathy mihalopoulos. PLoS ONE. (2017) 12:e0170581. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170581

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Pew Research Center. Teens, Social Media & Technology. Report (2018).

Google Scholar

11. Rideout V, Robb MB. The Common Sense Census: Media Use by Tweens and Teens, 2019. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media (2019).

Google Scholar

12. Radovic A, Badawy SM. Technology use for adolescent health and wellness. Pediatrics. (2020) 145(Suppl. 2):S186–94. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-2056G

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Grist R, Porter J, Stallard P. Mental health mobile apps for preadolescents and adolescents: a systematic review. J Med Intern Res. (2017) 19:e176. doi: 10.2196/jmir.7332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Hollis C, Falconer CJ, Martin JL, Whittington C, Stockton S, Glazebrook C, et al. Annual research review: digital health interventions for children and young people with mental health problems - a systematic and meta-review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2017) 58:474–503. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Sohn S, Rees P, Wildridge B, Kalk NJ, Carter B. Prevalence of problematic smartphone usage and associated mental health outcomes amongst children and young people: a systematic review, meta-analysis and GRADE of the evidence. BMC Psychiatry. (2019) 19:356. doi: 10.1186/s12888-019-2393-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Stiglic N, Viner RM. Effects of screentime on the health and well-being of children and adolescents: a systematic review of reviews. BMJ Open. (2019) 9:e023191. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Davey S, Davey A. Assessment of smartphone addiction in Indian adolescents: a mixed method study by systematic-review and meta-analysis approach. Int J Prev Med. (2014) 5:1500–11.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

18. Patel SASAW, Puri PA. A Study of mobile phone addiction and mental health among adolescent girls studying in various streams. Int J Indian Psychol. (2017) 5:146–51. doi: 10.25215/0501.099

CrossRef Full Text

19. Adhikari RP, Upadhaya N, Pokharel R, Suwal BR, Shrestha MP, Subedi PK. Health and social vulnerability of adolescents in Nepal. SM J Public Health Epidemiol. (2016) 2:1032.

Google Scholar

20. Amin S, Bajracharya A, Chau M, Puri M. Highlights from the Unicef Adolescent Development and Participation Baseline Study. New York, NY: Population Council (2014). doi: 10.31899/pgy10.1017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Ghimire A, Samuels F, Giri I, Adhikari P. Communication Strategies for Addressing Discriminatory Social Norms in Marriage and Education for Adolescent Girls in Nepal. London: Overseas Development Institute (ODI) (2015).

22. UNDP, editor. Human Development for Everyone. Human Development Report 2016. New York, NY: United Nations Development Programme (2016).

23. World Bank. The Little Data Book on Information and Communication Technology 2017. The World Bank (2017).

Google Scholar

24. Kamrudin A. Assessing the mental health of girls in Nepal: a study of the impact of societal factors on former child soldiers conscripted into Nepal's maoist movement and non-conscripted children Atlanta, GA: Emory University (2009).

25. Luitel NP, Jordans MJD, Sapkota RP, Tol WA, Kohrt BA, Thapa SB, et al. Conflict and mental health: a cross-sectional epidemiological study in Nepal. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2013) 48:183–93. doi: 10.1007/s00127-012-0539-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Meyers J, Ozonoff A, Baruwal A, Pande S, Harsha A, Sharma R, et al. Combining healthcare-based and participatory approaches to surveillance: trends in diarrheal and respiratory conditions collected by a mobile phone system by community health workers in rural Nepal. Edited by Delmiro Fernandez-Reyes. PLoS ONE 11: e0152738. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152738

