Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Public Health, 25 November 2021
Sec. Environmental health and Exposome

Impact of Exposure to Natural and Built Environments on Positive and Negative Affect: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

\nWenfei Yao,
Wenfei Yao1,2*Fei Chen,
Fei Chen1,2*San WangSan Wang1Xiaofeng ZhangXiaofeng Zhang1
  • 1Department of Landscape Architecture, College of Architecture and Urban Planning, Qingdao University of Technology, Qingdao, China
  • 2Innovation Institute for Sustainable Maritime Architecture Research and Technology (iSMART), Qingdao University of Technology, Qingdao, China

There is increasing evidence that the natural environment provides substantial benefits to human emotional well-being. The current study synthesized this body of research using the meta-analysis and assessed the positive and negative effects of exposure to both the natural and built environments. We searched four databases and 20 studies were included in the review. The meta-analysis results showed the most convincing evidence that exposure to the natural environment could increase positive affect (standardized mean difference, SMD = 0.61, 95% CI 0.41, 0.81) and decreased negative affect (SMD = −0.47, 95% CI −0.71, −0.24). However, there was extreme heterogeneity between the studies, and the risk of bias was high. According to the subgroup analysis, study region, study design, mean age of the sample, sample size, and type of natural and built environment were found to be important factors during exposure to the natural environment. The implications of these findings for the existing theory and research are discussed. These findings will help convince the health professionals and policymakers to encourage the residents to increase their time spent in the natural environment. These findings of this systematic review also suggested that the creation, maintenance, and enhancement of accessible greenspaces or existing natural environments may form part of a multidimensional approach to increasing emotional well-being of the local populations.

Introduction

A considerable number of theoretical and empirical studies support the notion that natural exposure contributes to both physiological and psychological health (15). Exposure to the natural environment is associated with psychological health, such as reducing negative emotions and fatigue, increasing energy, improving attention, and increasing satisfaction and enjoyment (6). Especially with the quick development of urbanization, there is an increasing recognition that the natural environment is a potential buffer for poor mental health (7, 8).

Several existing theories demonstrate that the natural environment is beneficial to the mental health of the population. Two existing theories focus on the effects of the natural environment on human health. The stress reduction theory (9) proposed that the presence of nature brings with it an evolutionary response to safety and survival which produces positive emotions (10). In support of this theory, the empirical studies indicated lower physiological arousal, less negative affect, and higher positive affect in the participants exposed to the natural environment when compared with those exposed to the built environment (1114). An alternative theory that concerns the effect of nature on well-being is the attention recovery theory (ART) (15). According to ART, fast-paced urban living leads to cognitive fatigue, which may manifest as difficulty concentrating, the higher levels of irritability, and negative affect. The natural environment provides a “soft fascination” that allows a person to pay attention effortlessly. Numerous studies have provided empirical support for the prediction of ART, with the subjects indicating better cognitive functioning and more positive emotions following exposure to the natural environment (1619).

As the above literature indicates, ample empirical evidence suggests that exposure to the natural environment is associated with increased emotional well-being. However, few studies have provided a quantitative synthesis of effect of the natural environment on the positive and negative emotions, especially for comparison with built environments. The hypotheses predict that if the individuals are stressed, an encounter with most unthreatening natural environments will have a stress reduction or restorative influence, whereas many built environments will hamper recuperation (9). Generally, natural landscapes have a stronger positive health effect than the urban landscapes. The studies have found that the effects of urban landscapes on health are less positive and, in some cases, even negative (20). Our primary objective in this systematic review and meta-analysis was to quantify the effect of natural and built environments on one facet of psychological well-being, namely, emotional well-being. Positive and negative affect are the two key components of this (21), as their outcome gives the researchers and therapists information concerning the mental state of the participants and patients. We examined the positive and negative effects of exposure to natural and built environments to compare the strength of the influence of both the environments. The second objective of the current study was to identify any potential moderators (such as, mean age of the sample, study region, study design, and sample size) that impact the effect of exposure to the natural environment on emotional well-being. The third objective was to evaluate the quality of the available evidence and thus to provide comprehensive evidence regarding the effect of the natural environment on positive and negative affect, discovering and addressing the limitations of existing theory and research, which may provide an opportunity for fruitful future work in this area.

Methods

Search Strategy

The protocol was registered using PROSPERO (22). We conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines as shown in Supplementary Table 1 (23, 24). A specific research question was formulated according to the “Participants,” “Exposure,” “Comparator,” and “Outcomes” (PECO) framework. The focus of the present systematic review and meta-analysis was: “Is exposure to the natural environment associated with positive and negative affect?” We searched the electronic databases, such as Web of Science, PubMed, ScienceDirect, and PsychINFO. Our search strategies were based on the combinations of natural environment terms (“natural environment,” “urban green environments,” “urban park,” “urban forest,” and “therapeutic landscape”) and positive and negative emotion terms (“positive affect,” “positive emotion,” “negative emotion,” “negative affect,” and “well-being”). The detailed search strategy is shown in Supplementary Table 2. The reference sections of the obtained papers were examined for the additional studies, and a descendancy search was conducted for the studies that cited the obtained papers.

