Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 18 June 2025

Sec. Nutritional Epidemiology

Volume 12 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnut.2025.1569850

Maintaining long-term frequent tea consumption could reduce the risk of cognitive decline: results from a 10-year longitudinal study

Jie Huang,&#x;Jie Huang1,2Hongfen Wang&#x;Hongfen Wang3Yuying Wang,Yuying Wang1,2Jiaqi Xu,Jiaqi Xu1,2Jiameng Ni,Jiameng Ni1,2Mengjie Li,Mengjie Li1,2Bin ChenBin Chen4Zaixiang Tang,
Zaixiang Tang1,2*Yu Zhang
Yu Zhang5*Liping Tan
Liping Tan1*
  • 1The Second Affiliated Hospital of Soochow University, Department of Biostatistics, School of Public Health, Suzhou Medical College of Soochow University, Suzhou, Jiangsu, China
  • 2Jiangsu Key Laboratory of Preventive and Translational Medicine for Major Chronic Non-Communicable Diseases, MOE Key Laboratory of Geriatric Diseases and Immunology, Suzhou Medical College of Soochow University, Suzhou, Jiangsu, China
  • 3Department of Neurology, The First Center of the PLA General Hospital, Beijing, China
  • 4Suzhou National New and Hi-tech Industrial Development Zone Center for Disease Control and Prevention, Suzhou, Jiangsu, China
  • 5School of Urban Governance and Public Affairs, School of Wellness Industry, Suzhou City University, Suzhou, Jiangsu, China

Background: Previous studies have suggested that tea may have neuroprotective effect. This study aimed to investigate the association between tea consumption frequency and cognitive decline, taking into account changes in consumption habits and selection bias.

Methods: This study used data from the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS) in 2008, 2011, 2014, and 2018 years. Changes in tea consumption habits were identified based on the frequency and consistency of tea consumption at baseline and at the last follow-up. Cognitive function was assessed by using Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Cox proportional hazards models were conducted to estimate association between tea consumption habits and cognitive decline. Inverse probability of treatment weighting (IPTW) was applied to reduce selection bias.

Results: Consistently frequent tea consumption reduced the risk of cognitive decline, whereas inconsistently frequent tea consumption did not. Compared with consistently infrequent tea drinkers, the multivariable-adjusted HR of cognitive decline was 0.98 (95% CI: 0.90, 1.06) for inconsistently frequent tea drinkers, and 0.86 (95% CI: 0.76, 0.96) for consistently frequent tea drinkers. With increasing frequency of tea consumption, the risk of developing cognitive decline decreased (P for trend: 0.023). Similarly, the association remained robust after IPTW adjustment. Consistently frequent tea drinkers had a 12% (HR:0.88, 95% CI: 0.77, 0.99) reduced risk of cognitive decline, whereas no significant association was observed for inconsistently frequent tea consumption (HR:0.98, 95% CI:0.91, 1.07) (P for trend < 0.001). Additionally, frequent consumption of both tea and fruit has a synergistic effect on cognitive health (P for interaction = 0.041).

Conclusion: Maintaining an uninterrupted habit of frequent tea consumption over time could reduce the risk of cognitive decline, whereas inconsistently frequent tea consumption was not significant, even after IPTW adjustment. Regular consumption of tea and fruit has a synergistic effect on cognitive health.

1 Introduction

The decline in cognitive function leading to dementia has become one of the leading causes of disability and death among older individuals worldwide (1, 2). A recent study suggested the number of people with dementia worldwide will increase from 57.4 million cases in 2019 to 152.8 million in 2050 (3). Due to the complexity and heterogeneity of dementia, treatment options are limited (4). Therefore, preventing and delaying the development of the disease in its early stages is particularly crucial. Cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment, as potential precursors of dementia (such as Alzheimer’s disease), can present early clinical indications (5). In recent years, a body of accumulating evidence has indicated a correlation between dietary habits and cognitive function (6).

Tea originated in China and has become popular all over the world. Tea contains a number of bioactive components with health benefits, such as catechins (especially epigallocatechin gallate [EGCG]), theaflavins, theanine, and caffeine (7). The neuroprotective mechanisms of tea are complex, with catechins generally considered to be the key compounds. In vivo and in vitro studies have shown that EGCG crosses the blood–brain barrier (8, 9). Long-term administration of catechins prevented oxidative stress-related brain aging in mouse models (10). At the same time, a study has suggested that long-term administration may prevent age-related spatial learning and memory decline in mice by regulating hippocampal cAMP-response element binding protein signaling cascade (11). In addition, theanine is a unique amino acid in tea that can exert neuroprotective effects by inhibiting glutamate receptors in rat models (12).

However, population-based prospective studies on the association between tea consumption and cognitive decline are still underdeveloped. Fewer than 10 prospective studies have been conducted and reported contrary results (6, 13). Two of them from China (14, 15), two from the Japan (16, 17), and one each from Singapore (18), the United States (19), and Germany (20). A study from Singapore suggested that tea consumption was associated with a lower risk of cognitive decline, and a dosage-response relationship existed (18). Two Japanese studies both showed green tea was associated with lower risk of cognitive decline (16, 17). In contrast, a prospective study conducted in eastern China showed that tea consumption was not associated with cognitive decline (15). Similarly, no association was found between green or black tea and cognitive decline among older adults in Germany (20) or the United States (19).

