Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sociol., 04 January 2023
Sec. Sociological Theory
Volume 7 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fsoc.2022.1048397

People's wellbeing, civic capital and sustainable practices: Evidence from the European Values Study survey

  • Department of Economics, Business and Statistics, University of Palermo, Palermo, Italy

The climate change issue is showing an unprecedented level of awareness in the political realm. Changing occasional sustainable practices into stable behaviors is the challenge that policymakers face. However, what makes people environmentally aware is an unsolved question, and research on this direction is in evolution. This paper examines factors that promote environmentally responsible behaviors. The study tests the hypothesis that people's wellbeing (SWB) predisposes individuals toward environmentalism. The mechanism of social and civic capital may underlie this association since people reporting higher wellbeing levels show empathy, solidarity, and greater civic engagement. This hypothesis is examined in the context of the European Union using micro-data from the European Values Study—EVS (wave 2017–2022). Results support the hypothesis that people's life satisfaction is compatible with the environmental mindset, given that those who report higher wellbeing express civicness and share pro-environmental beliefs and values. Evidence from this research suggests that supporting SWB growth may offer a fertile ground for promoting ecological awareness and developing more sustainable societies.

1. Introduction

In recent times, environmental issues have dominated political agendas worldwide, and many spontaneous movements claim urgent actions for a more sustainable and inclusive society1 (Signoretta et al., 2020). At the individual level, adopting eco-friendly lifestyles is essential to coping with ecological concerns (York et al., 2003). For example, reducing household energy use, supporting the recycling principle, and limiting food waste in favor of greener consumption are practical solutions that individuals can do to limit their ecological footprint (Dietz et al., 2007; Thompson et al., 2011).

However, a better understanding of environmental attitudes is the prerequisite for effectively transforming people's lifestyles. The first step is identifying factors that encourage or discourage such attitudes. Literature indicates many drivers of environmental intentions, such as social and personal norms (Schwartz, 1994, 1996; Stern et al., 1999; Turaga et al., 2010), awareness of consequences (Gifford and Nilsson, 2014), and locus of control (Coşkun et al., 2022). Studies also indicate that altruism and social engagement are relevant factors for pro-environmentalism (Dietz et al., 2002; Knez, 2016). Based on this argument, Schultz (2000, 2001) has found that sustainably oriented people support each other in a social-altruistic manner, whereas Macias (2016) has found that social capital variables are good predictors of sustainable practices. It means that social and civic capital are essential for pro-environmental behaviors, given the importance of citizen cooperation in preserving the environment (see Andreoni, 1990; Steg and De Groot, 2010; Goldy and Piff, 2020).

From another perspective, growing studies point out that more satisfied people often operate in a cooperative and trustful manner (Tov and Diener, 2009). For example, Kushlev et al. (2020) have found that both happiness and life satisfaction positively affect prosocial behaviors, whereas recently, Ciziceno and Pizzuto (2022) argue that satisfied people accept moral and civic values and less justify the extra-legal ones.

Although these findings suggest that people's subjective wellbeing (SWB) elicits civicness and that civicness explains, in part, environmental behavior, few studies have linked SWB directly to people's ecological orientation.

This paper extends the current research on ecocentrism vs. anthropocentrism (see Thompson and Barton, 1994) and subjective wellbeing (SWB). The idea is that higher SWB is associated with individuals' ecocentric orientation (than anthropocentric one). Being more satisfied with their own lives makes people sensitive to their community's health and wellbeing (in other words, helping them to be more civic). The relationship between environmentalism and SWB is explored using micro-data from the European Values Study (EVS) wave 2017–2022. The results suggest that life satisfaction is positively associated with ecocentrism, and this association is stable after several controls. To further confirm the hypothesis that SWB activates a virtuous cycle encompassing both sustainable and civic attitudes, it has been tested the association between life satisfaction and a set of variables proxy for individuals' civicness. The main finding from this exercise is that people with higher life satisfaction systematically reject those behaviors at the expense of others or their community, such as falsely claiming benefits or accepting a bribe.

This paper contributes to the literature on ecological orientation and subjective wellbeing from a different two-fold perspective. Firstly, it adds further knowledge to the positive factors underlying environmental orientations, namely peoples' wellbeing. Secondly, it supports studies that have found as civic duty helps urban development and increases environmentalism (Bamberg and Möser, 2007; Corral-Verdugo et al., 2009). Finally, the policy implications of such findings are discussed.

2. Literature review

2.1. Environmental orientation: Ecocentrism vs. anthropocentrism

Environmental orientation encompasses a series of individual attitudes aimed at protecting both the physical and the social environment (Axelrod and Lehman, 1993; Dietz et al., 2007; Prati et al., 2017). However, a crucial point in environmental studies is the relationship between peoples' orientation and their actual behavior. Mainstream literature reports that people with a pro-environmental orientation act environmentally consequently (Schlegelmilch et al., 1996; Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002). Kopnina (2012, p. 611) argues that people with ecocentric orientations are much more likely to actually act upon their values, attitudes, and beliefs in order to protect the environment than those with anthropocentric orientations.

