Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 20 August 2025

Sec. Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health

Volume 12 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnut.2025.1571677

Early-life nutritional supplementation protects against home environmental risks in Ghanaian children's social-emotional development


Lindsey C. Partington,
Lindsey C. Partington1,2*Haiying Yuan,Haiying Yuan1,3Maku E. DemuyakorMaku E. Demuyakor4Seth Adu-AfarwuahSeth Adu-Afarwuah5Amanda E. Guyer,Amanda E. Guyer2,6Kathryn G. DeweyKathryn G. Dewey1Sika M. KumordzieSika M. Kumordzie1Brietta M. OaksBrietta M. Oaks7Charles D. ArnoldCharles D. Arnold1Elizabeth L. Prado&#x;Elizabeth L. Prado1Paul D. Hastings,,Paul D. Hastings1,2,8
  • 1Institute for Global Nutrition, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, United States
  • 2Center for Mind & Brain, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, United States
  • 3Texas Health and Human Services Commission, Austin, TX, United States
  • 4McKing Consulting Corporation, Atlanta, GA, United States
  • 5Department of Nutrition and Food Science, University of Ghana, Accra, Ghana
  • 6Department of Human Ecology, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, United States
  • 7Department of Nutrition, University of Rhode Island, Kingston, RI, United States
  • 8Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, United States

Introduction: This study examines how an early-life small-quantity lipid-based nutrient supplement (LNS) interacts with the home environment to influence Ghanaian children's social-emotional functioning at preschool age.

Method: In a randomized controlled trial, 1,320 Ghanaian women received either daily LNS, multiple micronutrients (MMN) or iron and folic acid (IFA) during pregnancy through 6 months postpartum. Infants in the LNS group received the daily supplement from 6 to 18 months. The IFA and MMN groups were combined to be the control group. At 4–6 years (n = 966), we assessed children's home environments (caregiver responsivity, negative behavior acceptance, physical environment, learning materials, academic stimulation) and their social-emotional strengths and difficulties.

Results: Both LNS and higher quality home environments—particularly caregiver responsivity and negative behavior acceptance—predicted children having fewer social-emotional difficulties. LNS supplementation moderated relations between the home environment and children's hyperactivity/inattention and total difficulties. Living in a safer physical environment was related to fewer hyperactivity/inattention problems and total difficulties only for children who received LNS. Less academic stimulation was related to more hyperactivity/inattention only for children in the control group.

Conclusion: Early-life LNS supplementation and supportive home environments reduce social-emotional difficulties in Ghanaian children. Nutritional interventions are most effective in the context of safe, stimulating households, demonstrating that integrated interventions support children's wellbeing in low- and middle-income countries.

Clinical trial registration: clinicaltrials.gov; identifier: NCT00970866.

Introduction

An estimated 250 million children under 5 years of age in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) are at risk of not fulfilling their developmental potential due to a combination of nutritional, psychosocial, and poverty-related risk factors (1, 2). Approximately 26% of young children in LMICs fail to meet social-emotional milestones by age 4 (3), placing them at heightened risk for poor mental health later in life (4, 5). Children in LMICs are disproportionately exposed to undernutrition (6, 7) and low-quality home environments (e.g., few learning opportunities, physically unsafe structures, lack of responsive caregiving) (1, 8), which together elevate children's social-emotional risk (9). Children with strong social-emotional skills—such as prosocial behavior—can better manage their stress and more effectively engage in learning (10, 11), whereas social-emotional difficulties—such as emotional problems, conduct issues, hyperactivity and peer relationship problems—are associated with poor academic performance and increased mental health risks (1215). Children's social-emotional difficulties are both prevalent and understudied in LMICs (3). Investigating how nutrition and specific home environment factors jointly influence children's social-emotional development is critical for designing effective interventions in low and middle income settings (5, 1618).