Frequency and duration of consumption were important factors influencing tea effects. However, tea drinking habits may change over time. Most previous studies have considered only baseline tea consumption information, ignoring the potential impact of longitudinal changes on cognitive function. This may not reflect their substantive associations. Therefore, a more refined assessment of cumulative exposure is more accurate for clarifying the causal relationship between tea consumption and cognitive decline. In addition, inverse probability of treatment weighting (IPTW) is a method based on propensity scores. The propensity score methods aim to approximate randomization (i.e., pseudo-randomization) by compensating for the different probabilities of participants being assigned to exposure, which could reduce the bias in estimating treatment effects and the likelihood of confounding in non-randomized observational data (21, 22). IPTW provides an unbiased estimate of the outcome by generating a sample with a distribution of potential confounders unrelated to exposure, thereby making the results more credible (22).

Therefore, this study investigated the causal relationship between tea consumption habits and cognitive decline by assessing cumulative exposure through refined measurements, and further validated the findings using the IPTW method.

2 Methods

2.1 Study participants

The data used in this study came from the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS), an ongoing, nationwide longitudinal study. The CLHLS employed multistage, stratified cluster sampling to randomly select counties and cities in 23 of 31 provinces, roughly covering more than 85% of the Chinese population. Beginning in 1998, CLHLS conducted eight waves of surveys, with follow-up surveys every 2 to 3 years (wave 1 to 8: 1998, 2000, 2002, 2005, 2008, 2011, 2014, and 2018). More details of the study have been published elsewhere (23, 24). The survey was conducted through face-to-face interviews with structured questionnaires completed by trained professional investigators.

Because participants’ heights were measured beginning in 2008, this study used 4 waves of longitudinal data from 2008, 2011, 2014, and 2018 years, with 2008 year as the baseline. Participants were included if they (1) had at least one follow-up visit, (2) had complete tea and cognition information at baseline and last survey, and (3) had no cognitive impairment at baseline. Due to the small percentage of missing key covariates, participants with missing data were directly excluded. Ultimately, this study included 6,641 participants aged 60 years and older. Supplementary Figure 1 demonstrates the detailed flowchart of the subjects in the study.

2.2 Tea consumption habits

Each survey collected information on the current frequency of tea drinking. The response was categorized into three groups: almost daily (≥1 cup/day), occasionally (<1 cup/day but ≥1 cup/month), and rarely or never (<1 cup/month or never drinks tea) (25). To further reflect the uniformity of tea consumption habits in subsequent years, we created a new variable. Tea consumption frequency was first divided into frequently (almost daily) and infrequently (occasionally and rarely or never). The frequency was then categorized into three groups based on the frequency of tea consumption at baseline and last follow-up survey (26). Participants were defined as “consistently infrequent drinkers” if they drank tea infrequently both at baseline and last follow-up survey, which served as the reference group in the analysis. In contrast, they were defined as “consistently frequent drinkers” if they drank frequently both at baseline and last follow-up. In between, they were defined as “inconsistently frequent drinkers” if they drank tea frequently/infrequently at baseline, but infrequently/frequently at last follow-up survey. The relevant definitional transitions are shown in Figure 1. The study used 2008 as the baseline and followed participants until the following occurred: cognitive decline, loss to follow-up, death, or study endpoint, whichever came first.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Definitional conversion of tea consumption habit.

2.3 Cognitive function

Cognitive function was assessed by the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) at baseline and each follow-up survey. This is an internationally standardized assessment tool for use in clinical cognitive screening tests (27). The Chinese version of the MMSE has been adapted for Chinese and has been shown to be valid and reliable in previous studies (24). The 24-item questionnaire assessed cognitive function across five dimensions: orientation, registration, attention and calculation, recall, and language. The total score ranges from 0 to 30, with higher scores representing better cognitive function (28). We defined the “Unable to answer” as “fault” (29). Based on previous literature, individuals with a baseline MMSE score of 18 or less were excluded, as they were considered to have severe cognitive impairment (30, 31). Cognitive decline was defined as a reduction of more than 3 points at follow-up (32, 33).

2.4 Covariates

Based on previous studies, we selected sociodemographic characteristics, lifestyle, and health status at baseline as potential confounders (14, 18, 28, 29). Covariates included age (continuous), gender (male/female), residence (urban/rural), education level (years of schooling: 0 year/ ≥1 year), household income (< 10,000/10,001–50,000/> 50,000 yuan), living state [living with members/living alone), body mass index (BMI, kg/m2; underweight[< 18.5]/normal[18.5–23.9]/overweight[24–27.9]/obese[≥ 28] (34)], current smoking status (yes/no), current drinking alcohol status (yes/no), current exercising status (yes/no), vegetable intake (daily or frequent intake/occasional or rare intake), fruit intake (daily or frequent intake/occasional or rare intake), and self-reported history of chronic diseases: hypertension (with/without/unknown), diabetes (with/without/unknown). In addition, baseline MMSE scores were considered.

2.5 Statistical analyses

Subjects were grouped according to the habits of tea consumption during the follow-up period. The characteristics were compared by using chi-square analysis for categorical variables and analysis of variance (ANOVA) for continuous variables. Cox proportional hazard regression models were performed to calculate hazard ratios (HRs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the association between tea consumption habits and risk of developing cognitive decline. Survival time was defined as the time from baseline to the date of the first occurrence of cognitive decline, loss to follow-up, or study endpoint, whichever came first. The median follow-up time was calculated by the reverse Kaplan–Meier method. The cumulative incidence curves were estimated by Kaplan–Meier method, and differences were tested by log-rank tests. We fitted four models: Model 1 was the unadjusted model; Model 2 adjusted for age (continuous), gender, BMI and residence; Model 3 further controlled for education level, household income, living state, exercising, vegetables and fruit intake based on model 2; Model 4 additionally adjusted for smoking, drinking, hypertension, diabetes, and baseline MMSE score based on model 3.