According to the Values Belief Norm Theory2 (Stern et al., 1999), individuals' values influence pro-environmental behaviors through the mediating effect of beliefs and personal norms. For example, if people believe that car use causes problems for the environment, it is more likely that they feel morally obligated to limit their personal use of the car (Hiratsuka et al., 2018). Ajzen and Fishbein (1975) have demonstrated that intentions are a predictor of volitional behavior, and cumulative results (see Hinesa et al., 1987; Sheppard et al., 1988) indicate the predictive utility that attitudes, values, and social norms have on human behaviors, including pro-environment actions3. However, environmental beliefs do not always translate into ecological efforts, and this gap is known as the knowledge-concern-action paradox (Lenzen and Cummins, 2011).

Besides, according to Schultz (2000, 2001), people's environmental concerns about their health or other people are a reliable proxy of environmentalism. Schultz (2000, 2001), adopting the tripartite structure of egoistic, socio-altruistic, and biospheric people's orientation, has found that those who meet an egoistic orientation tend to emphasize the consequences of environmental deterioration only for themselves, whereas those who express apprehension for other humans have a socio-altruistic' view. Lastly, individuals representing a biospheric orientation stress the consequences of environmental deterioration for humans and non-humans (including animals, plants, and ecosystems). Starting from the former perspective, Thompson and Barton (1994) have classified environmental orientation using the ecocentric, anthropocentric, and apathy scales. In this approach, Schultz's egoistic and socio-altruistic dimensions collapse into a single construct indicating an ecocentric orientation. Individuals with an ecocentric orientation are more sensitive about problems afflicting nature. For this reason, the literature argues that relevant intrinsic motives for pro-environmental behaviors are prosociality and altruism (Owen and Videras, 2006). On the contrary, anthropocentrism is a human-centered approach (Abun and Racoma, 2017). In this view, nature essentially exists to meet human needs, and the main reason for preserving biodiversity is to ensure people live. People who express environmental apathy consider the risks related to the environment as exaggerated (Yildiz and Erciş, 2022) and tend to minimize the relevance of ecological concerns.

2.2 The relationship between environmental orientation and subjective wellbeing

During the last few years, literature on subjective wellbeing (SWB) has grown dramatically as an intersection between sociology, psychology, and economics to study people's evaluation of their lives (Kroll, 2014). This research area has become popular in the political arena, even if sociologists do not consider SWB a big topic (Veenhoven, 2000). However, scholars (Nolan and Lenski, 2004; Kroll, 2014) agree that wellbeing study addresses crucial questions for sociology, connecting the individual-level with the social one. The SWB is the combination of both individuals' emotional states (i.e., pleasant and unpleasant emotions) and cognitive evaluation of his/her life (i.e., life satisfaction, see Diener et al., 1985). Experimental studies have demonstrated that life satisfaction is a separable component of SWB that could be analyzed independently (Diener et al., 2002). Besides, life satisfaction refers to a general evaluation of individuals' lives, and research on this topic often relies on life satisfaction as a proxy of SWB. Indeed, compared to other components of SWB (e.g., happiness), Fujita and Diener (2005) have found that life satisfaction is less susceptible to cultural differences and that it shows appreciable psychometric properties.

Existing literature indicates an association between SWB and pro-environmental behaviors, even if part of such research offers contrasting or less generalizable results. For example, Brown and Kasser (2005) have found that SWB and ecologically responsible behaviors are compatible, demonstrating that people with intrinsic value orientation are both happier and more ecological. However, they provide results from two small-size samples mainly composed of students and voluntary respondents. Similarly, Prati et al. (2017), using a restricted sample of students, tested the causal relationship between social wellbeing and pro-environmental behavior. They show that a reciprocal relationship exists, in which acting pro-environmentally increases SWB and vice versa. Suárez-Varela et al. (2016) have found that acting environmentally has either a positive or no significant influence on SWB. Other studies, such as those of Venhoeven et al. (2013), indicate that eco-friendly behaviors increase SWB, but only partially (i.e., they affect eudaimonic wellbeing). Welsch and Kühling (2010) have demonstrated that life satisfaction is positively associated with environmental-friendly behaviors (e.g., green consumption), and such association may be driven by peoples' intrinsic motives such as empathy or altruism.

However, despite their ecological awareness, people do not ever operate environmentally (Lenzen and Cummins, 2011). Furthermore, greener solutions are often more expensive than traditional ones, and this may discourage any complaint intentions. Other studies have focused on the negative influences environmental concerns have on SWB (Cottrell, 2003). For example, Rehdanz and Maddison (2008) find that concern about noise levels and air pollution reduces SWB, whereas Ferrer-i-Carbonell and Gowdy (2007) indicate that caring about climate change factors (i.e., ozone layer) constitutes a mental stressor for individuals' wellbeing.

3. Data and measures

3.1 Data description

Data used in this study are drawn from the European Values Study (EVS 2017–2022)4. The EVS survey collects large-scale, cross-national information about a multitude of social, political, and cultural issues of European citizens. The latest wave available (2017–2022) also includes detailed information on public opinion about the actual environmental situation. The survey data were collected by representative single-stage or multi-stage sampling of the adult population (18 years old and over). The EVS dataset is fully exploited, and the list of countries included in the analysis has been determined by data availability.

3.2 Measures

This subsection describes all the survey items included in the analyses.