Animal and human studies demonstrate that both nutritional deficiencies and low-quality, under-stimulating environments disrupt similar neurodevelopmental processes (e.g., reduced dendritic branching, altered synaptic density, and impaired myelination), leading to negative impacts on physical, motor, cognitive, and social-emotional development in early life and highlighting the need to consider both nutrition and home context when evaluating child development (19, 20). While the developmental risks from undernutrition are well-documented (18, 2123), there is considerable variability in how children respond to environmental risk. Some children display resilience, developing strong social-emotional competencies despite adversity, whereas others exhibit greater social-emotional difficulties (24). Resilience frameworks have traditionally focused on fixed traits like temperament or genetics (2528), with limited research on modifiable resilience factors—like early nutrition interventions—that could buffer children against the negative effects of low-quality home environments. Investigating these interactive effects on social-emotional development can help identify modifiable factors that buffer children against adverse conditions in LMICs and can better inform integrated intervention design.

Systematic reviews and meta-analytic evidence support independent and additive effects of adequate nutrition and stimulating home environments in promoting a broad range of child developmental outcomes (e.g., physical, motor, cognitive, language, and social development) for children in LMICs (18, 2932). Indeed, recent meta-analytic evidence shows that integrated interventions targeting both nutrition and psychosocial stimulation (i.e., interventions targeting sensory intervention that the child receives from social and environmental interactions) have more pronounced language and cognitive benefits for undernourished children as compared to nutrition supplementation only (33). However, it is unclear which specific aspects of integrated interventions are uniquely associated with positive outcomes. Identifying the key elements of effective integrated interventions is crucial for designing high-quality, scalable, and context-specific interventions for children living in LMICs (17, 32, 33).

Small-quantity lipid-based nutrient supplements (LNS) are a recommended preventative strategy for addressing maternal and child undernutrition and optimizing children's health, growth, and development in LMICs (34) with meta-analytic evidence suggesting that small-quantity LNS for children 6–23 months of age reduces the risk for child stunting, wasting, anemia, mortality and developmental delay (35). The International Lipid-Based Nutrient Supplements (iLiNS) DYAD-Ghana trial—a randomized controlled trial of pre- and postnatal small-quantity LNS supplementation—was designed to evaluate the efficacy of LNS in preventing multiple nutritional limitations common in LMICs, such as micronutrient deficiencies, inadequate intake of essential fatty acids (EFAs), and deficiencies in several key macrominerals (36), and in preventing nutrition-related risks (e.g., low birth weight, stunting, wasting, and development delay) (13).

By targeting both prenatal and postnatal periods, the LNS intervention was hypothesized to enhance developmental trajectories during the first 1,000 days—a sensitive window for promoting long-term cognitive and socio-emotional outcomes (19, 22, 23). Maternal and infant diets during the first 1,000 days frequently do not provide adequate iron, zinc, vitamin A, and essential fatty acids, especially for women and children living in LMICs; therefore, daily SQ-LNS during this sensitive window is recommended to meet these nutrient requirements and promote optimal growth and developmental outcomes, especially for children in low-resource settings (37). The supplement provided a small daily dose (20 g/day) of energy-dense food (118 kcal/d, 2.6 g protein/d) that included a full complement of micronutrients, and four macrominerals not usually present in multiple micronutrient supplements (calcium, phosphorus, potassium, and magnesium) in combination with EFAs (e.g., α-linolenic acid, a precursor to DHA). Detailed descriptions of the LNS composition have been published elsewhere (36, 38, 39) and are presented in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Nutrient and energy contents of the iron and folic acid capsule, multiple micronutrient supplement capsule, and small-quantity lipid-based nutrient supplement used in the international lipid-based nutrient supplements (iLiNS) DYAD Ghana trial.

In previous work, we found that young Ghanaian children whose mothers were randomized to receive LNS during pregnancy and for the first 6 months postpartum, and who themselves received LNS from 6 to 18 months of age, had significantly lower overall social-emotional difficulties at 4–6 years of age, compared to children in the control group (40). Furthermore, the effect of LNS on social-emotional difficulties was greater among children living in lower quality home environments compared to children in higher quality homes. These findings suggested that early-life nutritional supplementation might be a modifiable resilience factor, protecting Ghanaian children's social-emotional development in lower quality home environments.

