Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 26 April 2021
Sec. Children and Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Global Excellence in Children and Health. View all 9 articles

Breastfeeding Duration Is Associated With Domain-Specific Improvements in Cognitive Performance in 9–10-Year-Old Children

\nDaniel A. Lopez,Daniel A. Lopez1,2John J. FoxeJohn J. Foxe2Yunjiao MaoYunjiao Mao3Wesley K. ThompsonWesley K. Thompson4Hayley J. MartinHayley J. Martin1Edward G. Freedman
Edward G. Freedman2*
  • 1Division of Epidemiology, Department of Public Health Sciences, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, United States
  • 2The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, The Del Monte Institute for Neuroscience, Department of Neuroscience, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, United States
  • 3Division of Health Service Research and Policy, Department of Public Health Sciences, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, United States
  • 4Division of Biostatistics, Department of Family Medicine and Public Health, University of California, San Diego, CA, United States

Significant immunological, physical and neurological benefits of breastfeeding in infancy are well-established, but to what extent these gains persist into later childhood remain uncertain. This study examines the association between breastfeeding duration and subsequent domain-specific cognitive performance in a diverse sample of 9–10-year-olds enrolled in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study®. The analyses included 9,116 children that attended baseline with their biological mother and had complete neurocognitive and breastfeeding data. Principal component analysis was conducted on data from an extensive battery of neurocognitive tests using varimax-rotation to extract a three-component model encompassing General Ability, Executive Functioning, and Memory. Propensity score weighting using generalized boosted modeling was applied to balance the distribution of observed covariates for children breastfed for 0, 1–6, 7–12, and more than 12 months. Propensity score-adjusted linear regression models revealed significant association between breastfeeding duration and performance on neurocognitive tests representing General Ability, but no evidence of a strong association with Executive Function or Memory. Benefits on General Ability ranged from a 0.109 (1–6 months) to 0.301 (>12 months) standardized beta coefficient difference compared to those not breastfed. Results indicate clear cognitive benefits of breastfeeding but that these do not generalize to all measured domains, with implications for public health policy as it pertains to nutrition during infancy.

Introduction

The health benefits of breastfeeding for the mother and child are well-established. They include protection against infection for the child and reduced breast cancer risk for the mother (1). There is less agreement on whether breastfeeding improves cognitive performance in offspring, and whether certain cognitive domains are differentially impacted by breastfeeding. To date, there are only a few studies that have explored the association between breastfeeding duration and child cognitive performance without focusing entirely on intelligence quotient (IQ) and similar measures of general intellectual ability (24).

One explanation posited for the inconsistent results between breastfeeding and improvements in cognition is residual confounding resulting from non-experimental study designs (3). Previous research has noted that there are major social differences in women who choose to breastfeed their children. In the United States, significant disparities in breastfeeding outcomes are seen among individuals from different racial and socioeconomic backgrounds (5). For example, the 2011–2015 National Immunization Survey reported breastfeeding initiation rates of 64.3% for non-Hispanic black infants, and 81.5% for white infants (6). As a result, selection bias in observational studies has been a persistent criticism by those who dispute the association, particularly among studies with participation rates that are differential with respect to participant race or socioeconomic status. In addition, quasi-experimental methods such as propensity scoring have shown diminished associations of breastfeeding and cognitive performance that possibly reflect residual confounding due to the non-randomized design of observational studies (7).

Here, we focus on the association of breastfeeding duration on the following broad components: General Ability, Executive Function, and Memory, with special emphasis on controlling for confounding factors. General Ability is a measure of global intellect (8). Observational studies have been inconsistent in reporting a positive association between breastfeeding and improvements in General Ability. The United Kingdom Millennium Cohort Study, a study consisting of 11,879 term and preterm children, concluded that breastfed children were 1–6 months ahead in their cognitive development when compared to children never breastfed (9). Likewise, the British Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children and the Brazilian Pelotas cohorts showed a strong positive association between breastfeeding and IQ scores, despite the two countries having vastly different breastfeeding patterns (i.e., in the UK breastfeeding is more common in middle/upper SES women, and in Brazil it is more common in lower SES women) (10). The only randomized control trial involving breastfeeding, conducted in Belarus with a sample of 13,889 newborns, concluded that breastfeeding had a significant positive association with IQ scores at age 6 (11). In contrast, a study using the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study Birth Cohort did not find a strong association between breastfeeding, math, and reading skills after adjustment for parent-child interaction (12). That study concluded that it was the characteristics of the mother, rather than the act of breastfeeding, that were the source of cognitive improvement.

Executive Function encompasses a broad range of behaviors that include planning, organization, impulse control, and goal-seeking (13). Executive function development begins in early childhood and can continue into adulthood (14). Developmental trajectories of executive function can be impacted by socioeconomic disparities and other early childhood adversities, such as preterm birth (15, 16). The few studies that examined the relationship between executive function and breastfeeding have had mixed conclusions. A study using 1,037 mother-child pairs in the Project Viva birth cohort did not find an association between breastfeeding duration and executive function in mid-childhood (median age = 7.7 years) (2). In contrast, a Spanish study using two birth-cohorts with 500 mother-child pairs reported a significant relationship between increased breastfeeding duration and increased executive functioning performance in 4-year-old children (17).