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Oh DH, Dabbagh A, Goodson JL, Strebel PM, Thapa S, Giri JN, et al. Real-Time Monitoring of Vaccination Campaign Performance Using Mobile Phones — Nepal, 2016. Vol. 65, No. 39. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. US Department of Health Services/Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2016). doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6539a5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Elhai JD, Dvorak RD, Levine JC, Brian Hall J. Problematic smartphone use: a conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. J Affect Disord. (2017) 207:251–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Reid Chassiakos Y, Radesky J, Christakis D, Moreno MA, Cross C, Council On Communications and Media. Children and adolescents and digital media. Pediatrics. (2016) 138:e20162593. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Vernon L, Modecki KL, Barber BL. Mobile Phones in the bedroom: trajectories of sleep habits and subsequent adolescent psychosocial development. Child Dev. (2018) 89:66–77. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Lemola S, Perkinson-Gloor N, Brand S, Dewald-Kaufmann JF, Grob A. Adolescents' electronic media use at night, sleep disturbance, and depressive symptoms in the smartphone age. J. Youth Adolesc. (2015) 44:405–18. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0176-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Seo DG, Park Y, Kim MK, Park J. Mobile phone dependency and its impacts on adolescents' social and academic behaviors. Comput Hum Behav. (2016) 63:282–92. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Harwood J, Dooley JJ, Scott AJ, Joiner R. Constantly connected – The effects of smart-devices on mental health. Comput Hum Behav. (2014) 34:267–72. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.02.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Billieux J, Van der Linden M, Rochat L. The role of impulsivity in actual and problematic use of the mobile phone. Appl Cogn Psychol. (2008) 22:1195–210. doi: 10.1002/acp.1429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Cheever NA, Rosen LD, Carrier LM, Chavez A. Out of sight is not out of mind: the impact of restricting wireless mobile device use on anxiety levels among low, moderate and high users. Comput Hum Behav. (2014) 37:290–97. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Clayton RB, Leshner G, Almond A. The extended iself: the impact of iphone separation on cognition, emotion, and physiology. J Comput Mediat Commun. (2015) 20:119–35. doi: 10.1111/jcc4.12109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Lu X, Katoh T, Chen Z, Nagata T, Kitamura T. Text messaging: are dependency and excessive use discretely different for Japanese University students? Psychiatry Res. (2014) 216:255–62. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Jun S. The reciprocal longitudinal relationships between mobile phone addiction and depressive symptoms among korean adolescents. Comput Hum Behav. (2016) 58:179–86. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.12.061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Eijnden RJJM, Meerkerk GJA, Vermulst A, Spijkerman R, Engels RCME. Online communication, compulsive internet use, and psychosocial well-being among adolescents: a longitudinal study. Dev Psychol. (2008) 44:655–65. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Yen JY, Cheng-Fang Y, Chen CS, Chang YH, Yeh YC, Ko CH. The bidirectional interactions between addiction, behaviour approach and behaviour inhibition systems among adolescents in a prospective study. Psychiatry Res. (2012) 200:588–92. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol. (2000) 55:68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Zhang J, Liu Y, Zhang J, Chen C. Linking psychological strain and suicide ideation: a test of the mediating effect of self-determination among Chinese workers. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2018) 206:362–9. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Gui J, Kono S, Walker GJ. Basic psychological need satisfaction and affect within the leisure sphere. Leisure Stud. (2019) 38:114–27. doi: 10.1080/02614367.2018.1539866

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Vandenkerckhove B, Brenning K, Vansteenkiste M, Luyten P, Soenens B. The explanatory role of basic psychological need experiences in the relation between dependency, self-criticism and psychopathology in adolescence. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2019) 41:574–88. doi: 10.1007/s10862-019-09719-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Deci EL, Vansteenkiste M. Self-determination theory and basic need satisfaction: understanding human development in positive psychology. Ricerche Psicol. (2004) 27:23–40.

Google Scholar

46. Masur PK, Reinecke L, Ziegele M, Quiring O. The interplay of intrinsic need satisfaction and facebook specific motives in explaining addictive behavior on facebook. Comput Hum Behav. (2014) 39:376–86. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Young KS, Yue XD, Ying L. “Prevalence estimates and etiologic models of internet addiction,” In: Young KS, de Abreu CN editors. Internet Addiction: A Handbook and Guide to Evaluation and Treatment Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons (2011). p. 3–17. doi: 10.1002/9781118013991.ch1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Knapp M, Ardino V, Brimblecombe N, Evans-Lacko S, Ieemi V, King D, et al. Youth Mental Health: New Economic Evidence. PSSRU: University of Kent. (2016). Available online at: https://www.pssru.ac.uk/publications/pub-5160/. (accessed March 1, 2020).