To obtain more robust results, inverse probability of treatment weighting (IPTW) based on propensity score was used. This method achieves some of the characteristics of a randomized controlled experiment by recalculating individual weights to create a pseudo-population that is almost perfectly balanced (21, 22). Propensity scores were computed using the generalized boosting model (GBM) of the “twang” package in R software (35). We weighted the variables that remained different (p < 0.05) after multivariate regression analysis in the cohort (36). Standardized mean differences (SMDs) were used to assess the balance of the covariates before and after weighting, and they were considered well balanced with absolute values less than 0.1 (22). Doubly robust Cox proportional hazard regression models, incorporating relevant covariates and weights, were used to assess the adjusted effect of tea consumption on cognitive decline.

To further investigate potential effect modification among different subgroups, stratified analyses by age, gender, education, fruit intake, and BMI were conducted within the IPTW-adjusted regression models, as well as the interaction between tea consumption habits and these subgroups. In addition, several sensitivity analyses were implemented based on IPTW adjustment before and after: (1) excluding participants with baseline cognitive scores of 3 or lower (because they were less likely to have experienced cognitive decline); (2) using a decrease in cognitive scores of more than 6 points at follow-up to define cognitive decline (33); (3) excluding participants with cognitive impairment defined as baseline cognitive scores of 24 or less (28); (4) Sleep quality (good/fair/bad) was additionally adjusted. All statistical analyses were performed using R software (version 4.4.0), with two-tailed p < 0.05 indicating statistical significance.

3 Results

3.1 Participant characteristics

Table 1 shows the baseline characteristics of participants by tea consumption habits. Overall, a total of 6,641 participants were included, with a mean age of 80.54 (SD: 10.23) years at baseline, and 49.51% were male. Compared to infrequent tea drinkers, frequent tea drinkers, especially consistent frequent tea drinkers, had higher cognitive scores. Inconsistently frequent and consistently frequent tea drinkers were more likely to be younger, male, educated, exercise regularly, live in urban area with members, have higher incomes, and consume vegetables and fruits frequently, compared to consistently infrequent tea drinkers. However, they were also more likely to smoke, drink alcohol, and have a higher prevalence of hypertension and diabetes. In addition, during a median follow-up period of 6.2 (95% CI: 6.1, 11.2) years, 3,177 (47.8%) participants with normal cognitive function at baseline developed cognitive decline, with a corresponding incidence density of 85.5 (95% CI: 78.8, 98.2) per 1,000 person-years (Table 2). There were 1937 (51.5%), 901 (46.0%) and 339 (36.6%) cognitive decline events among consistent infrequent, inconsistent frequent and consistent frequent tea drinkers, respectively, with corresponding incidence densities of 92.6 (95% CI: 83.3, 101.9), 81.6 (95% CI: 69.5, 93.7), and 65.3 (95% CI: 49.3, 81.2) per 1,000 person-years.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of participants by tea consumption habits.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Incidence densities of cognitive decline development according to tea consumption habits.

3.2 Association between tea consumption habits and cognitive decline

The cumulative incidence curve indicated that consistently frequent tea drinkers had the lowest incidence of cognitive decline, which gradually increased as tea consumption frequency decreased (log-rank test: p < 0.001, Figure 2A). Figure 3A demonstrates that consistent and frequent tea consumption over time was significantly associated with a reduced risk of cognitive decline. In the unadjusted model, inconsistently frequent tea drinkers and consistently frequent tea drinkers had a 13% (HR:0.87, 95% CI: 0.80, 0.94) and 30% (HR: 0.70, 95% CI: 0.62, 0.79) reduced risk of cognitive decline than consistently infrequent drinkers, respectively (Supplementary Table 1). After controlling for sociodemographic, lifestyle, and health status variables, the magnitude of the associations decreased. In fully adjusted models (Model 4), the HR with 95% CI for consistently frequent tea drinkers was 0.86 (95% CI: 0.76, 0.96). However, did not observe an association between inconsistently frequent tea drinkers and cognitive decline. The frequency of tea consumption showed a significant linear trend with cognitive decline (P for trend = 0.023). The results suggested that under certain conditions, the higher the frequency of consumption, the stronger the potential protective effect may be. In addition, we found that the relationship between consistently frequent tea consumption and cognitive decline was statistically significant across all four models. Therefore, tea consumption could be considered an independent protective factor against cognitive decline, but it requires maintaining a habit of long-term and frequent tea drinking.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Cumulative incidence curve for cognitive decline according to tea consumption frequency before (A) and after IPTW adjustment (B). aThe p-values was calculated by log-rank test.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Association between tea consumption habits and cognitive decline before (A) and after IPTW adjustment (B)a. HR, hazard ratio; CI, confidence interval; IPTW, inverse probability of treatment weighting. aModel 1 was unadjusted; Model 2 adjusted for age, gender, BMI and residence; Model 3 additional adjustments for education level, household income, living state, exercising, vegetables and fruit intake upon model 2; Model 4 additional adjustments for smoking, drinking, hypertension, diabetes, and baseline MMSE score upon model 3. The IPTW analysis was based on model 4, adjusting for variables that remain different after multivariate adjustment: age, gender, BMI, education level, exercising, fruit intake, diabetes, baseline MMSE score, and weights.