Memory is a broad construct that encompasses separate systems of information encoding, storage, and retrieval (18). Memory is a multifaceted cognitive domain that includes subdomains like declarative memory and episodic memory (19). In children and adolescents, the ability to recollect past events improves throughout the teenage years (20). The literature on breastfeeding and memory is nearly non-existent. The few studies that have reported results have found no associations. A study that examined the relationship between breastfeeding and memory in 6–8 year-old children that had very low birth weight did not detect a strong association (21). A separate assessment of breastfeeding and verbal memory at age 53 also did not find a strong association (22). In addition, the Project Viva birth cohort did not find any association between memory scores at age seven and breastfeeding duration (23).

The aims of the current study were 2-fold. First, we attempt to thoroughly address confounding introduced by the observational study design using a quasi-experimental method. Second, we examined the association between breastfeeding duration and separate components representing cognition. We hypothesized that greater periods of breastfeeding duration would be strongly associated with improved scores for all neurocognitive components in later childhood (ages 9–10). The study was conducted using data from the baseline visit of the ABCD Study® (Data Release 2.0.1).

Materials and Methods

Study Design and Sample

The ABCD Study enrolled 11,875 children aged 9–10 throughout the United States between 2016–2018. Children were enrolled using a stratified, probability sample of schools at 21 currently active study sites and are followed using yearly in-person visits along with semi-annual follow-up calls. The ongoing study will continue until the original cohort of children are at least 19–20 years old (24). The study was designed to approximate the sociodemographic sampling distribution of the American Community Survey (24). In addition, parents/guardians were enrolled alongside children and completed a series of questionnaires related to the youth's early developmental history (25). Parental consent and child assent was obtained from all ABCD participants (24).

For this cross-sectional analysis, we excluded children who did not attend the baseline visit with a biological mother to minimize the impact of measurement error related to breastfeeding duration and other observed covariates (e.g., prenatal alcohol exposure). More than 85% (n = 10,131) of ABCD participants attended the baseline with their biological mother. As a result, all covariates reflect the experience of the participating child or the biological mother (e.g., parental education). Participants missing breastfeeding information (n = 175) or any neurocognitive test (n = 854) were excluded from the analyses. The final analyses exploring the association between breastfeeding duration and neurocognitive performance were conducted using data from 9,116 children. This study follows the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline for cross-sectional studies.

Breastfeeding Measures

Parents at baseline were asked whether the child had ever been breastfed, and for how many months the child had been breastfed. Duration of breastfeeding was originally measured in months and was operationalized as a categorical variable with four levels (0 months, 1–6 months, 7–12 months, and more than 12 months) to examine the relationship between length of breastfeeding and lack of exposure to breastfeeding. Categorical levels of breastfeeding duration were selected to reflect cut-off points that are both commonly reported in the literature and recommended in pediatric guidelines (3, 26). Additional consideration was also made to prevent creating subgroups that had small sample sizes that could increase standard errors and decrease precision (27). Breastfeeding exclusivity was not collected as part of the ABCD assessment.

Neurocognitive Measures

Children completed a neurocognitive battery reflecting many facets of cognition during the baseline ABCD visit (28). The ABCD neurocognitive battery incorporated several measures from the NIH Toolbox®-Cognition battery, the Flanker Test, the Picture Sequence Memory Test, the List Sorting Working Memory Test, the Picture Vocabulary Test, the Oral Reading Recognition Test, the Dimensional Change Card Sort Test, and the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test. In addition, each child at baseline completed the Matrix Reasoning Test from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), the Little Man Task, and the Cash Choice Task (28). The RAVLT is used to assess short-term and long-term retention of information (28). For our analyses, the RAVLT was transformed into three variables, encompassing short-term, long-term, and working memory. The short-term component incorporated the first five trials of the RAVLT, which asked children to recall a list of 15 words immediately after each trial. The sixth trial involved recalling the original 15 words after a “distractor” list of 15 words was presented (28). The long-term memory component included only the seventh trial that was administered after a 30-min delay.

Cognitive Outcomes

The latent structure of the cognitive tests was identified using principal component analysis (PCA) with varimax-rotation. PCA is a dimensionality reduction technique used to group highly correlated variables into fewer components to improve interpretation (29). PCA with varimax-rotation has previously been used with the ABCD baseline data to identify the latent dimensions of the administered neurocognitive tests (30). For our analyses, all neurocognitive tests were standardized with a mean of zero and a variance of one prior to PCA estimation due to the differing scales of the original measures. After the initial factor extraction, a varimax rotation was applied to maximize the shared variance of items within each principal component and improve interpretation of the results (31). An eigenvalue-one criterion was used to determine whether to retain a component (32). Factor loadings with a standardized estimate of 0.5 or higher were considered strong (33). Variables were considered for removal if they had significant cross-loading (e.g., 0.4 loading on more than one factor). Factor scores for each participant were then extracted to reflect that individual's performance across the separate components. The PCA with varimax-rotation was conducted using the psych package in R (34).