Google Scholar

49. Knapp M, King D, Healey A, Thomas C. Economic outcomes in adulthood and their associations with antisocial conduct, attention deficit and anxiety problems in childhood. J Ment Health Policy Econ. (2011) 14:137–47.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

50. Snell T, Knapp M, Healey A, Guglani S, Evans-Lacko S, Fernandez JL, et al. Economic impact of childhood psychiatric disorder on public sector services in Britain: estimates from national survey data. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2013) 54:977–85. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Beyhan O. University students grit level and grit achievement relation. Soc Sci Educ Res Rev. (2016) 3:13–23.

Google Scholar

52. Mooradian T, Matzler K, Uzelac B, Bauer F. Perspiration and inspiration: grit and innovativeness as antecedents of entrepreneurial success. J Econ Psychol. (2016) 56:232–43. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2016.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Shapka JD, Onditi HZ, Collie RJ, Lapidot-Lefler N. Cyberbullying and cybervictimization within a cross-cultural context: a study of Canadian and Tanzanian adolescents. Child Dev. (2018) 89:89–99. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. McKinnon B, Gariépy G, Sentenac M, Elgar FJ. Adolescent suicidal behaviours in 32 low- and middle-income countries. Bull World Health Organ. (2016) 94:340–50F. doi: 10.2471/BLT.15.163295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Tian L, Tian Q, Huebner ES. School-related social support and adolescents' school-related subjective well-being: the mediating role of basic psychological needs satisfaction at school. Soc Indic Res. (2016) 128:105–29. doi: 10.1007/s11205-015-1021-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Orkibi H, Ronen T. Basic psychological needs satisfaction mediates the association between self-control skills and subjective well-being. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:936. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Sheldon KM, Gunz A. Psychological needs as basic motives, not just experiential requirements. J Pers. (2009) 77:1467–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00589.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Rigby CS, Ryan RM. Chapter 3: Time Well Spent?. In: L Reinecke, MB Oliver, editors. The Routledge Handbook of Media Use and Well-Being: International Perspectives on Theory and Research on Positive Media Effects. New York, NY: Routledge (2017). p. 34–48.

59. Jin B, Kim J. Grit basic needs satisfaction, and subjective well-being. J Individ Diff. (2017) 38:29. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Jiang W, Jiang J, Du X, Gu D, Sun Y, Zhang Y. Striving and happiness: between- and within-person-level associations among grit, needs satisfaction and subjective well-being. J Posit Psychol. (2019) 15:543–55. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2019.1639796

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Musumari PM, Tangmunkongvorakul A, Srithanaviboonchai K, Techasrivichien T, Suguimoto SP, Ono-Kihara M, et al. Grit is associated with lower level of depression and anxiety among University students in Chiang Mai, Thailand: a cross-sectional study. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0209121. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0209121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Lan X, Ma C, Radin R. Parental autonomy support and psychological well-being in tibetan and han emerging adults: a serial multiple mediation model. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:621. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1988) 56:893–97. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kohrt BA, Kunz RD, Koirala NR, Sharma VD, Nepal MK. Validation of the Nepali version of beck anxiety inventory. J Inst Med. (2003) 26:1–4.

65. Duckworth A. Grit: The Power of Passion and Perseverance. First Scribner hardcover edition. New York, NY: Scribner (2016).

Google Scholar

66. Bianchi A, Phillips JG. Psychological predictors of problem mobile phone use. CyberPsychol Behav. (2005) 8:39–51. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2005.8.39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Vainio MM, Daukantaite D. Grit and different aspects of well-being: direct and indirect relationships via sense of coherence and authenticity. J Happiness Stud. (2016) 17:2119–47. doi: 10.1007/s10902-015-9688-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Hill PL, Burrow AL, Bronk KC. Persevering with positivity and purpose: an examination of purpose commitment and positive affect as predictors of grit J Happiness Stud. (2016) 17:257–69. doi: 10.1007/s10902-014-9593-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Baert S, Vujić S, Amez S, Claeskens M, Daman T, Maeckelberghe A, et al. Smartphone use and academic performance: correlation or causal relationship? Kyklos 73:22–46. doi: 10.1111/kykl.12214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. French MT, Popovici I. That instrument is lousy! In search of agreement when using instrumental variables estimation in substance use research. Health Econ. (2011) 20:127–46. doi: 10.1002/hec.1572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Inchley J, Currie D, Young T, Samdal O, Torsheim T, Augustson L, Mathison F, et al. editors. (2016). Growing Up Unequal: Gender and Socioeconomic Differences in Young People's Health and Well-Being: Health Behaviour in School-Aged Children (HBSC) Study: International Report from the 2013/2014 Survey. Health Policy for Children and Adolescents, no. 7. Copenhagen: World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe.