3.3 Inverse probability of treatment weighting adjustment

In order to obtain more robust results, we further adjusted for variables that have a potential impact on the effect in the cohorts. Supplementary Table 2 shows the baseline characteristics before and after IPTW adjustment. After adjusting for IPTW, these features were well balanced, and the standardized mean differences (SMD) were all less than 0.1 (Supplementary Figure 2). Figure 2B illustrates the IPTW adjusted cumulative incidence curves between different tea frequencies, which still remained significantly different (adjusted log-rank test: p < 0.001).

Maintaining frequent tea consumption over time could reduce the risk of cognitive decline (Figure 3B). Based on the IPTW-adjusted Cox proportional hazards regression model, consistently frequent tea drinkers had a 12% (HR: 0.88, 95% CI: 0.77, 0.99) lower risk of cognitive decline than consistently infrequent tea drinkers (Supplementary Table 3). Similarly, no association was also found between inconsistently frequent tea consumption and cognitive decline. In addition, the risk of cognitive decline decreased with increasing frequency of tea consumption (P for trend < 0.001). The above results all indicated that consistent daily tea consumption over a long period, maintaining good consumption habits, could potentially exert protective effects and reduce the risk of cognitive decline.

3.4 Subgroup and interaction analysis

Based on IPTW-multivariate adjusted models, subgroup analyses of age, gender, BMI, education, and fruit intake were performed (Figure 4). The protective effect of tea appeared to be more pronounced in the subgroups of older, male, and regular fruit intake, except for the BMI ≥ 28 kg/m2 subgroup. In addition, we found a significant interaction between fruit and tea consumption, suggesting that the protective effect was greater in those who frequently consume both fruit and tea (P for interaction: 0.041).

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Association between tea consumption habits and cognitive decline in the subgroups after IPTW adjustmenta. Frequency = 0, consistently infrequent drinker; 1, inconsistently frequent drinker; 2, consistently frequent drinker. HR, hazard ratio; CI, confidence interval. aBased on model 4 adjusted for variables that remain different after multivariate adjustment: age, gender, BMI, education level, exercising, fruit intake, diabetes, baseline MMSE score, and weights. *p < 0.05.

3.5 Sensitivity analysis

To obtain more robust findings, multiple sensitivity analyses before and after IPTW adjustment were conducted. Firstly, we excluded participants with baseline cognitive scores of 3 or less (Supplementary Table 4). Second, cognitive decline was defined by a reduction in cognitive scores of more than 6 points at follow-up (Supplementary Table 5). Third, cognitive impairment defined as a baseline cognitive score of 24 or lower was excluded (Supplementary Table 6). Finally, sleep quality was included (Supplementary Table 7). Similar results were obtained before and after the corresponding IPTW adjustment.

4 Discussion

In this large population study of Chinese adults aged 60 and older, we found that consistently maintaining frequent tea consumption reduced the risk of cognitive decline during 10 years of follow-up, whereas inconsistently frequent tea consumption did not. This protective effect remained robust after adjusting for IPTW. In addition, the interaction analysis revealed that frequent intake of tea and fruit had a synergistic effect on reducing cognitive decline.

Ng et al. revealed that higher frequency of tea consumption was associated with lower risk of cognitive decline among Chinese Singaporeans (18). In a prospective study implemented by Li et al. with a one-year follow-up, it was shown that drinking tea was associated with a lower incidence of cognitive impairment, and prevented declines in memory and associative learning by affecting the volume of the posterior corpus callosum among Chinese older adults (14). Both Noguchi-Shinohara et al. and Shirai et al. (16, 17) suggested that green tea consumption was associated with a reduced risk of cognitive decline in Japanese older adults. These findings are partially consistent with the present study.

It is worth noting that previous studies have only considered baseline tea consumption information and ignored changes in tea drinking habits. Therefore, the present study developed a new measurement method that assessed cumulative exposure using baseline and last follow-up tea consumption information and consistency, to identify changes in tea drinking habits. The study suggested that consistently frequent tea consumption could reduce the risk of cognitive decline, whereas inconsistent and frequent tea consumption did not. This suggested that the protective effect of tea on cognitive function may be realized in the form of long-term accumulation. In other words, it is more likely to benefit from drinking tea every day without interruption for a long period of time. This finding provided a more precise and reliable conclusion on the effect of tea consumption frequency on cognitive decline, and helps to advance the field of research focusing on the dose of tea intake.

In contrast, a prospective study conducted in Shanghai, China, did not find an association between tea consumption and cognitive decline (15). The reasons for this heterogeneity may be due to the relatively short follow-up period of just 2 years, during which the benefits of tea consumption may not have manifested. Furthermore, previous research was solely based on the urban community in Shanghai, whereas the current study was a nationwide survey that included both urban and rural areas. In addition, two prospective studies in the United States and Germany reported no effect of black and green tea consumption on cognitive decline (19, 20). Direct comparisons could not be made because the baseline survey for the present study did not have information on the type of tea consumption. In addition, there may have been differences in genetic exposure and tea brewing methods. In Chinese culture, tea is traditionally consumed without milk or sugar, whereas in the West, the addition of additives is common (19). Studies have shown that tea polyphenols interact with milk proteins, especially caseins, to reduce the antioxidant activity of tea (37).