Building upon this, the current study had three aims. First, we examined which specific social-emotional outcomes were associated with early-life nutritional supplementation to better understand how LNS may influence social-emotional strengths (i.e., prosocial behavior) and social-emotional difficulties (i.e., emotional, conduct, peer relationship, hyperactivity/inattention problems, and total difficulties). Second, we examined which specific aspects of the home environment were associated with specific social-emotional strengths and difficulties in this cohort. We built upon our previous work by investigating how to meaningfully characterize the Ghanaian home environment, adapting the subscales of the Early Childhood Home Observation for the Measurement of Environment (EC-HOME) inventory (41, 42) to account for Ghanaian culture and provide a more accurate measure of specific environmental factors relevant to Ghanaian child development. Third, we examined whether early-life nutritional supplementation modified the associations between specific aspects of the home environment and social-emotional strengths and difficulties and the extent to which LNS buffered against social-emotional risk in lower quality home environments.

Method and materials

Study design and participants

Between 2009 and 2014, the iLiNS-DYAD-Ghana trial was conducted in semi-urban communities in the Eastern Region of Ghana. A total of 1,320 pregnant women were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 daily intervention arms from enrollment to delivery: (1) 60 mg of iron plus 400 μg of folic acid [iron and folic acid (IFA) group: n = 441]; (2) multiple micronutrient capsule containing 18 vitamins and minerals [multiple micronutrients (MMN) group: n = 439]; and (3) LNS with similar micronutrients as the MMN supplement, plus other minerals and macronutrients (LNS group: n = 440). LNS was a suspension-based colloid and was not microencapsulated. Detailed information about LNS formulation and acceptability in pregnant and lactating Ghanaian women and their infants has been published elsewhere (43, 44). After birth, women in the MMN and LNS groups continued to receive the same supplements until 6 months postpartum, whereas the control IFA group received a calcium placebo capsule (200 mg/d) during that period. Children in the LNS group received LNS designed for children from 6 to 18 months of age, whereas children in the other 2 groups received no supplement (36). Details of the study profile are presented in Figure 1, and detailed information about the nutrient and energy contents of the study's dietary supplements are in Table 1. Maternal and child biological samples (blood, urine, and breastmilk) were collected for biomarker assays, and detailed protocols and results are available elsewhere (4549). In this analysis, we considered the LNS group as the intervention group and the IFA and MMN groups were combined to be the control group, as previous analyses have not shown significant differences between the IFA and MMN groups in social-emotional outcomes (40).

Figure 1
Flowchart shows the process of a study with pregnant women. Out of 1,575 recruited, 1,320 were enrolled. Participants received either IFA, MMN, or LNS supplements. Reasons for ineligibility and re-enrollment numbers at age four to six are detailed, showing losses at each stage. Final testing included 299, 327, and 340 children for each group respectively.

Figure 1. Study profile of the international lipid-based nutrient supplements (iLiNS) DYAD Ghana trial. Study profile showing infants whose mothers were enrolled into the trails, and reasons for participants being lost to follow-up. IFA, iron and folic acid capsule; MMN, multiple micronutrient supplement capsule; LNS, lipid-based nutrient supplement. In the LNS group, women received 20g LNS daily during pregnancy and 6 mo postpartum; infants received 20g LNS daily from 6 to 18mo of age. In the Control group (i.e., IFA or MMN), women received either IFA during pregnancy and placebo for 6 mo postpartum or MMN capsules during pregnancy and 6 mo postpartum. Infants in the Control group did not receive any supplement. Groups shown are based on supplements women received at enrollment.

Between January and December 2016, all parents or caregivers of children who had participated in the iLiNS-DYAD-Ghana trial were contacted for enrollment in the follow-up study. During this period, children who had been born during the trial were 4–6 years of age. We re-enrolled 1,014 children whose mothers or caregivers provided informed consent to participate, and we obtained data from 966 participants (40). Ethical approval for this follow-up study was obtained from the Institutional Review Board of the University of California, Davis, the Ethics Committee for the College of Basic and Applied Sciences of the University of Ghana, and the Ghana Health Service Ethical Review Committee.