Ecocentric vs. Anthropocentric attitudes: a 3-item scale measures respondents' environmental orientation. The items asked the following questions: (1) It is just too difficult for someone like me to do much about the environment, (2) There are more important things to do in life than protect the environment, and (3) Many of the claims about environmental threats are exaggerated.

Participants' answers range from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree), where higher scores (strongly disagree) indicate their ecocentric orientation. A single-item using a scenario asks the following question: here are two statements people sometimes make when discussing the environment and economic growth. Which of them comes closer to your own point of view? Respondents may answer by giving priority to the environment (ecocentric orientation) or preferring economic growth and job creation (anthropocentric orientation). Factor analysis tests whether all items used are loaded on the same factor. The study uses principal components as a method of extraction (varimax rotation) and indicates that all the items led to a one-factor solution (range factor loadings 0.65 to 0.77), explaining 50.01% of the variance.

Life satisfaction: a single-item measures the satisfaction with life. Participants in the EVS answered the following question: all things considered, how satisfied are you with your life as a whole these days? The score ranges from 1 (completely dissatisfied) to 10 (completely satisfied).

Control variables: literature indicates numerous demographic and socio-economic factors shaping environmental attitudes, such as education (Kopnina, 2012), age, and economic status (for a review, see Gifford and Nilsson, 2014). The control variables included in the analyses are gender (0 = female; 1 = male), the age of respondents (recoded as 1 = 15–29 years; 2 = 30–49 years; 3 = 50 and more years), educational level (ISCED classification harmonized in 8 categories from 1 = uncomplete elementary education to 8 = university degree), household total net income5 (recoded into three levels as 1 = low; 2 = middle and 3 = high), marital status (recoded into the following categories: married; registered partnership; widowed; divorced; separated; never married and never registered partnership). The analyses also include individuals' per-capita GDP6 as a control variable for life satisfaction levels across countries. The SWB literature stresses the notion that happiness parallel increases to personal income and economic growth. However, Easterlin (1995) has found that turning the satiation point, such a relationship loses statistical significance. To control for the effect of the implementation of sustainable policies on individuals' environmental attitudes, the country-specific score of the Sustainable Development Goal index (SDGi)7 has been added to the models.

Robustness check variables: the EVS survey uses multiple response questions measuring individuals' civicness, such as their beliefs on civic duty, community respect, and fairness. Those variables are included in the analyses as a robustness check (see subsection 5.1). The 4-items measure respondents' justification of different non-compliant behaviors: (1) falsely claiming government benefits; (2) cheating on tax; (3) someone accepting a bribe; (4) avoiding a fare on public transport. Respondents' answers range from 1 (never justified) to 10 (always justified). To facilitate the interpretation of the results, the items have been recoded, and the answer is never justified… assumes the highest value (=10), indicating better compliance with social norms and higher prosocial orientation. The factor analysis indicates that all the items used led to a one-factor solution (range factor loadings 0.77 to 0.73), explaining 57.01% of the variance.

4. Analytical strategy and method

This paper aims to investigate the role of life satisfaction as a potential driver for individuals' environmental orientation. Thus, the research hypotheses are the following:

H1: higher life satisfaction (LS) is associated with ecocentric orientation than with anthropocentric one.

H2: this association is robust after controlling for both individuals' socio-demographic characteristics and country-specific variables.

H3: life satisfaction is also linked to other relevant civic and ethical beliefs (under the assumption that life satisfaction encourages both pro-environmental and civic values).

To investigate the relationship between ecocentric orientation and life satisfaction, it has been used an ordered logit model. The coefficients are estimated using maximum likelihood (MLE). The ordered logit models are discrete choice models used in several econometric applications, including, for example, individuals' recycling preferences (Nixon et al., 2009). The model considers the ordinality of response variables and estimates the category that most closely fits with respondents' feelings on questions8 (Grilli and Rampichini, 2014). The ordinal logistic regression model used is based on the following specification:

yi*=α+βLSi+γxi+εi    (1)

where, xi is a matrix of variables indicating the socio-economic and demographic individuals' characteristics (such as age, gender, and educational level) and other control variables often associated with environmental behavior, such as per-capita GDP and the SGDi score. LSi is the reported level of life satisfaction for the i-th individual participating in the survey. Then, α is the intercept term, and εi is the error term capturing the unobserved individuals' characteristics. Finally, yi*is the dependent variable indicating the ecocentrism level for the i-th individual, that in this dataset can take values from 1 to 5. To eases the interpretation of results for each explanatory variable (except life satisfaction9 and continuous variables), one level has been omitted. The combination of omitted levels gives the characteristics of the reference group, and the estimate for this group is reflected by the coefficient for the constant. Finally, Equation (1) is augmented with a set of variables indicating prosocial orientation, to shed light on hypothesis 3.