Selection Criteria

The following aprior eligibility criteria were based on the PECO framework:

Participants: any human adult population

Exposure: exposure to the natural environment

Comparator: exposure to the built environment

Outcomes: positive and negative affect.

In terms of the populations considered, any adult population was eligible, regardless of the physical or mental health conditions. The studies involving children were excluded from the review (2). Exposure was defined as the placement of participants in direct physical or sensory contact with a real exposure environment, within the context of a randomized or non-randomized trial. The duration of exposure was not limited. The representations of an environment using virtual reality, photography, or video were not included (2529). The environments were deemed as “natural” if they were defined by a high level of greenness and had not been extensively transformed by human activity. In contrast, a built environment was defined as a predominantly man-made environment with a low level of greenness or an indoor environment (10, 14). The studies in which the participants were engaged in more than two environments were eligible, however, only data from the natural and built environments were included in the meta-analysis. The studies had to include a comparison group (natural vs. built environment) and a self-report assessment of current emotional state that was administered following exposure to the natural and built conditions. The studies that did not include an emotion assessment and examined only cognitive and/or psychophysiological responses to nature were excluded.

The reviewers (XZ) initially screened titles and abstracts to remove obviously irrelevant articles, and then the two reviewers (WY and CF) independently screened all the full texts for eligibility. The discrepancies were resolved through discussion.

Data Extraction

Two reviewers (WY and XZ) independently extracted the following information from each included study: the names of authors, publication year, location, study design, sample, environment type, exposure procedure, and effect measures. Data were extracted into a coding frame using Microsoft Excel, synthesized, and tabulated. We used the risk of bias tool employed by Twohig-Bennett and Jones (30) and Ogilvie et al. (31) (Supplementary Table 3) which was adapted for this purpose.

Narrative Summary and Meta-Analysis

A narrative summary was compiled after a critical review of each study. All the studies were considered to be included in the meta-analysis. Mean, SD, and cell count (N) for all positive and negative affect the outcomes in each included study were extracted. In the first instance, data were extracted directly from the studies.

The studies that included the participants exposed to the natural environment only were excluded from the review (1, 3234). Two studies had multiple natural environments: Tyrväinen et al. (35) investigated the psychological and physiological effects of short-term visits to city centers, urban parks, and woodlands; Janeczko et al. (36) considered both the urban environments (apartment and green suburbs) and two forests (coniferous and deciduous). For the cases of multiple exposures to the same environment, data were extracted immediately before the first exposure, and immediately after the final exposure.

Only data from the time points closest to the start and end times of the exposure were extracted, follow-up measures, or measurements taken during exposure, were not included in the analysis. The results were presented as the forest plots with 95% CIs. The I2-statistic was calculated to quantify the degree of heterogeneity between the studies (I2 = 0–25% represents no heterogeneity; I2 = 25–50% represents moderate heterogeneity; I2 = 50–75% represents large heterogeneity; and I2 = 75–100% represents extreme heterogeneity) (37). A random-effects model was employed for all the meta-analyses as it is considered to represent a more conservative approach, suitable for the cases of high heterogeneity (38). A sensitivity analysis was then performed. Additionally, a univariate meta-regression and a Galbraith radial plot were performed to explore the source of heterogeneity (38, 39).

Publication bias across the studies within the meta-analysis was first tested with the funnel plots using standardized mean difference (SMD) as the measure of study size on the vertical axis and mean difference on the horizontal axis. A symmetrical, inverted funnel indicates the absence of bias. In addition, a funnel plot asymmetry was tested using Begg's and Egger's tests (40), which examined the association between the effect estimates and their variances.

Quality of evidence was assessed using the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development, and Evaluation (GRADE) Working Group guideline (41, 42). The risk of bias, inconsistency, indirectness, imprecision, and publication bias are considered. The GRADE system classified the quality of evidence in one of four grades finally as “high,” “moderate,” “low,” and “very low.”

Subgroup Analyses

The subgroup analyses were conducted based on the study characteristics (study region and study design), participant characteristics (mean age, sample size, and gender), and process of exposure (type of natural environment, type of built environment, and active category). Missingness was very limited. Two studies did not show the gender of the participants (35, 36). To accommodate the different age group categories across the included studies, we stratified pooled estimates for different age groups as those aged <45 and ≥45 years. The sample size was also grouped by the median (42).

All the statistical analyses were performed using the “metan” command in the STATA package version 15.1 software program, and a p-value <0.05 was defined as statistically significant.