Data collection

Detailed descriptions of the study procedures for the randomized trial and the follow-up study—including randomization procedures and pre-intervention covariate balance checks—have been published elsewhere (36, 40). Information relevant to this secondary analysis is summarized here. At enrollment into the original trial, maternal and household information, including maternal education and household assets, was collected by trained fieldworkers using a questionnaire. In 2016, field staff visited participants' homes to explain the follow-up study, obtain consent for participation, and collect updated sociodemographic information. Consenting participants were scheduled to visit the test center for neurodevelopmental measures. An additional home visit was conducted to administer the EC-HOME to measure various aspects of the home environment.

Measures

EC-HOME inventory

We measured children's home environment using the EC-HOME, which we adapted to assess the quality of children's home environment in the local context. The adapted version of the EC-HOME included 46 items scored on a binary (Yes-No) scale to measure children's home environment. Details on how the EC-HOME was adapted and used to assess the quality of children's home environment in the local context have been previously published (40). Past research with the EC-HOME has used eight subscales that characterize the quality of the home environment based on Learning Materials, Language Stimulation, Physical Environment, Caregiver Responsivity, Academic Stimulation, Desirable Behavior Modeling, Family Lifestyle Variety, and Negative Behavior Acceptance. However, those subscales were developed using samples representative of young children in the United States (41, 42). Considering the cultural and language differences, the same subscales may be inappropriate to measure home environments in Ghana. Therefore, we conducted an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to investigate the underlying factors of the adapted version of EC-HOME in the local context in Ghana. After performing the EFA, we dropped 14 items, resulting in an adapted scale with 32 items and 5 factors (see Supplementary material S1 for details).

A confirmatory factor analysis showed that the 5-factor model fit our data well [χ(496)2 = 921.98, CFI = 0.97, RMSEA = 0.04]. The Cronbach's alpha of the five new subscales (ranged from 0.43 to 0.79) and the mean item-total correlations of those subscales (ranged from 0.45 to 0.83) were improved from the Cronbach's alpha of the eight standard subscales (ranged from 0.14 to 0.69) and their mean item-total correlations (ranged from 0.45 to 0.69) originally calculated with our sample (Supplementary Table S2). Collectively, these results suggested that the following 5 subscales meaningfully characterized the Ghanaian home environment: Learning Materials (10 items, “Child has toys or games or posters which help teach letters/alphabet”), Physical Environment (i.e., a more physically safe household free of common building hazards that provides a clean living space; 3 items, “Outside play environment appears safe”), Caregiver Responsivity (8 items, “Parent hugs, kisses, or holds child during visit”), Academic & Language Stimulation (7 items, “Child is encouraged to learn numbers”), and Negative Behavior Acceptance (i.e., avoiding punitive control; parent's ability to accept negative behavior from the child as something to be expected from young children rather than as an act demanding immediate harsh reprisal, 4 items, “Parent does not use physical restraint during visit”). All items for each subscale are presented in Supplementary Tables S1, S2.

Strengths and difficulties questionnaire

We assessed child social-emotional competence and difficulties by caregiver interview using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (50). The SDQ is comprised of 25 items divided into 5 subscales: (1) Emotional Symptoms, (2) Conduct Problems, (3) Hyperactivity/inattention, (4) Peer Relationship Problems, and (5) Prosocial Behavior. Each item is scored on a 3-point scale with 0 = “not true,” 1 = “somewhat true,” and 2 = “certainly true.” We translated the measure into the local languages. Subscale scores can be computed by adding scores on relevant items (after recoding reversed items; range 0–10). Cronbach's alpha for the total difficulties score was 0.62, and for the subscales ranged from 0.66 to 0.77 (see Supplementary Table S2 for further detail).