5. Results

Table 1 reports the baseline results from Equation (1) estimation of the whole sample. The ordered log-odds models in Table 1 indicate that life satisfaction is associated with different measures of ecocentrism (H1). Notably, for one unit of increase in the self-reported level of life satisfaction (from 1 to 10 points), the likelihood of environmental verbal commitment increases by 13% (odds ratio = 1.133, SE = 0.012, see Table 1, model 1). A potential explanation of such a relationship may reside in the fact that satisfied people are more socially responsible and sensitive to the community's needs than unsatisfied ones (Tov and Diener, 2009). This, in turn, may drive their attitudes toward altruistic social practices, which also include environmentalism. Similar evidence has been found by Welsch and Kühling (2010), who showed as life satisfaction increases environmental friendliness and pro-environmental behaviors. Kasser (2017) has provided empirical evidence that a positive correlation between wellbeing and pro-ecological behaviors exists.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Baseline estimations.

The set variables included in the models as covariates indicate that age and gender are predictors of ecocentrism. Consistently to existing studies, aged people tend to be more interested in the environmental issue than younger ones, whereas being female increases the log odds of being environmentally responsible by 32% more than male (see Table 1, model 3). In support of these results, Casey and Scott (2006) have found that younger people are less ecocentric than older, whereas Scannell and Gifford (2013) pointed out that women are more environmentally concerned than men, and for this reason, they frequently act environmentally.

Looking at socio-demographic variables, having a higher educational level (i.e., university degree) and being married or divorced are predictors of ecocentrism (see Table 1, models 1, 2, 3, 4). In a meta-analysis have found that both environmental knowledge and education level determine pro-environment behaviors. In line with these findings, Chanda (1999) has found that individuals higher in education display greater attention to environmental concerns. Also, results from Table 1 indicate that belonging to the upper level of income (from 1 to 3) increases the likelihood of being more ecocentric. Studies supporting this finding suggest that, for example, in Germany, environmentalists are often people from the middle and middle-upper classes rather than lower (Balderjahn, 1988).

To clearly identify the association between life satisfaction and ecocentrism, Equation (1) has been augmented with control variables often associated with SWB and environmental intentions. The results of the robustness check are reported in Table 2. Notably, after the introduction of per-capita GDP and the Sustainable Development Goal index (SDGi) results are very similar and broadly unchanged with respect to the baseline estimation (see Table 2). In line with the H2, results show that the increases in per-capita GDP are associated likelihood that individuals prefer respecting the environment (Table 2, models 1, 2, 3, 4). An explanation of such a finding has been provided by Inglehart (1997). According to the author, in economically developed countries, individuals often shift their reference values from materialistic to post-materialistic ones, including environmental protection and ecological respect.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Log-odds estimations with additional control variables.

5.1 Robustness check: The relationship between SWB and civicness

Equation (1) has been re-estimated by adding a set of civic norms as response variables and life satisfaction as a predictor (see Table 3). Respect for civic norms is an expression of social capital and helps people live in a more stable and predictable environment (Putnam, 2000; Bjørnskov, 2003). Studies have shown as following these norms prevents natural resources degradation and limit individuals' opportunistic behaviors (Pretty and Ward, 2001; Owen and Videras, 2006). Other empirical findings link the effects of SWB on social and civic capital (see Bjørnskov, 2003; Helliwell and Putnam, 2004; Kroll, 2011). To further confirm the hypothesis that life satisfaction is a predictor (or driver) of positive outcomes not limited to environmental issues, I check for the effect of life satisfaction on a set of other variables expressing civicness (H3). Civic behavior often refers to actions aimed at limiting free riding for the benefit of others or the community. Examples of such behaviors include donating blood and organs and volunteerism (Putnam, 1993), but also compiling with taxes and respecting the physical environment. This definition of civicness drowns back from the socialization theory (Durkheim, 1951[1897]; Parsons, 1967), which solves the puzzle of civic engagement by looking at the individuals' level of internalization of norms or other institutional factors such as the social cohesion within a society.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Life satisfaction and civic capital.

Results from this robustness check show that for one unit of increase in life satisfaction, the likelihood that individuals do not justify false state benefits increases by 9%, reject tax cheating by 10%, do not justify bribery by 11%, and refuse to cheat on public transport 8% (Table 3, models 1, 2, 3, 4). These findings are consistent with those of Ciziceno and Pizzuto (2022), who have shown that life satisfaction fosters tax morale and civic values, using a similar set variable from the World Values Survey.

6. Discussion

This study provides preliminary evidence that environmentalism and civicness share similar features. Both concepts seem to be moved by the same mechanism, people's wellbeing. The social capital theory (see Putnam, 1993, 2000) could explain how these associations run. Acting environmentally is a social practice expressing altruism and civic engagement, as previous studies have established (Macias, 2016). On the other side, peoples' wellbeing (i.e., life satisfaction) systematically predisposes individuals to altruism, empathy, and solidarity. Thus, individuals who are more satisfied with their life meet civic values, including preserving public goods and respecting the environment. Brown and Kasser (2005) have found that happier people live in more ecological ways than unhappy peers. Indeed, they are more focused on their community's wellbeing and may voluntarily limit their ecological footprint to benefit future generations. According to Aknin et al. (2018), a positive feedback loop exists between happiness and civic behaviors, such as donating blood or helping others. Studies discussed in previous sections, especially that of Tov and Diener (2009), indicate that subjective wellbeing increases the spirit of collaboration among citizens (i.e., increases social capital). Kushlev et al. (2020), using data from the Gallup World Poll (GWP), found that both life satisfaction and happiness were mainly associated with altruism worldwide.