In addition, we found tea consumption appeared to be detrimental to cognitive function in the obese subgroup (BMI ≥ 28 kg/m2). Studies have shown that obesity indices such as BMI, waist circumference, and waist-to-hip ratio were all associated with poorer performance on multiple tests of cognitive function, and there was a cumulative effect among them (38, 39). Generally, obese individuals have multiple obesity indices above the normal range with cumulative effects, and the cognitive benefits of tea may not be enough to offset these deficits. In addition, we also found an interaction between fruit and tea intake. Regular consumption of both fruit and tea has a greater protective effect on cognitive health. A study suggested daily fruit intake reduced risk of cognitive impairment in older Chinese adults (40). A multicenter study from Europe also suggested that regular fruit intake improves cognitive and mental health (41). A study has shown that flavanol-rich extract prepared from lychee fruit and green tea could reduce progressive cognitive and molecular deficits in a triple-transgenic animal model of Alzheimer disease (42). In addition, early-stage AD has been found to be associated with high insulin concentrations in response to glucose challenge (hyperinsulinaemia) in combination with reduced insulin-mediated glucose uptake (insulin resistance) (43). Tea and fruit may affect cognitive function by influencing blood sugar levels and insulin. Therefore, regular intake of tea and fruit may enhance the benefits on cognitive function.

Results from multifactorial Cox proportional risk modeling based on the 2008–2018 cohort have shown that consistently frequent tea consumption reduced the risk of cognitive decline, whereas inconsistently frequent tea consumption was not significant. Due to the non-random assignment of study participants in observational studies, there is selection bias and significant differences in baseline characteristics between groups. In order to reduce selection bias to obtain more reliable results, we used propensity score method. The propensity score methods aim to simulate randomization, attenuate the likelihood of confounding in non-randomized observational studies (21). IPTW was chosen because it uses the entire dataset, increasing the effective sample size (21). Therefore, we further adjusted all covariates that could confound the association after multivariate regression in the cohort and important covariates, such as age and gender. In fact, they themselves were significant. After IPTW adjustment, all covariates were balanced with the standardized mean difference being less than 0.1. The protective effect of tea consumption against cognitive decline remained robust. All results indicated that the protective effect of tea against cognitive decline requires a long-term commitment to drinking tea daily and maintaining an unbroken habit.

As mentioned earlier, the neuroprotective mechanisms of tea are complex. A growing number of studies have explored the potential mechanisms of tea’s effects on neurodegenerative diseases (7). Studies have shown that excessive reactive oxygen species (ROS) production and accumulation of defective mitochondria in brain cells, were associated with neurodegenerative diseases. Whereas catechins, especially epigallocatechin gallate (EGCG), were strong antioxidants that scavenge free radicals (44). Administration of catechins to rats prevented amyloid-induced cognitive impairment and reduced lipid peroxide and ROS levels in both hippocampus and plasma by 20% (45). In addition, it involves the modulation of cell signaling pathways and metal chelator activity (7). Theaflavins, which are mainly found in black tea, can protect neuronal cells (PC12) from H2O2-induced apoptosis and exert neuroprotective effects (46). In addition, caffeine is known for its effects on attention, improving cognitive and physical function by blocking adenosine receptors (47).

Cognitive impairment may also affect eating habits. Studies have shown that low levels of blood carotenoids were found among Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients compared to healthy controls (48). Therapeutic supplements of carotenoids have been proposed as an alternative treatment and prevention strategy for AD (48). Antioxidant therapy has been explored as a new approach to the prevention and treatment of neurodegenerative diseases in which oxidative stress was a contributing factor (49). Natural antioxidants include enzymatic and non-enzymatic antioxidants, such as polyphenols, vitamins, minerals and carotenoids (49). Tea is rich in non-enzymatic antioxidants and may be recommended as an effective food for people with early-stage dementia. In addition, dietary treatments have been proposed (50).

This study has several strengths. First, instead of relying solely on baseline tea consumption information, we developed a more refined measurement method, considering that tea drinking habits may change over time, which was more conducive in clarifying the causal relationship between tea consumption and cognitive decline. It indicated that the protective effect of tea on cognitive function is realized through long-term accumulation, and promoted a focus on cumulative exposure. Second, we controlled for as many confounders as possible, including using traditional multivariate adjustments and IPTW adjustments, and the results remained robust. Last, this study was based on a nationally representative cohort of older adults with a large sample size and a long follow-up period. It has favorable applicability in China.

There are also several limitations in this study. First, unmeasured and residual confounders may still be present. Second, tea consumption frequency was self-reported by participants, recall bias may exist. Third, although the MMSE has been well validated in assessing cognition, there was a lack of pathophysiologic assessment of cognitive function in the community-based survey. Some studies have used 28 or 24 points as the initial cut-off point with 2 or 6 points per decline as the threshold for moderate and severe cognitive impairment, respectively (51, 52). Therefore, in this study, we used 3 and 6 points as thresholds to reduce the possibility of misclassification, respectively. More clinical trials are needed to validate the results of this study. Finally, due to lack of data, we could not obtain the exact quantity and type of tea intake. Green tea contains rich catechins, while in black, oolong and dark teas catechins are converted to theaflavins, thearubigins and theabrownins (53). Different types of tea have varying bioactive components, which may have varying effects on cognitive decline. More information needs to be collected in the future for further research.