Propensity Score Estimation and Diagnostics

The propensity score (PS) is an estimate of the probability of exposure to a treatment conditional on observed covariates (35). The purpose of the PS is to allow for an unbiased estimate of the treatment effect on an outcome when randomization (e.g., assigning breastfeeding exposure) is not feasible (36). For our analyses, PS weights were used to balance the distribution of confounding variables between each level of breastfeeding duration. The decision to use PS weights instead of a regression-based, covariate-adjustment technique (e.g., multinomial logistic regression) was due to multiple factors. First, a large number of variables can be summarized using PS techniques to minimize residual confounding in observational studies (37). Second, there were significant differences in the balance of certain participant characteristics between levels of breastfeeding duration (e.g., race/ethnicity). Relatedly, PS weighting can account for the unequal probability of participating in a study due to the characteristics of the participant (27). Third, PS weights reduce the risk of bias by estimating scores independent of the outcome (i.e., cognitive outcomes were excluded from the estimation of the PS weights).

The PS weights were estimated via generalized boosted modeling (GBM). GBM is a multivariate non-parametric regression technique that iteratively estimates the PS of individuals to maximize balance in observed covariates (38, 39). GBM can also incorporate interactions among a large number of covariates to reduce the risk of model misspecification (39, 40). Previous research has shown that GBM outperforms logistic regression in PS estimation when there is moderate non-additivity and moderate non-linearity in the model (27). GBM was selected for the current analyses to adequately address the risk of biased estimates resulting from model misspecification. In addition, GBM can be extended to treatments with more than two levels to improve covariate balance between different exposure groups (38). The mean outcomes of different treatment groups can then be compared to describe average treatment effects (e.g., the mean outcome had all children in the study been breastfed for 7–12 months compared with the mean outcome had all children in the study been breastfed for 1–6 months). The present analyses used a 30,000- tree GBM with an interaction depth of 4, a shrinkage of 0.001, and a bag fraction of 0.5 to optimize covariate balance and minimize extreme weights. Estimation of the PS for multiple treatments using observed covariates was conducted with the R-package twang (41).

Variables for the PS estimation were selected using a review of established selection criteria (42, 43). Child measures included gender, race/ethnicity, age at baseline, birth weight, child relationship in his/her family, family conflict, school risk and protective factors, weeks born premature, cesarean birth, and whether the child had other complications at birth (e.g., required oxygen, slow heartbeat, cyanosis, number of days in incubator). Maternal measures included age at birth of child, educational attainment and household income at baseline, tobacco and alcohol use during pregnancy, marital status at baseline, whether the pregnancy was planned, prenatal vitamin usage, and pregnancy-related diabetes. In addition, sampling weights provided in the ABCD dataset were included to account for underrepresentation or overrepresentation of certain subgroups (44).

Balance diagnostics for PS weights were included in the analyses according to established best practices (45). The diagnostics focused on two criteria for determining effective balancing: the absolute standardized mean difference (ASMD) of weighted and unweighted variables, and the range of weights to determine the presence of extreme weights. For our analysis we considered any ASMD >0.10 as a sign of covariate imbalance. Anything 0.10 or less has been suggested in the literature as a negligible difference in relative balance (36, 46). A form of doubly robust estimation further adjusted for any covariates that remained imbalanced to improve the accuracy of the estimation and minimize the mean square error (38, 47).

Missing Data

The amount of missing data were <2% for all covariates included in the PS estimation except for days in incubator (3.3%) and household income (8.6%). Inverse probability weighting using GBM is considered an alternative to multiple imputation that is effective and requires fewer assumptions (48). GBM can account for missing data by creating a missing value indicator to avoid discarding of data (i.e., listwise deletion) (38). As a result, the reported estimates only utilize the non-imputed data and did not require the removal of any additional participants due to missing data.

Sensitivity Analyses

Sensitivity analysis was important due to the lack of maternal neurocognitive testing in the ABCD Study. A method for quantifying vulnerability to unmeasured confounding was used that leveraged the strength of observed covariates to estimate the robustness of the research conclusions. Maternal education was selected to bound the strength of an unobserved confounder due to its high correlation with intelligence in the literature (49). The sensitivity analysis was conducted using the Sensemakr package in R (50).

An additional sensitivity analysis was conducted using multiple imputation by chained equations to explore the impact of missing neurocognitive data on the PCA loadings. The multiple imputation was conducted using the mice package in R (51).

Analytic Strategy

Comparison of variable distributions used the mean for continuous variables and percentages for categorical variables. Significance testing used t tests for continuous variables and χ2 tests for categorical variables. After PS estimation, a doubly robust propensity-score adjusted linear regression model was used to estimate the association of breastfeeding duration on separate components. Results of the linear regression are presented as standardized beta coefficients to reflect the standardization of factor scores extracted from the PCA. All tests for statistical significance were two sided and considered statistically significant at P < 0.05. Standard errors and corresponding 95% confidence intervals are reported using bootstrapping with 1,000 replications. The regression was performed using the Survey package in R (52). All statistical analyses were performed using R version 3.6.2. R code for replication can be retrieved at https://git.io/JOIJJ.