Another relevant finding is that peoples' ecological attitudes differ across nations. Inglehart (1995) supports the thesis that environmental protection is higher in richer countries than in poor or underdeveloped ones and results from this study are consistent with it. According to postmaterialist theory (Inglehart, 1995, 1997), people living in economically developed countries perceive the environment as a national priority because, in those countries, postmaterialist values are prevalent. However, comparative studies have demonstrated that environmentalism is also culturally dependent. For example, Eom et al. (2016) have compared two groups of individuals from individualistic (i.e., U.S.) and collectivistic (i.e., Japan) cultures, demonstrating that cultural dimensions drive sustainable behaviors. Similarly, Xiang et al. (2019) found that individualist/collectivist orientations influence individuals' climate change actions, with collectivist people more sensitive to climate change actions.

Despite literature indicating that being connected with nature improves peoples' wellbeing [see the concept of biophilia in Wilson (1984)], results from the current study do not exclude diverse causal evidence. For example, Kasser (2017) finds that a reverse causal direction of the relationship between SWB and environmentalism could coexist. Following Kasser's finding, Zelenski and Desrochers (2021) have demonstrated that positive and self-transcendent emotions (including happiness) foster prosocial and pro-environmental behaviors.

7. Conclusion

The ecosystems' degradation has increased at alarming levels, and it is even more evident the direct repercussions of human actions on the ecological sphere. This circumstance has imposed a reflection on peoples' habits and lifestyles (York et al., 2003). Considering that actual environmental concerns (e.g., heatwaves, weather extremes, loss of biodiversity, desertification) are direct consequences of anthropic activities (IPCC, 2022), adopting pro-environmental practices could mitigate these effects. However, a better understating of which mechanisms promote individuals' environmental intentions is the prerequisite for an effective transformation of people's lifestyles. This paper offers a preliminary indication that more satisfied people meet ecocentric values and beliefs. This finding suggests that increasing the SWB levels is not only a desirable policy goal by itself. Supporting the SWB growth may offer a fertile ground for the promotion of ecological awareness and the development of more sustainable societies. There is evidence that most pro-environmental behaviors are also prosocial since they require cooperation and civic duty. It is well-noted that one of the crucial elements in implementing environmental public policies is community involvement and commitment. Thompson et al. (2011) used the expression “ecological solidarity,” indicating a conceptual way for rethinking ecological and social interdependence. Results from this study support the idea that greater life satisfaction encourages peoples' ecocentric values. In this context, the path toward the development of individuals' greener orientation involves their levels of wellbeing. However, promoting environmental matters at the individual level is not enough to change citizens' lifestyles. Policymakers should implement collective policy strategies to enhance the level of civic cooperation and people's wellbeing. As an indirect result, this could make more accessible the implementation of environmental policies aimed at modifying people's lifestyles in favor of greener social practices.

8. Limitations of the study and directions for future research

Some limitations affect this research. First, it is based on a representative sample from European countries that limit cross-cultural comparisons with other areas of the world. Further research in this direction could include larger samples from worldwide. Second, a potential limitation of the current research may be the nature of the data analyzed. Indeed, they came from the wave 2017–2022 of the EVS, and they do not consider the evolution of environmental values over time. Given that environmental values and individual life satisfaction are time-sensitive variables, future research on this topic should start from longitudinal data analysis.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://doi.org/10.4232/1.13897.

Author contributions

MC: conception and design of the research, theoretical framework, statistical analysis and interpretation of results, writing, and revising of the manuscript.

Funding

This publication has been realized with the financial contribution of the European Union, FSE, PON Ricerca e Innovazione 2014-2020, DM 1062/2021.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^See for example, the School Strike for Climate (SS4C)/Fridays for Future youth collectivities: https://fridaysforfuture.org/.

2. ^The VBN Theory is an applied version of the moral norm-activation theory (Schwartz, 1977) in the environmental context.

3. ^In this paper, pro-environmental attitudes are considered as individuals' dispositions in respect of the environment and not their effective behavior. The concept of attitude can be expressed as the potential behavior that an individual is expected to exhibit towards a situation, event, or phenomenon (see the Ajzen and Fishbein's, 1975 model).

4. ^EVS (2022).

5. ^The income variable has been recoded for each country according to its distribution into three categories, each containing approximately one-third of the country's sample. For detailed information, see EVS (2022) Variable Report App. B1: mapping of income deciles into income terciles.

6. ^Data on per capita GDP came from the IMF's World Economic Outlook (WEO) and are expressed in current prices (U.S. dollars per capita).

7. ^Data are available at: https://eu-dashboards.sdgindex.org/downloads. For details about the Sustainable Development Goals Index see: https://eu-dashboards.sdgindex.org/downloads.

8. ^However, literature alert about the so-called “social desirability bias” present in survey data (Nederhof, 1985). In some circumstances respondents give the most socially acceptable answer than their real point of view. Moreover, social desirability bias tends to be country and cultural dependent. According to McCulloch et al. (2021) giving respondents the possibility to refuse or shift the questions (as in the EVS survey) alleviates the social desirability bias.

9. ^Consolidated literature shows that ignoring the ordinality of the life satisfaction data does not affect the results (Ferrer-i-Carbonell and Frijters, 2004).