Results

The initial database search yielded 17,620 articles, of which 6,237 were removed as repeats, and 11,266 were removed as clearly irrelevant after reviewing the titles and/or abstracts. We retrieved the full text of the 83 relevant papers. A further three studies were retrieved from the reference lists of the review articles. After independent assessment by the reviewer (WY), 20 articles met the inclusion criteria and were eligible for inclusion in the synthesis. A review flowchart is shown in Figure 1. The characteristics and synthesized results for all the 20 studies are presented in Table 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Study characteristics.

Study Characteristics

Although there was no date restriction on the search, 90% of the studies were published in the past 10 years. The studies were conducted in nine different countries. Most were conducted in North America (n = 10), followed by Europe (n = 8), and Asia (n = 2).

Participants

The populations under investigation varied greatly in size, ranging from 14 (47) to 180 (48). Overall, nine studies included fewer than 50 subjects. The participants were typically young, with 13 studies recruiting the college or university students. A significant difference was found in the sample populations, which included the university students, healthy adults (17, 18), full-time regularly employed people (35, 47), and subjects with specific mental or physical illnesses, such as major depressive disorder (16), and coronary artery disease (49).

Outcome Measures

We recorded the instruments used to assess current emotional state, which included the Positive and Negative Affective Schedule (PANAS, n = 18), the Zuckerman Inventory of Personal Reactions (ZIPERS, n = 1), and the Physical Activity Affective Scale (PAAS, n = 1). All the 20 studies assessed the positive affect changes and 18 studies assessed the negative affect changes after exposure to the natural environment, two studies reported on positive affect only (47, 50).

Intervention

The experimental time ranged from 5 (52) to 55 min (16) within 1 day. Two studies had a considerably longer exposure time whereby they completed their experiment in 7 days (49) and 9 weeks (18). For the exposure procedure, 10 studies had participants walking in the environment for a period of time, and the three studies (43, 44, 52) asked participants to complete a viewing. Six studies had multiple exposure (walking and/or viewing) methods (35, 46, 48, 5557). One study (47) had participants exercise (25 min biking + 20 min strength session) in a green/natural area or in an indoor exercise setting.

Most of the studies used forests (n = 7) or urban parks (n = 6) as their natural environment. Four studies used the natural environments characterized by their biodiversity, which included the arboretum (16, 18), wetland (55), and natural preserves (51). Two studies used the university campuses (48, 53). One study used multiple natural environments (woodland and urban parks) (35). For a comparative built environment, most of the studies described an urbanized location on a city street (n = 8) and building site (n = 7). Three studies used the indoor environments (hallways, blank white walls, and gym-halls). One used a parking lot (51) and one used an urban bench outside the center (55).

Study Quality

All the 20 studies were assessed for quality using an adapted version of the Twohig-Bennett and Jones (30) and Ogilvie et al. (31) risk of bias tool. The quality assessments were initially done by the first reviewer (WY) and then all the studies were cross-checked by another (FC) for discrepancies.

For the 20 studies detailed in Supplementary Table 4, the scores ranged from 6 to 10 out of a total of 10 criteria. One study scored 10 (16), and 80% of the studies scored ≥8. Three studies scored 7 (45, 50, 53) and one scored 6 (48). The two criteria that were the most recurrently missing from the studies were “4. Exposure: Did the authors show that participants did not receive concurrent intervention which could have influenced the results (no explanation scores zero)?” and “5. Representativeness: Were the study samples shown to be representative of the study population?

Meta-Analysis

The results of the 20 studies and the meta-analysis estimates are shown in Figures 2, 3. Most of the studies reported that the natural environment was associated with a significant increase in positive affect (SMD = 0.61, 95% CI 0.41, 0.81, p < 0.001) and decreased negative affect (SMD = −0.47, 95% CI −0.71, −0.24, p < 0.001). The observed positive affect sizes ranged from −0.17 to 1.81, and the negative affect sizes ranged from −2.41 to 0.15. The I2 scores for both the positive and negative affect meta-analyses were higher than 75%, indicating extreme heterogeneity between the studies. To test whether significant the meta-analysis results were due to the inclusion of poor-quality studies, a sensitivity analysis was conducted where possible. The sensitivity analyses excluding single studies did not materially change the pooled results. Supplementary Figures 1, 2 show the results from the sensitivity analysis.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Meta-analysis results for the association between natural and built environment exposure on positive affect.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Meta-analysis results for the association between natural and built environment exposure on negative affect.