5 Conclusion

Long-term consistently frequent tea consumption was associated with a reduced risk of cognitive decline in older Chinese adults, while inconsistently frequent tea consumption was not associated, even after IPTW adjustment. In addition, there may be a synergistic effect between regular tea drinking and fruit consumption on cognitive health. Advocating long-term frequent tea consumption and maintaining an unbroken habit could promote neurocognitive health.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Biomedical Ethics Committee of Peking University (IRB00001052-13074). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JH: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing – original draft. HW: Conceptualization, Project administration, Validation, Writing – review & editing. YW: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. JX: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. JN: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. ML: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. BC: Data curation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. ZT: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. YZ: Data curation, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. LT: Data curation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (82373688 and 81773541), and funded by the Priority Academic Program Development of Jiangsu Higher Education Institutions at Soochow University, the State Key Laboratory of Radiation Medicine and Protection (GZK1201919), Jiangsu Key Laboratory of Preventive and Translational Medicine for Geriatric Diseases (KJS2222), Suzhou Key Laboratory of Neuro-oncology and Nano-bionics (SZZD003), 2024 Suzhou Science and Science Resources Project “Health Guide for the Elderly in Suzhou under the Background of Healthy China Strategy (Jiangnan Culture Seasonal Food and Nutrition)” (No.: ZYKF2024-20), and 2024 Suzhou Social Science Popularization and Huimin Support Project “Suzhou “One Old and One Young” Suzhou Flavor Seasonal Food and Nutritional Recipes” Health Popularization Reading (No. 100). The funding body did not play any role in the design or conduct of this research.

Acknowledgments

We thank the staff and the participants of the CLHLS study. We are grateful to the Center for Healthy Aging and Development Studies and Peking University for managing the CLHLS study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2025.1569850/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

CLHLS, Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; IPTW, Inverse probability of treatment weighting; HR, Hazard ratio; CI, Confidence interval; EGCG, Epigallocatechin gallate; BMI, Body mass index; PS, Propensity scores; SMD, Standardized mean differences; ROS, Reactive Oxygen Species; AD, Alzheimer’s disease.

References

1. GBD 2016 Neurology Collaborators. Global, regional, and national burden of neurological disorders, 1990-2016: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2016. Lancet Neurol. (2019) 18:459–80. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(18)30499-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Gale, SA, Acar, D, and Daffner, KR. Dementia. Am J Med. (2018) 131:1161–9. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2018.01.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. GBD 2019 Dementia Forecasting Collaborators. Estimation of the global prevalence of dementia in 2019 and forecasted prevalence in 2050: an analysis for the global burden of disease study 2019. Lancet Public Health. (2022) 7:e105–25. doi: 10.1016/s2468-2667(21)00249-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Rosenberg, A, Mangialasche, F, Ngandu, T, Solomon, A, and Kivipelto, M. Multidomain interventions to prevent cognitive impairment, Alzheimer's disease, and dementia: from FINGER to world-wide FINGERS. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. (2020) 7:29–36. doi: 10.14283/jpad.2019.41

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Levey, A, Lah, J, Goldstein, F, Steenland, K, and Bliwise, D. Mild cognitive impairment: an opportunity to identify patients at high risk for progression to Alzheimer's disease. Clin Ther. (2006) 28:991–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.clinthera.2006.07.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Rajaram, S, Jones, J, and Lee, GJ. Plant-based dietary patterns, plant foods, and age-related cognitive decline. Adv Nutr. (2019) 10:S422–s436. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmz081

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Chen, SQ, Wang, ZS, Ma, YX, Zhang, W, Lu, JL, Liang, YR, et al. Neuroprotective effects and mechanisms of tea bioactive components in neurodegenerative diseases. Molecules. (2018) 23:512. doi: 10.3390/molecules23030512

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lin, LC, Wang, MN, Tseng, TY, Sung, JS, and Tsai, TH. Pharmacokinetics of (−)-epigallocatechin-3-gallate in conscious and freely moving rats and its brain regional distribution. J Agric Food Chem. (2007) 55:1517–24. doi: 10.1021/jf062816a

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Pogačnik, L, Pirc, K, Palmela, I, Skrt, M, Kim, KS, Brites, D, et al. Potential for brain accessibility and analysis of stability of selected flavonoids in relation to neuroprotection in vitro. Brain Res. (2016) 1651:17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.09.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Li, Q, Zhao, H, Zhao, M, Zhang, Z, and Li, Y. Chronic green tea catechins administration prevents oxidative stress-related brain aging in C57BL/6J mice. Brain Res. (2010) 1353:28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.07.074

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Li, Q, Zhao, HF, Zhang, ZF, Liu, ZG, Pei, XR, Wang, JB, et al. Long-term administration of green tea catechins prevents age-related spatial learning and memory decline in C57BL/6 J mice by regulating hippocampal cyclic amp-response element binding protein signaling cascade. Neuroscience. (2009) 159:1208–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.02.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Zukhurova, M, Prosvirnina, M, Daineko, A, Simanenkova, A, Petrishchev, N, Sonin, D, et al. L-theanine administration results in neuroprotection and prevents glutamate receptor agonist-mediated injury in the rat model of cerebral ischemia-reperfusion. Phytother Res. (2013) 27:1282–7. doi: 10.1002/ptr.4868

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Ma, QP, Huang, C, Cui, QY, Yang, DJ, Sun, K, Chen, X, et al. Meta-analysis of the association between tea intake and the risk of cognitive disorders. PLoS One. (2016) 11:e0165861. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165861

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Li, W, Yue, L, and Xiao, S. Prospective associations of tea consumption with risk of cognitive decline in the elderly: a 1-year follow-up study in China. Front Nutr. (2022) 9:752833. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.752833