References

Abun, D., and Racoma, A. (2017). Environmental attitude and environmental behavior of catholic colleges' employees in Ilocos Sur, Philippines, Texila Int. J. Acad. Res. 4, 1–30. doi: 10.21522/TIJAR.2014.04.01.Art003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajzen, I., and Fishbein, M. (1975). A Bayesian analysis of attribution processes. Psychol. Bull. 82, 261–277. doi: 10.1037/h0076477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aknin, L. B., Van de Vondervoort, J. W., and Hamlin, J. K. (2018). Positive feelings reward and promote prosocial behavior. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, 55–59. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreoni, J. (1990). Impure altruism and donations to public goods: a theory of warm-glow giving. Econ. J. 100, 464–477. doi: 10.2307/2234133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axelrod, L. J., and Lehman, D. R. (1993). Responding to environmental concerns: what factors guide individual action? J. Environ. Psychol. 13, 149–159. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80147-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balderjahn, I. (1988). Personality variables and environmental attitudes as predictors of ecologically responsible consumption patterns. J. Bus. Res. 17, 51–56. doi: 10.1016/0148-2963(88)90022-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bamberg, S., and Möser, G. (2007). Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: a new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 27, 14–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjørnskov, C. (2003). The happy few: Cross–country evidence on social capital and life satisfaction. Kyklos 56, 3–16. doi: 10.1111/1467-6435.00207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Kasser, T. (2005). Are psychological and ecological well-being compatible? The role of values, mindfulness, and lifestyle. Soc. Indic. Res. 74, 349–368. doi: 10.1007/s11205-004-8207-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, P. J., and Scott, K. (2006). Environmental concern and behaviour in an Australian sample within an ecocentric-anthropocentric framework. Aust. J. Psychol. 58, 56–67. doi: 10.1080/00049530600730419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chanda, R. (1999). Correlates and dimensions of environmental quality concern among residents of an African subtropical city: Gaborone, Botswana. J. Environ. Educ. 30, 31–39. doi: 10.1080/00958969909601868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciziceno, M., and Pizzuto, P. (2022). Life satisfaction and tax morale: the role of trust in government and cultural orientation. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 97, 101824. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2021.101824

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corral-Verdugo, V., Bonnes, M., Tapia-Fonllem, C., Fraijo-Sing, B., Frías-Armenta, M., and Carrus, G. (2009). Correlates of pro-sustainability orientation: the affinity towards diversity. J. Environ. Psychol. 29, 34–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coşkun, A, Polonsky, M., and Vocino, A. (2022), Pro-environmental purchase intentions in a low-involvement context: the role of myopia and apathy. J. Glob. Responsibility (ahead-of-print). doi: 10.1108/JGR-04-2022-0034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cottrell, S. P. (2003). Influence of sociodemographics and environmental attitudes on general responsible environmental behavior among recreational boaters. Environ. Behav. 35, 347–375. doi: 10.1177/0013916503035003003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., Oishi, S., and Suh, E. M. (2002). Looking up and looking down: weighting good and bad information in life satisfaction judgments. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 437–445. doi: 10.1177/0146167202287002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietz, T., Kalof, L., and Stern, P. C. (2002). Gender, values, and environmentalism. Soc. Sci. Q. 83, 353–364. doi: 10.1111/1540-6237.00088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietz, T., Rosa, E. A., and York, R. (2007). Driving the human ecological footprint. Front. Ecol. Environ. 5, 13–18. doi: 10.1890/1540-9295(2007)513:DTHEF2.0.CO;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durkheim, E. (1951[1897]). Suicide: A Study in Sociology [1897]. Translated by JA Spaulding and G. Simpson. Glencoe, Illinois: The Free Press 1951.

Google Scholar

Easterlin, R. A. (1995). Will raising the incomes of all increase the happiness of all? J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 27, 35–47. doi: 10.1016/0167-2681(95)00003-B

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eom, K., Kim, H. S., Sherman, D. K., and Ishii, K. (2016). Cultural variability in the link between environmental concern and support for environmental action. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1331–1339. doi: 10.1177/0956797616660078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

EVS. (2022). European Values Study 2017: Integrated Dataset (EVS 2017). ZA7500 Data file Version 5.0.0. Cologne: GESIS. doi: 10.4232/1.13897