A univariate meta-regression was performed to explore the source of heterogeneity for the meta-analysis with ≥10 studies included (38). The variables included study region, study design, sample size, mean age, percentage of females, total exposure time, type of natural environment, type of built environment, and active category (Supplementary Tables 5, 6). The results showed that a study design was associated with positive affect differences. The study design contributed to the heterogeneity with tau2 reduced from 0.1534 to 0.0940. The mean age (coefficient = −0.0246, p = 0.040) and sample size (coefficient = 0.0072, p = 0.011) were significantly associated with negative affect. The other variables did not reach significance. The results from the Galbraith plot (Supplementary Figures 3, 4) indicated that the studies with the highest positive affect (46) and lowest positive affect (55) may have been the main cause of the high heterogeneity. After excluding those two studies, the adjusted positive affect was 0.54 (95% CI 0.37–0.70, I2 = 63.9%, p < 0.001), and approximately 14.5% of the heterogeneity was attributed to the study of Browning et al. (46). The negative affect was −0.36 (95% CI −0.54 to −0.17, I2 = 73.5%, p < 0.001), and 10.5% of the heterogeneity was attributed to the study by Reeves et al. (55). Additionally, our confidence in the cumulative evidence was “low” for natural exposure to positive affect, and was “very low” for negative affect according to GRADE system (Supplementary Table 7).

Using the funnel plots (Supplementary Figures 5, 6), the studies of the positive affect were identified as visually symmetrical, both Egger's (PPOS = 0.217) and Begg's (PPOS = 0.206) tests of funnel asymmetry were non-significant, suggesting that the effects of publication bias on positive affect were negligible. However, both Egger's and Begg's (PNEG = 0.019) tests indicated the presence of publication bias on negative affect. The trim and fill analysis suggested that the three studies were missing from our dataset (Figure 4). After adding the missing data to the original dataset, the reported significant effects of the natural environment on the negative affect were intact (SMD = −0.62, 95% CI −0.90, −0.34, p < 0.001), suggesting that the effects of publication bias on the negative affect were negligible.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Filled funnel plot to access potential publication bias of the negative affect studies (n = 18). The circle and square with circle inside represent observed data (18 comparisons) and data added (three studies) by the trim-and-fill analysis.

Subgroup Analyses

The subgroup analyses of the study characteristics, the participant characteristics and process of exposure identified the effect of the natural environment on positive and negative affect. A summary of the subgroup analysis is given in Tables 2, 3. As shown in Table 2, the older samples (effect size = 0.941) exposed to the natural environment were found to be more positive. Furthermore, we also assessed whether the effect size differed based on the type of natural and built environment, and found that biodiverse areas (effect size = 0.654) and forests (effect size = 0.682) had a higher effect on positive emotion. Moreover, the natural environment had a stronger effect on the positive emotions than the indoor environment (effect size = 1.208).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of subgroup analysis of positive affect studies (n = 20) conducted by study characteristics, participants characteristics, and process of exposure.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of subgroup analysis of negative affect studies (n = 18) conducted by study characteristics, participants characteristics, and process of exposure.

For the effect on negative affect, the results indicated a significant difference between the study region (p = 0.016), sample size (p = 0.006), type of natural (p = 0.003), and built (p < 0.001) environments. A larger effect size was observed in the studies conducted in Europe (effect size = −0.833). The older samples (effect size = −0.873) and the smaller samples (effect size = −0.853) were found to be more effective during exposure to the natural environment. A subgroup analysis based on the process of exposure revealed that exposure of the participants to the biodiverse areas was associated with a significant reduction in negative affect (effect size = −1.019).

Discussion

This systematic review and meta-analysis of 20 studies from 9 countries provided evidence that exposure to the natural environment is associated with the positive and negative affect. The meta-analysis results have showed that brief contact of the participants with the natural environment was associated with the higher levels of positive affect and the lower levels of negative affect.

This analysis is similar to the results of a previous meta-analysis which found that the type of emotion assessment, type of exposure to nature, study location, and mean age of the participants significantly moderated the effect of the natural environment on positive and negative mood (58). In that meta-analysis, the authors assessed the mean effect size of exposure to the natural environments on both positive and negative affect. They included 32 studies from 23 articles that used a randomized controlled design. By comparison, we included all the empirical studies that focused on both the natural and built environments associated with positive and negative affect. This provided a more precise estimate of the difference between the effects of the natural and built environments on positive and negative affect. In addition, the prior meta-analysis was published in 2015, and only included studies published before 2010. In our analysis, 90% of the studies published between 2010 and 2020, highlighted recent developments in the natural environment and psychological health. In addition to study quality, which is different with the previous research, we had performed a risk of bias assessment for each study, a sensitivity analysis, a univariate meta-regression, and a Galbraith radial plot to explore the sources of heterogeneity, and the funnel plots were made to assess the potential publication bias, GRADE system was also used to estimates the quality of evidence. Collectively, our systematic review and meta-analysis builds on the prior meta-analysis, strengthening the contrast between the natural and built environments, and performing more thorough assessments of the included studies. The evidence from our study may be more comprehensive and precise. Our findings are consistent with those of a previous study from some perspectives. The results showed that exposure to the natural environment was associated with an increase in positive affect (SMD = 0.61, 95% CI 0.41, 0.81) and a decrease in negative affect (SMD = −0.47, 95% CI −0.71, −0.24) relative to the comparison conditions, which is consistent with a prior study (positive affect: effect size estimate = 0.31, 95% CI 0.24, 0.37; negative affect, effect size estimate = −0.12, 95% CI −0.17, −0.07). Our findings also converge with previous research that demonstrated that exposure to nature is beneficial to health (10, 30).