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Wang, G, Tang, H-D, Zhuang, J-P, Xu, X-H, Liu, L-H, Li, B, et al. Risk factors for cognitive decline in elderly people: findings from the two-year follow-up study in Shanghai Urban Community. J Alzheimers Dis. (2014) 39:891–7. doi: 10.3233/JAD-131514

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Noguchi-Shinohara, M, Yuki, S, Dohmoto, C, Ikeda, Y, Samuraki, M, Iwasa, K, et al. Consumption of green tea, but not black tea or coffee, is associated with reduced risk of cognitive decline. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e96013. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Shirai, Y, Kuriki, K, Otsuka, R, Kato, Y, Nishita, Y, Tange, C, et al. Green tea and coffee intake and risk of cognitive decline in older adults: the National Institute for longevity sciences, longitudinal study of aging. Public Health Nutr. (2020) 23:1049–57. doi: 10.1017/s1368980019002659

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Ng, TP, Feng, L, Niti, M, Kua, EH, and Yap, KB. Tea consumption and cognitive impairment and decline in older Chinese adults. Am J Clin Nutr. (2008) 88:224–31. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/88.1.224

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Feng, L, Langsetmo, L, Yaffe, K, Sun, Y, Fink, HA, Shikany, JM, et al. No effects of black tea on cognitive decline among older US men: a prospective cohort study. J Alzheimers Dis. (2018) 65:99–105. doi: 10.3233/jad-180103

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Fischer, K, Melo van Lent, D, Wolfsgruber, S, Weinhold, L, Kleineidam, L, Bickel, H, et al. Prospective associations between single foods, Alzheimer's dementia and memory decline in the elderly. Nutrients. (2018) 10:852. doi: 10.3390/nu10070852

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Chesnaye, NC, Stel, VS, Tripepi, G, Dekker, FW, Fu, EL, Zoccali, C, et al. An introduction to inverse probability of treatment weighting in observational research. Clin Kidney J. (2022) 15:14–20. doi: 10.1093/ckj/sfab158

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Haukoos, JS, and Lewis, RJ. The propensity score. JAMA. (2015) 314:1637–8. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.13480

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Lv, YB, Gao, X, Yin, ZX, Chen, HS, Luo, JS, Brasher, MS, et al. Revisiting the association of blood pressure with mortality in oldest old people in China: community based, longitudinal prospective study. BMJ. (2018) 361:k2158. doi: 10.1136/bmj.k2158

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Zeng, Y, Feng, Q, Hesketh, T, Christensen, K, and Vaupel, JW. Survival, disabilities in activities of daily living, and physical and cognitive functioning among the oldest-old in China: a cohort study. Lancet. (2017) 389:1619–29. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(17)30548-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Shen, K, Zhang, B, and Feng, Q. Association between tea consumption and depressive symptom among Chinese older adults. BMC Geriatr. (2019) 19:246. doi: 10.1186/s12877-019-1259-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ruan, R, Feng, L, Li, J, Ng, TP, and Zeng, Y. Tea consumption and mortality in the oldest-old Chinese. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2013) 61:1937–42. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12498

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Folstein, MF, Folstein, SE, and McHugh, PR. Mini-mental state. J Psychiatr Res. (1975) 12:189–98. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Zhang, Q, Wu, Y, Han, T, and Liu, E. Changes in cognitive function and risk factors for cognitive impairment of the elderly in China: 2005-2014. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:847. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16162847

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Lv, X, Li, W, Ma, Y, Chen, H, Zeng, Y, Yu, X, et al. Cognitive decline and mortality among community-dwelling Chinese older people. BMC Med. (2019) 17:63. doi: 10.1186/s12916-019-1295-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Du, M, Tao, L, Zhu, L, and Liu, J. Association between biomass fuel use and the risk of cognitive impairment among older populations in China: a population-based cohort study. Environ Health. (2021) 20:21. doi: 10.1186/s12940-021-00706-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Zhu, A, Chen, H, Shen, J, Wang, X, Li, Z, Zhao, A, et al. Interaction between plant-based dietary pattern and air pollution on cognitive function: a prospective cohort analysis of Chinese older adults. Lancet Reg Health West Pac. (2022) 20:100372. doi: 10.1016/j.lanwpc.2021.100372

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Nilsson, H, Sanmartin Berglund, J, and Renvert, S. Longitudinal evaluation of periodontitis and development of cognitive decline among older adults. J Clin Periodontol. (2018) 45:1142–9. doi: 10.1111/jcpe.12992

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Xu, T, Ye, X, Lu, X, Lan, G, Xie, M, Huang, Z, et al. Association between solid cooking fuel and cognitive decline: three nationwide cohort studies in middle-aged and older population. Environ Int. (2023) 173:107803. doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2023.107803

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Huang, Y, Chen, H, Gao, M, Lv, X, Pang, T, Rong, S, et al. Self-and interviewer-reported cognitive problems in relation to cognitive decline and dementia: results from two prospective studies. BMC Med. (2024) 22:23. doi: 10.1186/s12916-023-03147-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. McCaffrey, DF, Griffin, BA, Almirall, D, Slaughter, ME, Ramchand, R, and Burgette, LF. A tutorial on propensity score estimation for multiple treatments using generalized boosted models. Stat Med. (2013) 32:3388–414. doi: 10.1002/sim.5753

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Xiang, X, Hu, D, Jin, Z, Liu, P, and Lin, H. Radiofrequency ablation vs. surgical resection for small early-stage primary intrahepatic cholangiocarcinoma. Front Oncol. (2020) 10:540662. doi: 10.3389/fonc.2020.540662