Google Scholar

72. WHO/CDC. Global School-Based Student Health Survey (GSHS): Nepal 2015 Fact Sheet. 2015. (2015). Available online at: http://www.who.int/ncds/surveillance/gshs/gshs_fs_nepal_2015.pdf. (accessed March 1, 2020).

Google Scholar

73. Kretman SE, Zimmerman MA, Morrel-Samuels M, Hudson D. Chapter 12: adolescent violence: risk, resilience, and prevention. In: RJ DiClemente, JS Santelli, RA Crosby editors. Adolescent Health: Understanding and Preventing Risk Behaviors. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass (2009). p. 213–32.

74. Barber BK, Stolz HE, Olsen JA, Collins WA, Burchinal M. Parental support, psychological control, and behavioral control: assessing relevance across time, culture, and method. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. (2005) 70:i–147. Available online at: www.jstor.org/stable/3701442

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

75. Wang MT, Sheikh-Khalil S. Does parental involvement matter for student achievement and mental health in high school? Child Dev. (2014) 85:610–25. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Miller AB, Esposito-Smythers C, Leichtweis RN. Role of social support in adolescent suicidal ideation and suicide attempts. J Adolesc Health. (2015) 56:286–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.10.265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Berkman LF, Glass T. Social integration, social networks, social support, and health. in Social Epidemiology. 1st ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2000). p. 137–73.

Google Scholar

78. Kohrt BA, Worthman CM. Gender and anxiety in Nepal: the role of social support, stressful life events, and structural violence. CNS Neurosci Ther. (2009) 15:237–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2009.00096.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Kann L, McManus T, Harris WA, Shanklin SL, Flint KH, Queen B, Lowry R, et al. Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance — United States, 2017. MMWR Surv Summ. (2018) 67:1–114. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6708a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. MacCallum RC, Austin JT. Applications of structural equation modeling in psychological research. Annu Rev Psychol. (2000) 51:201–26. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. 4th ed. Methodology in the Social Sciences. New York, NY: The Guilford Press (2016).

Google Scholar

82. Fornell C, Larcker DF. Structural Equation Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error: Algebra and Statistics. (1981). doi: 10.2307/3150980

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. CommonSense. Technology Addiction: Concern, Controversy, Finding Balance. A CommonSense Research Brief. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media (2016).

84. Dubicka B, Theodosiou L. Technology Use and the Mental Health of Children and Young People. College Report CR225. Manchester: Royal College of Psychiatrists (2020).

85. Orben A, Przybylski AK. The association between adolescent well-being and digital technology use. Nat Hum Behav. (2019) 3:173–82. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0506-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Takao M, Takahashi S, Kitamura M. Addictive personality and problematic mobile phone use. CyberPsychol Behav. (2009) 12:501–7. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2009.0022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Hong FY, Chiu SI, Huang DH. A model of the relationship between psychological characteristics, mobile phone addiction and use of mobile phones by taiwanese university female students. Comput Hum Behav. (2012) 28:2152–59. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.06.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Gillion L. Does Birth Weight Influence Grit or Can Grit Be Learned After Birth? Princeton, NJ: Working Paper.