The results showed extreme heterogeneity (IPOS2 = 78.4%, INEG2 = 84.0%) between the studies, which remained largely unexplained following a sensitivity analysis. We used the meta-regression models to probe the source of heterogeneity. The univariate meta-regression results showed that study design, study region, mean age, and sample size contributed to the heterogeneity. The model showed that study design was associated with positive affect between natural and built environment. Study region, mean age (p = 0.040) and sample size (p = 0.011) were related to negative affect between natural and built environment. Other variables did not reach the significance level (Supplementary Tables 5, 6). A Galbraith plot indicated that the two studies (46, 55) may have been the main cause of the high heterogeneity. After excluding these two studies, the I2-values decreased (IPOS2 = 63.9%, INEG2 =73.5%). However, the meta-analysis results still showed large heterogeneity. Due to high between-study heterogeneity, the confidence in the pooled estimates were graded as “low” or “very low.” Therefore, the future meta-analyses should include more scientific empirical studies to draw more definitive conclusions.

During the subgroup analysis, we examined factors that may moderate the effect of nature on the positive and negative emotions. The results of these analyses indicated that the study region, study design, mean age of the sample, sample size, and type of natural and built environment were all important factors contributing to increased positive affect and decreased negative affect. As shown in Table 2, the positive effect was 0.923 (95% CI 0.369–1.477), 0.980 (95% CI 0.486–1.474), and 0.401 (95% CI 0.235–0.567), respectively. The study design with within-subject and between-subject got the higher positive affect. This finding was a supplement to the results of previous reports (10, 58). However, significant heterogeneity was found between the groups (p = 0.027), which was consistent with the results of the meta-regression models. On the other hand, study region, sample size, type of natural, and built environment were all found significantly heterogeneity between the groups in the subgroup analysis of negative affect, this might be explained by the potential bias in the included studies and “low” or “very low” quality evidence. To identify the participant characteristic changes in positive and negative affect between natural and built environment, mean age, sample size and gender were considered and the studies were divided into 2–3 groups, i.e., the studies were divided into two groups (18–45, ≥45) according to mean age of the participants. The older samples exposed to natural environment were found to have more positive (effect size = 0.941) and fewer negative (effect size = −0.873) emotions than those exposed to a built environment. This finding was consistent with a previous study (58) and should be taken seriously. There are 16 studies recruiting college students or youth groups in our pooled studies. Future research should pay more attention to the elderly. In addition, a smaller sample size (effect size = −0.853) was found to be significantly associated with a reduction in the negative emotions, which might be explained by the different experimental procedures. Nisbet and Zelenski (53) made all the participants (indoor walking route, n = 78; outdoor walking route, n = 72) walk along their assigned routes. Interference between the participants was inevitable. Another subgroup meta-analysis was based on the process of exposure. The positive affect was 0.654 (95% CI 0.319–0.989), 0.682 (95% CI 0.171–1.193), 0.452 (95% CI 0.147–0.758), and 0.569 (95% CI 0.140–0.999) for biodiverse area, forest, urban park, and university campus, respectively. Thus, the discrepancy was stable regardless of the type of natural environment for positive affect, suggesting that contact with managed nature and wild nature produces similar effect for positive emotion. In contrast, the negative affect was −1.019 (95% CI −2.279 to 0.241), −0.523 (95% CI −0.757 to −0.289), −0.006 (95% CI −0.170 to 0.158), and −0.165 (95% CI −0.662 to 0.333) for biodiverse area, forest, urban park, and university campus, respectively. The findings indicated significant moderation, with exposure to wild natural environments having a greater effect on decreasing negative affect than exposure to the managed natural environments. As a result, managed nature could serve as substitute for wild nature to increase positive emotions, while this is not suitable for alleviating negative emotions.

Strengths and Limitations

The current study provides the most comprehensive and up-to-date findings on the effect of exposure to the natural environment on positive and negative affect. Its key strengths are its broad range of included studies, and its fully reproducible and transparent meta-analysis approach. Second, we assessed the quality and risk of bias for each included study according to the validated scales and determined confidence of our pooled estimates according to the GRADE system. Thus, our results may be valuable for the researchers in this area and improve quality of future research. Third, we conducted the subgroup analyses to show the positive and negative affect between natural and built environment, potential moderators of the effect of natural on positive and negative affect were discussed, thus, to help clarify the manner in which nature contributes to human well-being.