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A., and Frijters, P. (2004). How important is methodology for the estimates of the determinants of happiness? Econ. J. 114, 641–659. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2004.00235.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A., and Gowdy, J. M. (2007). Environmental degradation and happiness. Ecol. Econ. 60, 509–516. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2005.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujita, F., and Diener, E. (2005). Life satisfaction set point: stability and change. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 158. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gifford, R., and Nilsson, A. (2014). Personal and social factors that influence pro-environmental concern and behaviour: a review. Int. J. Psychol. 49, 141–157. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldy, S. P., and Piff, P. K. (2020). Toward a social ecology of prosociality: why, when, and where nature enhances social connection. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 32, 27–31. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grilli, L., and Rampichini, C. (2014). “Ordered logit model,” in Encyclopedia of Quality of Life and Well-Being Research, ed A. C. Michalos (Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands), 4510–4513. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-0753-5_2023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helliwell, J. F., and Putnam, R. D. (2004). The social context of well–being. Phil. Trans. Royal Soc. London. B.: Biol. Sci. 359, 1435–1446. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinesa, J. M., Hungerforda, H. R., and Tomerab, A. N. (1987). Analysis and synthesis of research on responsible environmental behavior: A meta-analysis. J. Environ. Educ. 18, 1–8. doi: 10.1080/00958964.1987.9943482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hiratsuka, J., Perlaviciute, G., and Steg, L. (2018). Testing VBN theory in Japan: relationships between values, beliefs, norms, and acceptability and expected effects of a car pricing policy. Transport. Res. F.: Traffic Psychol. Behav. 53, 74–83. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2017.12.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inglehart, R. (1995). Public support for environmental protection: objective problems and subjective values in 43 societies. PS: Polit. Sci. Polit. 28, 57–72. doi: 10.1017/S1049096500056080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inglehart, R. (1997). “The trend towards postmaterialist values continues, in Citizen Politics in Post-industrial Societies, eds T. Clark Nichols and M. Rempel (Boulder and Oxford: Westview Press), 57–66.

Google Scholar

IPCC (2022). Climate Change 2022: Impacts, Adaptation and Vulnerability. Contribution of Working Group II to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Kasser, T. (2017). Living both well and sustainably: a review of the literature, with some reflections on future research, interventions and policy. Phil. Trans. Royal Soc. A.: Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 375, 20160369. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2016.0369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knez, I. (2016). Is climate change a moral issue? Effects of egoism and altruism on pro-environmental behavior. Curr. Urban Stud. 4, 157–174. doi: 10.4236/cus.2016.42012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kollmuss, A., and Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 8, 239–260. doi: 10.1080/13504620220145401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopnina, H. (2012). The Lorax complex: deep ecology, ecocentrism and exclusion. J. Integr. Environ. Sci. 9, 235–254. doi: 10.1080/1943815X.2012.742914

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroll, C. (2011). Different things make different people happy: examining social capital and subjective well-being by gender and parental status. Soc. Indic. Res. 104, 157–177. doi: 10.1007/s11205-010-9733-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroll, C. (2014). Towards a sociology of happiness: the case of an age perspective on the social context of well-being. Soc. Res. Online 19, 1–18. doi: 10.5153/sro.3205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kushlev, K., Radosic, N., and Diener, E. (2020). Subjective Well-being and Prosociality Around the Globe: Happy People Give More of their Time and Money to Others (Preprint). doi: 10.31234/osf.io/7wzan

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenzen, M., and Cummins, R. A. (2011). Lifestyles and well-being versus the environment. J. Industr. Ecol. 15, 650–652. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-9290.2011.00397.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macias, T. (2016). Convergent links of social capital, sustainable practices, and support for cost-specified climate policy in two Vermont and Arizona Counties. Environ. Sociol. 2, 220–232. doi: 10.1080/23251042.2016.1144253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCulloch, N. E. I. L., Moerenhout, T., and Yang, J. (2021). Building a social contract? Understanding tax morale in Nigeria. J. Dev. Stud. 57, 226–243. doi: 10.1080/00220388.2020.1797688

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nederhof, A. J. (1985). Methods of coping with social desirability bias: a review. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 15, 263–280. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420150303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nixon, H., Saphores, J. D. M., Ogunseitan, O. A., and Shapiro, A. A. (2009). Understanding preferences for recycling electronic waste in California: the influence of environmental attitudes and beliefs on willingness to pay. Environ. Behav. 41, 101–124. doi: 10.1177/0013916507310053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolan, P., and Lenski, G. (2004). Human Societies: An Introduction to Macrosociology. Boulder, CO: Paradigm Publishers.

Google Scholar

Owen, A. L., and Videras, J. (2006). Civic cooperation, pro-environment attitudes, and behavioral intentions. Ecol. Econ. 58, 814–829. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2005.09.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, T. (1967). Durkheim's Contribution to the Theory of Integration of Social Systems. Sociological Theory and Modern Society. New York: Free Press, 3–34.

Google Scholar

Prati, G., Albanesi, C., and Pietrantoni, L. (2017). Social well-being and pro-environmental behavior: a cross-lagged panel design. Hum. Ecol. Rev. 23, 123–139. doi: 10.22459/HER.23.01.2017.07

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pretty, J., and Ward, H. (2001). Social capital and the environment. World Dev. 29, 209–227. doi: 10.1016/S0305-750X(00)00098-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnam, R. D. (1993). Making Democracy Work. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1515/9781400820740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling Alone: The collapse and Revival of American community. New York: Simon and Schuster. doi: 10.1145/358916.361990