Results

Demographic Characteristics of the Sample

The analytic sample included 9,116 children that attended the baseline evaluation with their biological mother and did not have missing breastfeeding information or cognitive test scores. As seen in Table 1, there was an imbalance in the characteristics of children across the different levels of breastfeeding duration. Breastfeeding duration was not significantly related to child's gender, cyanosis, slow heartbeat at birth, and school risk report (all p-values > 0.05). There were significant differences across breastfeeding duration for the remaining covariates (all p-values < 0.05). Of the 1,875 children never breastfed, 36.9% were White, 32.9% were Black and 19.3% were Hispanic. When compared to the other breastfeeding groups, a greater proportion of never breastfed children had unmarried mothers (52.5%), had mothers with a high school diploma or less (33.0%), and came from households with a combined income under $50,000 (43.9%).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the ABCD Sample (n = 9,116).

A separate comparison of children missing neurocognitive data (Table 2) revealed significant differences only for race/ethnicity (p = 0.03), household income (p = 0.02), maternal education (p < 0.01), school risk report (p < 0.01), and mother's marital status (p < 0.01). A sensitivity analysis using multiple imputation of the missing neurocognitive tests did not reveal any changes to the PCA loadings (Supplementary Table 2). Characteristics of the full sample without exclusion for those that did not attend baseline with their biological mother also did not reveal any significant differences in the distribution of important covariates (Supplementary Table 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Characteristics of participants missing a neurocognitive test.

Results of the Principal Component Analysis

Factor loadings (Table 3) supported the extraction of three components from the PCA. The resulting three-component model excluded the Cash Choice Task because it loaded separately into a fourth factor and did not meaningfully improve the variance explained by the PCA. The three-component model was further supported by the scree plot showing eigenvalues above 1 only for the first three components (Supplementary Figure 1). There was no evidence of significant cross-loadings for any of the variables included in the PCA. The three-component model explained 59.2% of the cumulative variance. Total variance around 60% is typically considered satisfactory due to the imprecise nature of data when dealing with human populations (33). General Ability loadings consisted of the List Sorting Working Memory test, the Oral Reading Recognition test, the Picture Vocabulary test, the Matrix Reasoning test, and the Little Man Task. The extracted General Ability scores ranged from −3.75 to 3.68 (median = 0.02). Executive Function loadings consisted of the Flanker test, the Dimensional Change Card Sort test, and the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed test. Executive Function scores ranged from −3.97 to 2.75 (median = 0.06). The Memory component included all three RAVLT measures, along with the Picture Sequence Memory test. Memory scores ranged from −4.14 to 3.36 (median = 0.04).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Principal component loadings with varimax-rotation for three-component model (n = 9,116).

Balance Diagnostics for the Propensity Score Estimation

The PS estimation improved the balance in potential confounding variables for nearly every covariate included in the model (Figure 1). Despite improvements in balancing there was evidence of a modest imbalance for race/ethnicity (ASMD = 0.13) and tobacco use during pregnancy (ASMD = 0.12). These variables were further adjusted for in the final weighted linear regression model to minimize bias.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Comparison of the absolute standardized mean difference (ASMD) between treatment groups (levels of breastfeeding duration) on pretreatment covariates, before and after weighting. The weighting was successful in reducing imbalance across all covariates included in the model except for three that had a slight decrease in balance.

The presence of extreme weights in PS estimation can increase the variance of association estimates, although there is no clear cutoff for what constitutes an extreme weight (53). The distribution of weights in this study (range of 0.10–8.80) suggested a modest amount of imbalance, although the large sample size and the absence of weights equal to 0 or >10 indicate that the weights obtained are not extreme (53). As a result, the reported analysis did not use ad-hoc methods like weight trimming or weight stabilization.

PS estimation requires sufficient “common support” between treatment groups to ensure adequate covariate balance (53). In our analyses, there was some visual evidence of a lack of common support between those breastfed for 0 months and those breastfed for more than 12 months, but the lack of overlap was not extreme (Supplementary Figure 1). In addition, the absence of weights with a value of zero and weights with extreme values suggests that common support was not an issue in achieving covariate balance (53).

Breastfeeding Duration Associated With General Ability

The PS-adjusted linear regression model found a strong association between breastfeeding duration and performance on General Ability scores (Table 4). The comparison was greatest when comparing those breastfed for more than 12 months to those never breastfed (β = 0.328; 95% CI, 0.238–0.418; p = 1.68 × 10−12). The estimate was reduced after additional adjustment of covariates that remained imbalanced (β = 0.301; 95% CI, 0.217–0.385; p = 4.04 × 10−12). The results suggest that being breastfed for more than 12 months is associated with an increase of about 0.30 standard deviations (SD) in General Ability scores. The association was also strong for those breastfed 7–12 months (β = 0.224; 95% CI, 0.147–0.301; p = 1.57 × 10−8) and 1–6 months (β = 0.109; 95% CI, 0.039–0.179; p = 0.002) relative to those not breastfed.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Association between breastfeeding duration and domain-specific neurocognitive performance (n = 9,116).

Breastfeeding Duration Is Not Associated With Executive Function

There was no indication that breastfeeding duration had an association with Executive Function (Table 4). Executive Function performance was lowest in those breastfed for more than 12 months, but the strength of the association was small and not statistically significant (β = −0.017; 95% CI, −0.101 to 0.066; p = 0.684). Executive Function scores were also lower for every level of breastfeeding duration relative to those not breastfed, although the estimated associations were consistently insignificant and likely not meaningful.