However, our study had some limitations. First, there was extreme heterogeneity in all of the pooled studies. Second, according to the GRADE system, the credibility of the cumulative evidence was “low” or “very low.” The main causes may include high heterogeneity, and inconsistent results across the studies. Third, the included studies were not representative of global populations, as half were from North America and, the others were form Europe and Asia, so our results may not be generalizable to the other areas. Fourth, several included studies used the small samples, which tend to provide a less stable estimate of the effect size. Fifth, nearly all our included studies controlled for potential confounders, yet confounding factors varied among the studies, and some important confounding factors, such as the quantity of greenspace were not considered.

Conclusion

In summary, the present systematic review and meta-analysis indicated an association between the natural environment and positive and negative affect. However, owing to extreme heterogeneity, it is difficult to draw a robust conclusion. Future research in this area should take appropriate steps to reduce bias and improve quality to build a stronger evidence-based medical foundation. The research focus should also be extended to other geographic areas. The advanced methods should be applied to assess the positive and negative effects. Further technological refinement is required to become a viable tool to support evidence-based decision-making for public health and greenspace provision. Some important confounding variables, such as meteorological factors and, quantity and quality of greenspace should be collected and adjusted. Finally, although our systematic review uncovered a large body of research on the relationship between the environment and positive and negative affect, there is a paucity of studies on the mechanisms underlying this relationship. The mechanistic studies are needed to clarify the manner in which nature contributes to the development of human feelings and functioning, which could possibly answer more questions regarding the nature of the relationship between human health and the environment.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

WY: conceptualization, project administration, funding acquisition, and writing. WY, SW, and XZ: assessment and analysis. WY and FC: data retrieval, curation, and methodology. All authors made substantial contributions to the study. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No.32001367) and Open Fund of innovation institute for Sustainable Maritime Architecture Research and Technology (iSMART), Qingdao University of Technology (No. C2020-037).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2021.758457/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Bielinis E, Bielinis L, Krupińska-Szeluga S, Łukowski A, Takayama N. The effects of a short forest recreation program on physiological and psychological relaxation in young Polish adults. Forests. (2019) 10:34. doi: 10.3390/f10010034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Dopko RL, Capaldi CA, Zelenski JM. The psychological and social benefits of a nature experience for children: a preliminary investigation. J Environ Psychol. (2019) 63:134–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lee I, Choi H, Bang KS, Kim S, Song M, Lee B. Effects of forest therapy on depressive symptoms among adults: a systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2017) 14:321. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Mao GXLX, Lan XG, Cao YB, Chen ZM, He ZH, Lv YD, et al. Effects of short-term forest bathing on human health in a broad-leaved evergreen forest in Zhejiang Province, China. Biomed Environ Sci. (2012) 25:317–24. doi: 10.3967/0895-3988.2012.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Yamaguchi M, Deguchi M, Miyazaki Y. The effects of exercise in forest and urban environments on sympathetic nervous activity of normal young adults. J Int Med Res. (2006) 34:152–9. doi: 10.1177/147323000603400204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Bowler DE, Buyung-Ali LM, Knight TM, Pullin AS. A systematic review of evidence for the added benefits to health of exposure to natural environments. BMC Public Health. (2010) 10:456. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Gascon M, Triguero-Mas M, Martínez D, Dadvand P, Forns J, Plasència A, et al. Mental health benefits of long-term exposure to residential green and blue spaces: a systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2015) 12:4354–79. doi: 10.3390/ijerph120404354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Houlden V, Weich S, Porto de Albuquerque J, Jarvis S, Rees K. The relationship between greenspace and the mental wellbeing of adults: a systematic review. PLOS ONE. (2018) 13:e0203000. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0203000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Ulrich RS, Simons RF, Losito BD, Fiorito E, Miles MA, Zelson M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J Environ Psychol. (1991) 11:201–30. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Roberts H, van Lissa C, Hagedoorn P, Kellar I, Helbich M. The effect of short-term exposure to the natural environment on depressive mood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Environ Res. (2019) 177:108606. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2019.108606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Elsadek M, Liu B, Lian Z, Xie J. The influence of urban roadside trees and their physical environment on stress relief measures: a field experiment in Shanghai. Urban Forest Urban Green. (2019) 42:51–60. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2019.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Grahn P, Stigsdotter UK. The relation between perceived sensory dimensions of urban green space and stress restoration. Landsc Urban Plan. (2010) 94:264–75. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2009.10.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Park BJ, Tsunetsugu Y, Kasetani T, Hirano H, Kagawa T, Sato M, et al. Physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku (taking in the atmosphere of the forest) –using salivary cortisol and cerebral activity as indicators. J Physiol Anthropol. (2007) 26:123–8. doi: 10.2114/jpa2.26.123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Yao W, Zhang X, Gong Q. The effect of exposure to the natural environment on stress reduction: a meta-analysis. Urban Forest Urban Green. (2021) 57:126932. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2020.126932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kaplan S. The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative framework. J Environ Psychol. (1995) 15:169–82. doi: 10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Berman MG, Kross E, Krpan KM, Askren MK, Burson A, Deldin PJ, et al. Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. J Affect Disord. (2012) 140:300–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bratman GN, Daily GC, Levy BJ, Gross JJ. The benefits of nature experience: improved affect and cognition. Landsc Urban Plan. (2015) 138:41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. De Brito JN, Pope ZC, Mitchell NR, Schneider IE, Larson JM, Horton TH, et al. Changes in psychological and cognitive outcomes after green versus suburban walking: a pilot crossover study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:2894. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16162894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Saadi D, Schnell I, Tirosh E, Basagaña X, Agay-Shay K. There's no place like home? The psychological, physiological, and cognitive effects of short visits to outdoor urban environments compared to staying in the indoor home environment, a field experiment on women from two ethnic groups. Environ Res. (2020) 187:109687. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2020.109687, 32480028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Velarde MD, Fry G, Tveit M. Health effects of viewing landscapes – landscape types in environmental psychology. Urban Forest Urban Green. (2007) 6:199–212. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2007.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Diener E. The remarkable changes in the science of subjective well-being. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2013) 8:663–6. doi: 10.1177/1745691613507583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Yao WF, Chen F, Zhu RR, Wang S, Zhang XF. Impact of exposure to natural and built environments on positive and negative affect: a systematic review and meta-analysis. (2021) PROSPERO CRD42021239711. Available online at: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/display_record.php?~ID=CRD42021239711 (accessed May 14, 2021).