Google Scholar

89. Hoeschler P, Balestra S, Backes-Gellner U. The development of non-cognitive skills in adolescence. Econ Lett. (2018) 163:40–45. doi: 10.1016/j.econlet.2017.11.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. American Psychiatric Association, and American Psychiatric Association, editors. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Deb S, Strodl E, Sun J. Academic stress, parental pressure, anxiety and mental health among indian high school students. Int J Psychol Behav Sci. (2015) 5:26–34. doi: 10.5923/j.ijpbs.20150501.04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Chaplin TM, Gillham JE, Seligman MEP. Gender, anxiety, and depressive symptoms: a longitudinal study of early adolescents. J Early Adolesc. (2009) 29:307–27. doi: 10.1177/0272431608320125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Fletcher JM. Adolescent depression and educational attainment: results using sibling fixed effects. Health Econ. (2009) 19:855–71. doi: 10.1002/hec.1526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Di Domenico SI, Fournier MA, Ayaz H, Ruocco AC. In search of integrative processes: basic psychological need satisfaction predicts medial prefrontal activation during decisional conflict. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2012) 142:967–78. doi: 10.1037/a0030257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Ryan RM, Deci EL, Vansteenkiste M. Autonomy and autonomy disturbances in self-development and psychopathology: research on motivation, attachment clinical process. Dev Psychopathol. (2016) 1:385–483. doi: 10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Pivetta E, Harkin L, Billieux J, Kanjo E, Kuss DJ. Problematic smartphone use: an empirically validated model. Comput Hum Behav. (2019) 100:105–17. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.06.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Guerrero LR, Dudovitz R, Chung PJ, Dosanjh KK, Wong MD. Grit: a potential protective factor against substance use and other risk behaviors among Latino Adolescents. Acad Pediatr. (2016) 16:275–81. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2015.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Griffin ML, McDermott KA, McHugh RK, Fitzmaurice GM, Weiss RD. Grit in patients with substance use disorders. Am J Addict. (2016) 25:652–58. doi: 10.1111/ajad.12460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Borzikowsky C, Bernhardt F. Lost in virtual gaming worlds: grit and its prognostic value for online game addiction: grit and online game addiction. Am J Addict. (2018) 27:433–38. doi: 10.1111/ajad.12762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Maddi SR, Erwin LM, Carmody CL, Villarreal BJ, White M, Gundersen KK. Relationship of hardiness, grit, and emotional intelligence to internet addiction, excessive consumer spending, and gambling. J Posit Psychol. (2013) 8:128–34. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2012.758306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Bessey P. Preferences personality and health behaviors: results from an explorative economic experiment. Int J Health Econ Manage. (2018) 18:437–56. doi: 10.1007/s10754-018-9236-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Duckworth AL, Peterson C, Matthews MD, Kelly DR. Grit: perseverance and passion for long-term goals. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2007) 92:1087. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Eskreis-Winkler L, Duckworth AL, Shulman EP, Beal S. The grit effect: predicting retention in the military, the workplace, school and marriage. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:36. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Hayes AF. Beyond baron and kenny: statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Commun Monogr. (2009) 76:408–20. doi: 10.1080/03637750903310360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Memon MA, Cheah J, Ramayah T, Ting H, Chuah F. Mediation analysis issues and recommendations. J Appl Struct Equat Model. (2018) 2:1–9. doi: 10.47263/JASEM.2(1)01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Zhao X, Lynch JG Jr, Chen Q. Reconsidering baron and kenny: myths and truths about mediation analysis. J Consum Res. (2010) 37:197–206. doi: 10.1086/651257

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Aker S, Sahin MK, Sezgin S, Oguz G. Psychosocial factors affecting smartphone addiction in University students. J Addict Nurs. (2017) 28:215–19. doi: 10.1097/JAN.0000000000000197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Chen L, Yan Z, Tang W, Yang F, Xie X, He J. Mobile phone addiction levels and negative emotions among Chinese young adults: the mediating role of interpersonal problems. Comput Hum Behav. (2016) 55:856–66. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.10.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Chen B, Vansteenkiste M, Beyers W, Boone L, Deci EL, Van der Kaap-Deeder J, Duriez B, et al. (2015). Basic psychological need satisfaction, need frustration, and need strength across four cultures. Motiv Emot. 39:216–36. doi: 10.1007/s11031-014-9450-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Allen JJ, Anderson CA. Satisfaction and frustration of basic psychological needs in the real world and in video games predict internet gaming disorder scores and well-being. Comput Hum Behav. (2018) 84:220–29. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.02.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Rodríguez-Meirinhos A, Antolín-Suárez L, Brenning K, Vansteenkiste M, Oliva A. A bright and a dark path to adolescents' functioning: the role of need satisfaction and need frustration across gender, age, and socioeconomic status. J Happiness Stud. (2019) 21:95–116. doi: 10.1007/s10902-018-00072-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Yu S, Levesque-Bristol C, Maeda Y. General need for autonomy and subjective well-being: a meta-analysis of studies in the US and East Asia. J Happiness Stud. (2018) 19:1863–82. doi: 10.1007/s10902-017-9898-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Seko Y, Kidd S, Wiljer D, McKenzie K. Youth mental health interventions via mobile phones: a scoping review. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Networking 17, 591–602. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Oppenheim S. Should You Trust An App With Your Mental Health? Forbes. Available online at: https://www.forbes.com/sites/serenaoppenheim/2019/01/16/should-you-trust-an-app-with-your-mental-health/#38b4f1ff24b8