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rehdanz, K., and Maddison, D. (2008). Local environmental quality and life satisfaction in Germany. Ecol. Econ. 64, 787–797. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2007.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scannell, L., and Gifford, R. (2013). The role of place attachment in receptivity to local and global climate change messages. Environ. Behav. 45, 60–85. doi: 10.1177/0013916511421196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlegelmilch, B. B., Bohlen, G. M., and Diamantopoulos, A. (1996). The link between green purchasing decisions and measures of environmental consciousness. Eur. J. Mark. 30, 35–55. doi: 10.1108/03090569610118740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, P. W. (2000). Empathizing with nature: the effects of perspective-taking on concern for environmental issues. J. Soc. Issues 56, 391–406. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, P. W. (2001). The structure of environmental concern: concern for self, other people, and the biosphere. J. Environ. Psychol. 21, 327–339. doi: 10.1006/jevp.2001.0227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H. (1977). “Normative influences on altruism,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, ed L. Berkowitz (New York, NY: Academic), 221–279. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60358-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? J. Soc. Issues 50, 19–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1994.tb01196.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H. (1996). “Value priorities and behavior: applying a theory of integrated value systems,” in The Psychology of Values: The Ontario Symposium, Volume 8, eds C. Seligman, J. M. Olson, and M. P. Zanna (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 1–24.

Google Scholar

Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J., and Warshaw, P. R. (1988). The theory of reasoned action: a meta-analysis of past research with recommendations for modifications and future research. J. Consum. Res. 15, 325–343. doi: 10.1086/209170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Signoretta, P., Bracke, P., and Buffel, V. (2020). “Fiddling while Rome burns”: The role of ecological states in the association between greenhouse gas emissions and subjective well-being. Front. Sociol. 5, 11. doi: 10.3389/fsoc.2020.00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steg, L., and De Groot, J. (2010). Explaining prosocial intentions: testing causal relationships in the norm activation model. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 49, 725–743. doi: 10.1348/014466609X477745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano, G. A., and Kalof, L. (1999). A value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: the case of environmentalism. Res. Hum. Ecol. 6, 81–97.

Google Scholar

Suárez-Varela, M., Guardiola, J., and González-Gómez, F. (2016). Do pro-environmental behaviors and awareness contribute to improve subjective well-being? Appl. Res. Qual. Life 11, 429–444. doi: 10.1007/s11482-014-9372-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, J. D., Mathevet, R., Delanoë, O., Gil-Fourrier, C., Bonnin, M., and Cheylan, M. (2011). Ecological solidarity as a conceptual tool for rethinking ecological and social interdependence in conservation policy for protected areas and their surrounding landscape. C. R. Biol. 334, 412–419. doi: 10.1016/j.crvi.2011.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, S. C. G., and Barton, M. A. (1994). Ecocentric and anthropocentric attitudes toward the environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 14, 149–157. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80168-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tov, W., and Diener, E. (2009). “The well-being of nations: linking together trust, cooperation, and democracy,” in The Science of Well-being. Social Indicators Research Series, Vol 37, ed E. Diener (Dordrecht: Springer), 9–41. doi: 10.1007/978-90-481-2350-6_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turaga, R. M. R., Howarth, R. B., and Borsuk, M. E. (2010). Pro-environmental behavior: rational choice meets moral motivation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1185, 211–224. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05163.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veenhoven, R. (2000). Views on happiness in early sociology. Introduction to a special issue. J. Happ. Stud. 1, 419–421. doi: 10.1023/A:1011515813942

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venhoeven, L. A., Bolderdijk, J. W., and Steg, L. (2013). Explaining the paradox: how pro-environmental behaviour can both thwart and foster well-being. Sustainability 5, 1372–1386. doi: 10.3390/su5041372

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welsch, H., and Kühling, J. (2010). Pro-environmental behavior and rational consumer choice: evidence from surveys of life satisfaction. J. Econ. Psychol. 31, 405–420. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2010.01.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, E. O. (1984). Biophilia. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. doi: 10.4159/9780674045231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiang, P., Zhang, H., Geng, L., Zhou, K., and Wu, Y. (2019). Individualist–collectivist differences in climate change inaction: the role of perceived intractability. Front. Psychol. 10, 187. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yildiz, T., and Erciş, A. (2022). Investigation of green consumers' subjective well-being within the scope of purchase behavior, attitudes and personality traits. J. Econom. Admin. Sci. 36, 29–42. doi: 10.54614/TBE.2022.881257

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

York, R., Rosa, E. A., and Dietz, T. (2003). Footprints on the earth: the environmental consequences of modernity. Am. Sociol. Rev. 68, 279–300. doi: 10.2307/1519769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelenski, J. M., and Desrochers, J. E. (2021). Can positive and self-transcendent emotions promote pro-environmental behavior? Curr. Opin. Psychol. 42, 31–35. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sociology, environmentalism, wellbeing, social capital, civicness

Citation: Ciziceno M (2023) People's wellbeing, civic capital and sustainable practices: Evidence from the European Values Study survey. Front. Sociol. 7:1048397. doi: 10.3389/fsoc.2022.1048397

Received: 30 September 2022; Accepted: 06 December 2022;
Published: 04 January 2023.

Edited by:

Vincenzo Auriemma, University of Salerno, Italy

Reviewed by:

Ivana Ferrara, Ordine Degli Psicologi Della Campania, Italy
Valeria Saladino, University of Cassino, Italy
Ihsana Sabriani Borualogo, Universitas Islam Bandung, Indonesia

Copyright © 2023 Ciziceno. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marco Ciziceno, yes marco.ciziceno@unipa.it

ORCID: Marco Ciziceno orcid.org/0000-0001-8183-5699

Download