Google Scholar

23. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC, Ioannidis JPA, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. J Clin Epidemiol. (2009) 62:e1–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLOS Med. (2009) 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Anderson AP, Mayer MD, Fellows AM, Cowan DR, Hegel MT, Buckey JC. Relaxation with immersive natural scenes presented using virtual reality. Aerosp Med Hum Perform. (2017) 88:520–6. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.4747.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Huang Q, Yang M, Jane H, Li S, Bauer N. Trees, grass, or concrete? The effects of different types of environments on stress reduction. Landsc Urban Plan. (2020) 193:103654. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2019.103654, 103654

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. McAllister E, Bhullar N, Schutte NS. Into the woods or a stroll in the park: how virtual contact with nature impacts positive and negative affect. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2017) 14:786. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14070786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Schutte NS, Bhullar N, Stilinović EJ, Richardson K. The impact of virtual environments on restorativeness and affect. Ecopsychology. (2017) 9:1–7. doi: 10.1089/eco.2016.0042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Valtchanov D, Ellard C. Physiological and affective responses to immersion in virtual reality: effects of nature and urban settings. J Cyber Ther Rehabil. (2010) 3:359.

Google Scholar

30. Twohig-Bennett C, Jones A. The health benefits of the great outdoors: a systematic review and meta-analysis of greenspace exposure and health outcomes. Environ Res. (2018) 166:628–37. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2018.06.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ogilvie D, Foster CE, Rothnie H, Cavill N, Hamilton V, Fitzsimons CF, et al. Interventions to promote walking: systematic review. BMJ. (2007) 334:1204–7. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39198.722720.BE

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Martens D, Gutscher H, Bauer N. Walking in “wild” and “tended” urban forests: the impact on psychological well-being. J Environ Psychol. (2011) 31:36–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Maund PR, Irvine KN, Reeves J, Strong E, Cromie R, Dallimer M, et al. Wetlands for wellbeing: piloting a nature-based health intervention for the management of anxiety and depression. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:4413. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16224413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Simkin J, Ojala A, Tyrväinen L. Restorative effects of mature and young commercial forests, pristine old-growth forest and urban recreation forest - a field experiment. Urban Forest Urban Green. (2020) 48:126567. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2019.126567

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Tyrväinen L, Ojala A, Korpela K, Lanki T, Tsunetsugu Y, Kagawa T. The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: a field experiment. J Environ Psychol. (2014) 38:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Janeczko E, Bielinis E, Wójcik R, Woznicka M, Kedziora W, Łukowski A, et al. When urban environment is restorative: the effect of walking in suburbs and forests on psychological and physiological relaxation of young Polish adults. Forests. (2020) 11:591. doi: 10.3390/f11050591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. (2003) 327:557–60. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Higgins JPT, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, et al. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 6.2 (updated February 2021). Cochrane (2021).