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Rashidinejad, A, Birch, EJ, Sun-Waterhouse, D, and Everett, DW. Addition of milk to tea infusions: helpful or harmful? Evidence from in vitro and in vivo studies on antioxidant properties. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. (2017) 57:3188–96. doi: 10.1080/10408398.2015.1099515

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Gunstad, J, Lhotsky, A, Wendell, CR, Ferrucci, L, and Zonderman, AB. Longitudinal examination of obesity and cognitive function: results from the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Neuroepidemiology. (2010) 34:222–9. doi: 10.1159/000297742

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Liu, Z, Yang, H, Chen, S, Cai, J, and Huang, Z. The association between body mass index, waist circumference, waist–hip ratio and cognitive disorder in older adults. J Public Health. (2018) 41:305–12. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdy121

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Xu, M, Ke, P, Wang, C, Xia, W, Meng, X, Di, H, et al. Association of food groups with the risk of cognitive impairment in Chinese older adults. J Affect Disord. (2022) 309:266–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.04.113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Gehlich, KH, Beller, J, Lange-Asschenfeldt, B, Köcher, W, Meinke, MC, and Lademann, J. Consumption of fruits and vegetables: improved physical health, mental health, physical functioning and cognitive health in older adults from 11 European countries. Aging Ment Health. (2020) 24:634–41. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2019.1571011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Chen, X, Benhong, X, Luling, N, Kaiwu, H, Li, Z, Xinfeng, H, et al. Flavanol-rich lychee fruit extract substantially reduces progressive cognitive and molecular deficits in a triple-transgenic animal model of Alzheimer disease. Nutr Neurosci. (2021) 24:720–34. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2019.1673527

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Camfield, DA, Owen, L, Scholey, AB, Pipingas, A, and Stough, C. Dairy constituents and neurocognitive health in ageing. Br J Nutr. (2011) 106:159–74. doi: 10.1017/S0007114511000158

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Afzal, O, Dalhat, MH, Altamimi, ASA, Rasool, R, Alzarea, SI, Almalki, WH, et al. Green tea Catechins attenuate neurodegenerative diseases and cognitive deficits. Molecules. (2022) 27:604. doi: 10.3390/molecules27217604

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Haque, AM, Hashimoto, M, Katakura, M, Hara, Y, and Shido, O. Green tea catechins prevent cognitive deficits caused by Abeta 1-40 in rats. J Nutr Biochem. (2008) 19:619–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2007.08.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Zhang, J, Cai, S, Li, J, Xiong, L, Tian, L, Liu, J, et al. Neuroprotective effects of Theaflavins against oxidative stress-induced apoptosis in PC12 cells. Neurochem Res. (2016) 41:3364–72. doi: 10.1007/s11064-016-2069-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. McLellan, TM, Caldwell, JA, and Lieberman, HR. A review of caffeine's effects on cognitive, physical and occupational performance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 71:294–312. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Varesi, A, Campagnoli, LIM, Carrara, A, Pola, I, Floris, E, Ricevuti, G, et al. Non-enzymatic antioxidants against Alzheimer's disease: prevention, diagnosis and therapy. Antioxidants. (2023) 12:180. doi: 10.3390/antiox12010180

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Collins, AE, Saleh, TM, and Kalisch, BE. Naturally occurring antioxidant therapy in Alzheimer's disease. Antioxidants. (2022) 11:213. doi: 10.3390/antiox11020213

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Yuan, S, Jiang, SC, Zhang, ZW, Fu, YF, Yang, XY, Li, ZL, et al. Rethinking of Alzheimer's disease: lysosomal overloading and dietary therapy. Front Aging Neurosci. (2023) 15:1130658. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1130658

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Kuriyama, S, Hozawa, A, Ohmori, K, Shimazu, T, Matsui, T, Ebihara, S, et al. Green tea consumption and cognitive function: a cross-sectional study from the Tsurugaya project. Am J Clin Nutr. (2006) 83:355–61. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/83.2.355

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Tombaugh, TN, and McIntyre, NJ. The mini-mental state examination: a comprehensive review. J Am Geriatr Soc. (1992) 40:922–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb01992.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Tang, GY, Meng, X, Gan, RY, Zhao, CN, Liu, Q, Feng, YB, et al. Health functions and related molecular mechanisms of tea components: an update review. Int J Mol Sci. (2019) 20:6196. doi: 10.3390/ijms20246196

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: tea, frequency, habits, cognitive function, inverse probability of treatment weighting

Citation: Huang J, Wang H, Wang Y, Xu J, Ni J, Li M, Chen B, Tang Z, Zhang Y and Tan L (2025) Maintaining long-term frequent tea consumption could reduce the risk of cognitive decline: results from a 10-year longitudinal study. Front. Nutr. 12:1569850. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2025.1569850

Received: 02 February 2025; Accepted: 04 June 2025;
Published: 18 June 2025.

Edited by:

Raul Zamora-Ros, Institut d’Investigacio Biomedica de Bellvitge (IDIBELL), Spain

Reviewed by:

Hakan Apaydın, Hittite University, Türkiye
Shihua Shi, ETH Zürich, Switzerland

Copyright © 2025 Huang, Wang, Wang, Xu, Ni, Li, Chen, Tang, Zhang and Tan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Zaixiang Tang, dGFuZ3p4QHN1ZGEuZWR1LmNu; Yu Zhang, emhhbmd5dUBzemN1LmVkdS5jbg==; Liping Tan, dGxwaW5nNjdAMTI2LmNvbQ==

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.