Breastfeeding Duration Is Not Associated With Memory

There was also no indication that breastfeeding had any meaningful impact on Memory scores. Like Executive Function, Memory scores were lower for every level of breastfeeding duration relative to those not breastfed, but the associations at each level were small and not significant.

Results of Sensitivity Analyses

Sensitivity analysis was conducted to gauge the robustness of the General Ability regression estimates (Supplementary Table 1). The analysis used a robustness value (RV) to summarize the resilience of the point estimates taking into account the partial R-squared of the treatment with the outcome (54). The RV was greatest in those breastfed more than 12 months (10%), indicating that any unmeasured confounder that cannot explain at least 10% of the residual variance in both the treatment and the outcome is insufficient to meaningfully change our conclusion (50). The RV is diminished but is still strong for 7–12 months (7.4%) and 1–6 months (5.1%). Finally, the contour plots (Supplementary Figure 2) show that a confounder with more than three times the strength of maternal education would be required to fully explain away our results.

An additional sensitivity analysis was conducted to examine the associations without a propensity model. A generalized linear model (GLM) was specified using the full cohort sample (n = 11,875) and adjustment for the same variables included in the PS model. It should be noted that multiple imputation was not used to account for missing data. As a result, the final sample size was 9,681 when using GLM. The results of the analysis (Supplementary Table 4) were almost identical to those found using PS weighting. Estimates were slightly attenuated for General Ability when using a GLM, although the differences are likely trivial. The associations for Executive Function and Memory remained small and not significant in the GLM results.

Discussion

A goal of this study was to analyze the association between breastfeeding duration and cognitive performance in 9–10-year-old children. Compared to those never breastfed, General Ability performance was highest in those breastfed more than 12 months after weighting and adjustment for any remaining imbalanced covariates. The association of breastfeeding for more than 12 months with General Ability scores was about three-tenths of a SD, and about two-tenths of a SD when comparing children breastfed for seven to 12 months to those not breastfed. The estimate was reduced to about one-tenth of a SD in children breastfed for only 1–6 months. There was no statistically significant association between breastfeeding duration and Executive Function or Memory scores. Note that this improvement in General Ability persists and is being assessed up to a decade after cessation of breastfeeding. Future analyses using the ABCD dataset can illuminate whether this association remains or is weakened as children enter adolescence.

The strengths of this study include a large, diverse study sample with measurement of many potential confounders and predictors of both breastfeeding initiation and cognitive performance. The use of inverse probability weights to achieve a balance in observed covariates further decreases the chance that residual confounding influenced our results. The results of the sensitivity analysis demonstrated the robustness of the conclusions to unmeasured confounding and described the potential impact on biasing effect estimates.

There are potential limitations in our study. First, whether the effect of an unmeasured confounder like maternal IQ is plausibly more than twice as strong as maternal education cannot be answered using the ABCD dataset. In an analysis using the Project Viva cohort, the association estimates of the model without maternal IQ decreased by about 17% after adjustment for maternal IQ (from β = 0.35 to β = 0.29, respectively) (23). In addition, a meta-analysis assessing 18 studies found that adjustment for maternal IQ did not change the overall conclusion that breastfeeding was associated with higher performance in IQ tests (4). Second, by excluding those with missing neurocognitive data we may have biased our effect estimates, although results from a sensitivity analysis using multiple imputation on missing neurocognitive tests did not reveal any evidence of bias on our outcome measures. Third, while breastfeeding recall has been found to be reliable (55), it is important to keep in mind that ABCD parents were asked to recall information that concluded for some nearly a decade earlier. A separate study in Norway found that the majority of women, even after 20 years, were able to accurately recall breastfeeding duration within 1 month (56). In addition, any misclassification in maternal recall of breastfeeding duration is non-differential with respect to cognitive performance (i.e., exposure error is independent of the outcome) and is likely to bias the effect estimate toward the null.

Breast milk is widely recognized as an important contributor to healthy brain development. Previous research on nutrients in breast milk and postnatal cognitive development has focused on the role of arachidonic acid (ARA) and docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) (57). DHA is a long-chain omega-3 fatty acid that is produced by the mother and transferred to the fetus during the third trimester of pregnancy (57). After birth, breast milk is the primary source of DHA for infants, and DHA concentrations in breast milk reflect the mother's nutritional intake (58). DHA is directly implicated in the myelination of brain frontal lobes throughout childhood and adolescence (59). Disruptions in myelination trajectories due to poor nutrition (e.g., DHA deficiency) have previously been associated with poor cognitive outcomes (60). The reasons for a differential impact of breastfeeding on cognitive domains are not entirely clear. An analysis of maternal milk DHA values and performance on math scores in 28 countries reported a strong association (β = 0.462, p = 0.006) after adjustment for socio-economic influences and macronutrients (61). The researchers concluded that maternal milk DHA accounted for more variance in math scores than socio-economic factors. A separate study in Ghana found a positive but insignificant association between DHA and executive function in a sample of 307 2–6-year-old children (β = 0.25, p = 0.06) (62). A review of 15 studies evaluating DHA supplementation in children and cognitive outcomes did not find a consistent association between intake and memory scores (59). The researchers concluded that increases in DHA generally resulted in improved learning and behavioral outcomes in children (59).