Google Scholar

39. Gu H, Wang L, Liu L, Luo X, Wang J, Hou F, et al. A gradient relationship between low birth weight and IQ: a meta-analysis. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:18035. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18234-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. (1997) 315:629–34. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. GRADE Working Group. Grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ. (2004) 328:1490. doi: 10.1136/bmj.328.7454.1490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Fan SJ, Heinrich J, Bloom MS, Zhao TY, Shi TX, Feng WR, et al. Ambient air pollution and depression: a systematic review with meta-analysis up to 2019. Sci Total Environ. (2020) 701:134721. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2019.134721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Bielinis E, Takayama N, Boiko S, Omelan A, Bielinis L. The effect of winter forest bathing on psychological relaxation of young Polish adults. Urban Forest Urban Green. (2018) 29:276–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2017.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bielinis Ł, Omelan B, Takayama G. The effect of recreation in a snow-covered forest environment on the psychological wellbeing of young adults: randomized controlled study. Forests. (2019) 10:827. doi: 10.3390/f10100827

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Brooks AM, Ottley KM, Arbuthnott KD, Sevigny P. Nature-related mood effects: season and type of nature contact. J Environ Psychol. (2017) 54:91–102. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Browning MHEM, Mimnaugh KJ, van Riper CJ, Laurent HK, LaValle SM. Can simulated nature support mental health? Comparing short, single-doses of 360-degree nature videos in virtual reality with the outdoors. Front Psychol. (2020) 10:2667. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Calogiuri G, Evensen K, Weydahl A, Andersson K, Patil G, Ihlebæk C, et al. Green exercise as a workplace intervention to reduce job stress. Results from a pilot study. Work. (2015) 53:99–111. doi: 10.3233/WOR-152219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Fuegen K, Breitenbecher KH. Walking and being outdoors in nature increase positive affect and energy. Ecopsychology. (2018) 10:14–25. doi: 10.1089/eco.2017.0036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Grazuleviciene R, Vencloviene J, Kubilius R, Grizas V, Danileviciute A, Dedele A, et al. Tracking restoration of park and urban street settings in coronary artery disease patients. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2016) 13:550. doi: 10.3390/ijerph13060550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Hartig T, Mang M, Evans GW. Restorative effects of natural environment experiences. Environ Behav. (1991) 23:3–26. doi: 10.1177/0013916591231001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Mayer FS, Frantz CM, Bruehlman-Senecal E, Dolliver K. Why is nature beneficial? The role of connectedness to nature. Environ Behav. (2009) 41:607–43. doi: 10.1177/0013916508319745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Neill C, Gerard J, Arbuthnott KD. Nature contact and mood benefits: contact duration and mood type. J Posit Psychol. (2019) 14:756–67. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2018.1557242

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM. Underestimating nearby nature: affective forecasting errors obscure the happy path to sustainability. Psychol Sci. (2011) 22:1101–6. doi: 10.1177/0956797611418527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Olafsdottir G, Cloke P, Schulz A, van Dyck Z, Eysteinsson T, Thorleifsdottir B, et al. Health benefits of walking in nature: A randomized controlled study under conditions of real-life stress. Environ Behav. (2020) 52:248–74. doi: 10.1177/0013916518800798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Reeves JP, Knight AT, Strong EA, Heng V, Neale C, Cromie R, et al. The application of wearable technology to quantify health and wellbeing co-benefits from urban wetlands. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:1840. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01840

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Takayama N, Korpela K, Lee J, Morikawa T, Tsunetsugu Y, Park B, et al. Emotional, restorative and vitalizing effects of forest and urban environments at four sites in Japan. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2014):7207–30. doi: 10.3390/ijerph110707207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Takayama N, Morikawa T, Bielinis E. Relation between psychological restorativeness and lifestyle, quality of life, resilience, and stress-coping in forest settings. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:1456. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16081456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. McMahan EA, Estes D. The effect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative affect: a meta-analysis. J Posit Psychol. (2015) 10:507–19. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.994224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: natural environment, built environment, meta-analysis, positive affect (PA), negative affect (NA)

Citation: Yao W, Chen F, Wang S and Zhang X (2021) Impact of Exposure to Natural and Built Environments on Positive and Negative Affect: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Public Health 9:758457. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.758457

Received: 14 August 2021; Accepted: 20 October 2021;
Published: 25 November 2021.

Edited by:

Dimirios Nikolopoulos, University of West Attica, Greece

Reviewed by:

Vahidreza Ostadmohammadi, Kashan University of Medical Sciences, Iran
Sui Zhu, Jinan University, China

Copyright © 2021 Yao, Chen, Wang and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fei Chen, chenfei3913@126.com; Wenfei Yao, yaowenfei8345604@163.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.