115. Lear JG. Health at school: a hidden health care system emerges from the shadows. Health Aff. (2007) 26:409–19. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.26.2.409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Brown C, Dahlbeck DT, Sparkman-Barnes L. (2006). Collaborative relationships: school counselors and non-school mental health professionals working together to improve the mental health needs of students. Profess Sch Counsel. 9:332–35. doi: 10.5330/prsc.9.4.p1757020377428qv

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Badawy SM, Radovic A. Digital approaches to remote pediatric health care delivery during the COVID-19 pandemic: existing evidence and a call for further research. JMIR Pediatr Parent. (2020) 3:e20049. doi: 10.2196/20049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Serlachius A, Badawy SM, Thabrew H. Psychosocial challenges and opportunities for youth with chronic health conditions during the COVID-19 pandemic. JMIR Pediatr Parent. (2020) 3:e23057. doi: 10.2196/23057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Calvo RA, Peters D. Positive Computing: Technology for Wellbeing and Human Potential. Boston, MA: The MIT Press (2014). doi: 10.7551/mitpress/9764.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Karlan P, Bankman J. Can Technology Solve the Mental Health Crisis? With Guests. Ruzek J, Harned Z, Darcy A. Standford Legal (nd). Available online at: https://law.stanford.edu/stanford-legal-on-siriusxm/can-technology-solve-the-mental-health-crisis-with-guests-dr-joe-ruzek-and-zach-harned/. (accessed March 1, 2020).

121. Muzaffar M. The Mental Health App That Checks In on You Like a Friend. The Daily Dose (2019). Available online at: https://www.ozy.com/good-sht/meet-youper-your-personal-therapist/95935/. (accessed March 1, 2020).

122. Perski O, Blandford A, Ubhi HK, West R, Michie S. Smokers' and drinkers' choice of smartphone applications and expectations of engagement: a think aloud and interview study. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. (2017) 17:25. doi: 10.1186/s12911-017-0422-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Perski O, Blandford A, West R, Michie S. Conceptualising engagement with digital behaviour change interventions: a systematic review using principles from critical interpretive synthesis. Transl Behav Med. (2017) 7:254–67. doi: 10.1007/s13142-016-0453-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Chen B, Liu F, Ding S, Ying X, Wang L, Wen Y. Gender differences in factors associated with smartphone addiction: a cross-sectional study among medical college students. BMC Psychiatry. (2017) 17:341. doi: 10.1186/s12888-017-1503-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

125. Maharjan RK, Plan Nepal (Organization) Save the Children (U.S.), World Vision International Nepal, editors. Child Marriage in Nepal: Research Report. Kathmandu: Plan Nepal : Save the Children, Nepal Country Office : World Vision International Nepal (2012).

126. Fatusi AO, Hindin MJ. Adolescents and youth in developing countries: health and development issues in context. J Adolesc. (2010) 33:499–508. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Chisholm D, Sweeny K, Sheehan P, Rasmussen B, Smit F, Cuijpers P, et al. Scaling-up treatment of depression and anxiety: a global return on investment analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. (2016) 3:415–24. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30024-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Blanchflower D. Is Happiness U-Shaped Everywhere? Age and Subjective Well-Being in 132 Countries. w26641. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research (2020). doi: 10.3386/w26641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Blanchflower D. Unhappiness and Age. w26642. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research (2020). doi: 10.3386/w26642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sociocultural, developing country, economic development, technological change, health, quantitative methods, human development, innovation

Citation: Yilmaz SK and Bohara AK (2021) mHealth: Potentials and Risks for Addressing Mental Health and Well-Being Issues Among Nepali Adolescents. Front. Public Health 9:563515. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.563515

Received: 19 May 2020; Accepted: 22 March 2021;
Published: 23 April 2021.

Edited by:

Lourdes Rey, University of Malaga, Spain

Reviewed by:

Siddharth Sarkar, All India Institute of Medical Sciences, India
Sherif Badawy, Northwestern University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Yilmaz and Bohara. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Siobhan K. Yilmaz, yilmazsiobhan@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.