Current policy recommends women breastfeed children through at least age 1 or as long as mutually desired (63). The results of our analysis support this recommendation to breastfeed for at least 1 year or longer to achieve full cognitive benefits, although whether the improvement on cognitive performance has any practical importance in the real world is not clear (64). One analysis using three separate cohorts in the United States found a significant economic gain in children who were breastfed in terms of income gained as an adult (65). Finally, from a public health perspective, small effects can have a large impact when the exposure is common (66). Breastfeeding is a common exposure that is not considered harmful, with numerous benefits for both mother and infant. In that sense, the recommendation of breastfeeding for longer durations can make a large impact on population health.

In conclusion, the current study identified a strong association between breastfeeding duration and General Ability scores, with the greatest effect found in those breastfed for more than 12 months. Sensitivity analysis showed that results were robust to unmeasured confounding, although the effect is substantially diminished under certain circumstances. We did not detect an association between breastfeeding duration and either Executive Function or Memory scores. Our findings suggest that the benefits of breastfeeding on General Ability are evident in offspring with only a few months of exposure and appear to increase past 12 months of exposure. The effect sizes are more meaningful when viewed from a population health perspective to measure the potential positive impact to society.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: The ABCD Study data are openly available to qualified researchers for free. Access can be requested at https://nda.nih.gov/abcd/request-access. Code for replication of the analyses conducted in this article can be retrieved at https://git.io/JOIJJ.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Office for Human Subject Protection, University of Rochester Medical Center. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

DL conceived of the presented idea. DL and YM performed the computations. WT, YM, and HM verified the analytical methods. JF and EF encouraged DL and supervised the findings of this work. All authors discussed the results and contributed to the final manuscript.

Funding

Ongoing work of our research group on the ABCD project was supported by a National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA) grant to JJF and EGF (U01DA050988). The ABCD Study was a registered trademark of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Participant recruitment, phenotyping and neuroimaging at the University of Rochester (UR) was conducted through cores of the UR Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center (UR-IDDRC), which was supported by a center grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (P50 HD103536–to JF).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Thank you to Elaine Smolock for proofreading the article—University of Rochester Medical Center.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2021.657422/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

ABCD, Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development; PCA, Principal Component Analysis; GBM, Generalized Boosted Model; ASMD, Absolute Standardized Mean Difference; RV, Robustness Value; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; CI, Confidence Interval; GLM, Generalized Linear Model.

References

1. León-Cava N, Lutter C, Ross J, Martin L. Quantifying the Benefits of Breastfeeding: A Summary of the Evidence. Washington DC: Pan American Health Organization (2002). p. 3.

Google Scholar

2. Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Kleinman KP, Bellinger DC, Harris MH, Taveras EM, et al. Infant breastfeeding duration and mid-childhood executive function, behavior, and social-emotional development. JJDBP. (2016) 37:43. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Walfisch A, Sermer C, Cressman A, Koren G. Breast milk and cognitive development—the role of confounders: a systematic review. BMJ Open. (2013) 3:e003259. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-003259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Horta BL, Loret De Mola C, Victora CG. Breastfeeding and intelligence: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Paediatrica. (2015) 104:14–9. doi: 10.1111/apa.13139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Colen CG, Ramey DM. Is breast truly best? Estimating the effects of breastfeeding on long-term child health and wellbeing in the United States using sibling comparisons. Soc Sci Med. (2014) 109:55–65. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.01.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Anstey EH, Chen J, Elam-Evans LD, Perrine CG. Racial and geographic differences in breastfeeding—United States, 2011–2015. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. (2017) 66:723. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6627a3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Girard LC, Doyle O, Tremblay RE. Breastfeeding, cognitive and noncognitive development in early childhood: a population study. Pediatrics. (2017) 139:e20161848. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Gottfredson LS. The General Intelligence Factor. New York, NY: Scientific American, Incorporated (1998).

Google Scholar

9. Quigley MA, Hockley C, Carson C, Kelly Y, Renfrew MJ, Sacker A. Breastfeeding is associated with improved child cognitive development: a population-based cohort study. J Pediatr. (2012) 160:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2011.06.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Brion M-JA, Lawlor DA, Matijasevich A, Horta B, Anselmi L, Araújo CL, et al. What are the causal effects of breastfeeding on IQ, obesity and blood pressure? Evidence from comparing high-income with middle-income cohorts. Int J Epidemiol. (2011) 40:670–80. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kramer MS, Aboud F, Mironova E, Vanilovich I, Platt RW, Matush L, et al. Breastfeeding and child cognitive development: new evidence from a large randomized trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2008) 65:578–84. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Gibbs BG, Forste R. Breastfeeding, parenting, and early cognitive development. J Pediatr. (2014) 164:487–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Welsh MC, Pennington BF, Groisser DB. A normative-developmental study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children. Dev Neuropsychol. (1991) 7:131–49. doi: 10.1080/87565649109540483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Best JR, Miller PH. A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. (2010) 81:1641–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Berthelsen D, Hayes N, White SLJ, Williams KE. Executive function in adolescence: associations with child and family risk factors and self-regulation in early childhood. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:903. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Burnett AC, Scratch SE, Anderson PJ. Executive function outcome in preterm adolescents. Early Hum Dev. (2013) 89:215–20. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Julvez J, Ribas-Fitó N, Forns M, Garcia-Esteban R, Torrent M, Sunyer J. Attention behaviour and hyperactivity at age 4 and duration of breast-feeding. Acta Paediatr. (2007) 96:842–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00273.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Bauer PJ, Dikmen SS, Heaton RK, Mungas D, Slotkin J, Beaumont JL. III. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring episodic memory. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. (2013) 78:34–48. doi: 10.1111/mono.12033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Harvey PD. Domains of cognition and their assessment Dialogues. Clin Neurosci. (2019) 21:227–37. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2019.21.3/pharvey

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Blankenship TL, O'Neill M, Ross A, Bell MA. Working memory and recollection contribute to academic achievement. Learn Individ Differ. (2015) 43:164–9. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Smith MM. Influence of breastfeeding on cognitive outcomes at age 6-8 years: follow-up of very low birth weight infants. Am J Epidemiol. (2003) 158:1075–82. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwg257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Richards M, Hardy R, Wadsworth ME. Long-term effects of breast-feeding in a national birth cohort: educational attainment and midlife cognitive function. Public Health Nutr. (2002) 5:631–5. doi: 10.1079/PHN2002338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Kleinman KP, Guthrie LB, Bellinger DC, Taveras EM, et al. Infant feeding and childhood cognition at ages 3 and 7 years. JAMA Pediatrics. (2013) 167:836. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Garavan H, Bartsch H, Conway K, Decastro A, Goldstein RZ, Heeringa S, et al. Recruiting the ABCD sample: design considerations and procedures. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2018) 32:16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Barch DM, Albaugh MD, Avenevoli S, Chang L, Clark DB, Glantz MD, et al. Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: rationale and description. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2018) 32:55–66. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Breastfeeding and the use of human milk. Pediatrics. (2012) 129:e827–41. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-3552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Lee BK, Lessler J, Stuart EA. Improving propensity score weighting using machine learning. Stat Med. (2010) 29:337–46. doi: 10.1002/sim.3782

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Luciana M, Bjork JM, Nagel BJ, Barch DM, Gonzalez R, Nixon SJ, et al. Adolescent neurocognitive development and impacts of substance use: overview of the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) baseline neurocognition battery. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2018) 32:67–79. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS, Ullman JB. Using Multivariate Statistics. Vol 5. Pearson Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc. (2007).

Google Scholar

30. Thompson WK, Barch DM, Bjork JM, Gonzalez R, Nagel BJ, Nixon SJ, et al. The structure of cognition in 9 and 10 year-old children and associations with problem behaviors: findings from the ABCD study's baseline neurocognitive battery. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2019) 36:100606. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Abdi H. Factor Rotations in Factor Analyses. Thousand Oaks, CA: Encyclopedia for Research Methods for the Social Sciences Sage (2003). p. 792–5.

Google Scholar

32. O'Rourke N, Hatcher L. A Step-By-Step Approach to Using SAS for Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling. Cary, NC: Sas Institute (2013).

Google Scholar

33. Hair JF. Multivariate Data Analysis. London: Cengage, (2009).

Google Scholar

34. Revelle W. How To: Use the Psych Package for Factor Analysis and Data Reduction. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University, Department of Psychology (2016).

Google Scholar

35. Rosenbaum PR, Rubin DB. The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika. (1983) 70:41–55. doi: 10.1093/biomet/70.1.41

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Austin PC. An introduction to propensity score methods for reducing the effects of confounding in observational studies. Multivariate Behav Res. (2011) 46:399–424. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2011.568786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Lash TL, VanderWeele TJ, Haneause S, Rothman K. Modern Epidemiology. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins (2020).

Google Scholar

38. McCaffrey DF, Griffin BA, Almirall D, Slaughter ME, Ramchand R, Burgette LF. A tutorial on propensity score estimation for multiple treatments using generalized boosted models. Stat Med. (2013) 32:3388–414. doi: 10.1002/sim.5753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. McCaffrey DF, Ridgeway G, Morral AR. Propensity score estimation with boosted regression for evaluating causal effects in observational studies. Psychol Methods. (2004) 9:403. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.9.4.403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Parast L, McCaffrey DF, Burgette LF, de la Guardia FH, Golinelli D, Miles JNV, et al. Optimizing variance-bias trade-off in the TWANG package for estimation of propensity scores. Health Serv Outcomes Res Methodol. (2017) 17:175–97. doi: 10.1007/s10742-016-0168-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Burgette L, Griffin BA, McCaffrey D. Propensity scores for multiple treatments: a tutorial for the mnps function in the twang package. R Package Rand Corporation. (2017). Available online at: https://www.rand.org/pubs/tools/TL169z1.html. doi: 10.7249/TL136.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Brookhart MA, Schneeweiss S, Rothman KJ, Glynn RJ, Avorn J, Stürmer T. Variable selection for propensity score models. Am J Epidemiol. (2006) 163:1149–56. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj149

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. PEARL J. (2010). On a class of bias-amplifying covariates that endanger effect estimates. in Proceedings of the 26th Conference on Uncertainty in Artificial Intelligence, p. 417–24.

Google Scholar

44. Heeringa SG, Berglund PA. A guide for population-based analysis of the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study baseline data. BioRxiv [Preprint]. (2020). doi: 10.1101/2020.02.10.942011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Austin PC, Stuart EA. Moving towards best practice when using inverse probability of treatment weighting (IPTW) using the propensity score to estimate causal treatment effects in observational studies. Stat Med. (2015) 34:3661–79. doi: 10.1002/sim.6607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Nguyen T-L, Collins GS, Spence J, Daurès JP, Devereaux PJ, Landais P, et al. Double-adjustment in propensity score matching analysis: choosing a threshold for considering residual imbalance. BMC Med Res Methodol. (2017) 17. doi: 10.1186/s12874-017-0338-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Hullsiek KH, Louis TA. Propensity score modeling strategies for the causal analysis of observational data. Biostatistics. (2002) 3:179–93. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/3.2.179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Seaman SR, White IR. Review of inverse probability weighting for dealing with missing data. Stat Methods Med Res. (2013) 22:278–95. doi: 10.1177/0962280210395740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Deary IJ, Johnson W. Intelligence and education: causal perceptions drive analytic processes and therefore conclusions. Int J Epidemiol. (2010) 39:1362–9. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Cinelli C, Ferwerda J, Hazlett C. sensemakr: Sensitivity Analysis Tools for OLS in R and Stata. J Stat Softw. (2020). doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3588978

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Buuren Sv, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J Stat Softw. (2010) 45:1–68. doi: 10.18637/jss.v045.i03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Lumley T. Analysis of complex survey samples. J Stat Softw. (2004) 9:1–19. doi: 10.18637/jss.v009.i08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Leite W. Practical Propensity Score Methods Using R. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications (2016). doi: 10.4135/9781071802854

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Cinelli C, Hazlett C. Making sense of sensitivity: extending omitted variable bias. J R Stat Soc [Ser B]. (2020) 82:39–67. doi: 10.1111/rssb.12348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Li R, Scanlon KS, Serdula MK. The validity and reliability of maternal recall of breastfeeding practice. Nutr Rev. (2005) 63:103–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2005.tb00128.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Natland ST, Andersen LF, Nilsen TIL, Forsmo S, Jacobsen GW. Maternal recall of breastfeeding duration twenty years after delivery. BMC Med Res Methodol. (2012) 12:179. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-12-179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Martin C, Ling P-R, Blackburn G. Review of infant feeding: key features of breast milk and infant formula. Nutrients. (2016) 8:279. doi: 10.3390/nu8050279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Innis SM. Polyunsaturated fatty acids in human milk. Prot Infants Through Human Milk. 2004:27–43. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4757-4242-8_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Kuratko C, Barrett E, Nelson E, Salem N. The Relationship of Docosahexaenoic Acid (DHA) with learning and behavior in healthy children: a review. Nutrients. (2013) 5:2777–810. doi: 10.3390/nu5072777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Deoni S, Dean D, Joelson S, O'Regan J, Schneider N. Early nutrition influences developmental myelination and cognition in infants and young children. NeuroImage. (2018) 178:649–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Lassek WD, Gaulin SJC. Maternal milk DHA content predicts cognitive performance in a sample of 28 nations. Mater Child Nutr. (2015) 11:773–9. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Adjepong M, Yakah W, Harris WS, Annan RA, Pontifex MB, Fenton JI. Whole blood n-3 fatty acids are associated with executive function in 2–6-year-old Northern Ghanaian children. J Nutr Biochem. (2018) 57:287–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2018.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Gartner LM, Morton J, Lawrence RA, Naylor AJ, O'Hare D, Schanler RJ, et al. Breastfeeding and the use of human milk. Pediatrics. (2005) 115:496–506. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-2491

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Jiang M, Foster EM, Gibson-Davis CM. Breastfeeding and the child cognitive outcomes: a propensity score matching approach. (2011) 15:1296–307. doi: 10.1007/s10995-010-0677-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Rothstein DS. Breastfeeding and children's early cognitive outcomes. Rev Econ Stat. (2013) 95:919–31. doi: 10.1162/REST_a_00282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Rose G. Strategy of prevention: lessons from cardiovascular disease. BMJ. (1981) 282:1847–51. doi: 10.1136/bmj.282.6279.1847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: child, breastfeeding, neurocognition, cognitive development, public health

Citation: Lopez DA, Foxe JJ, Mao Y, Thompson WK, Martin HJ and Freedman EG (2021) Breastfeeding Duration Is Associated With Domain-Specific Improvements in Cognitive Performance in 9–10-Year-Old Children. Front. Public Health 9:657422. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.657422

Received: 25 January 2021; Accepted: 29 March 2021;
Published: 26 April 2021.

Edited by:

Udo Rolle, University Hospital Frankfurt, Germany

Reviewed by:

Suman Kundu, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, United States
Satinder Aneja, Sharda University, India
Rolf Lambert Schlößer, University Hospital Frankfurt, Germany

Copyright © 2021 Lopez, Foxe, Mao, Thompson, Martin and Freedman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Edward G. Freedman, ed_freedman@urmc.rochester.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.