Clinical cut-off scores for the subscales are: Emotional Symptoms ≥5, Conduct Problems ≥4, Hyperactivity/Inattention ≥7, Peer Relationship Problems ≥4, and Prosocial Behavior ≤ 4. The subscales 1–4 are added to generate a “Total Difficulties” scale (range 0–40) with the clinical cut-off score being ≥17. Importantly, the SDQ is not a clinical diagnostic tool and cannot confirm or rule out a mental health diagnosis. The SDQ has been used as a screening instrument, with clinical cut-off scores indicating elevated and potentially clinically concerning behavior (12, 14). Of note, SDQ clinical cut-off scores have been normed in predominantly high-income countries (51), and there is mixed evidence supporting these clinical cut-off scores as valid for African children (4). For our analyses, we calculated clinical cut-offs for descriptive purposes, to more comprehensively characterize children in the LNS and control groups. Raw scale scores were used for hypothesis testing.

Covariates

We controlled for data collector, child age, gender, years of maternal education, maternal age, household assets index, maternal depression, and maternal agency in hypothesis testing analyses. All maternal covariates were measured at baseline. Detailed descriptions of the creation of these variables have been published elsewhere (36, 40), and brief descriptions of questionnaire-based covariates are provided here.

Home assets index

We created a proxy indicator for household socioeconomic status for each household based on ownership of assets (e.g., radio, television), lighting sourcing, drinking water supply, sanitation facilities, and flooring materials. Using principal components analysis, household ownership of these assets was combined into an index with a mean of zero and standard deviation of 1. Higher values represent higher socioeconomic status.

Maternal depression

We used the 10-item Edinburg Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) to measure maternal depressive symptoms at baseline, which has been validated among both postpartum and non-postpartum women (52). Mothers rated items on how frequently they had experienced the described symptom in the past week (e.g., “I have felt sad or miserable”). Mothers rated each item on a 4-point scale, and 7 items were reverse-scored. Items were summed together, and total scores could range from 0 to 30. Higher EPDS scores indicate more depressive symptoms, with scores ≥12 suggesting severe depressive symptoms.

Maternal agency

We used the 10-item General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSES) to measure maternal agency and general sense of perceived self-efficacy at baseline (53). Mothers rated how well an item described them (e.g., “I can usually handle whatever comes my way”) on a 4-point scale. Items were summed together with higher GSES scores reflecting more maternal agency and self-efficacy (range 10 to 40).

Statistical analysis

Study pre-registration

We posted a statistical analysis plan with pre-specified potential covariates to the OSF website (https://osf.io/bmv9d/). Analyses were performed using R version 4.4.1. The R package “lavaan” (54) was used to run multi-group comparison of the path analyses. Results with a p-value < 0.05 were considered significant, and results with a p-values of < 0.10 were considered marginally significant.

Attrition and missing data

Of the 1,320 women enrolled in the trial, 966 children were assessed in the follow-up study. Only four children had completely missing EC-HOME and SDQ measures; they were excluded from hypothesis testing analyses. Children included in this analysis (n = 962) did not differ from those excluded (n = 358) in background characteristics such as child sex, maternal education, household asset scores, maternal depression, and maternal agency (p's > 0.13). However, a greater proportion of children included in the analysis were in the LNS group (35.1%) compared to LNS representation in those excluded from analyses (28.7%; p = 0.03), which indicates more children in the control group were lost to follow up compared to the LNS group (Supplementary Table S3). Little's Missing Completely at Random test suggested that data could be treated as missing completely at random, χ(247)2 = 269.97, p = 0.15.

Hypothesis testing

As part of preliminary data screening, we assessed univariate normality for all continuous variables by examining skewness and kurtosis values. All variables fell within acceptable thresholds for univariate normality, defined as skew values within ±2 and kurtosis values within ±7 (55). To test Aims 1 and 2, we fit six path models regressing the EC-HOME subscales, nutrition group (dummy-coded, 1 = LNS, 0 = Control), and covariates onto each SDQ Subscale Score: Total Difficulties, Emotional Problems, Conduct Problems, Peer Relationship Problems, Hyperactivity/Inattention, and Prosocial Behavior. Figure 2 provides a conceptual figure of these path models. Variables with significant zero-order correlations were covaried in each path model. For Aim 3, we used multiple group comparison to examine the potential moderating effect of early-life nutritional supplementation (LNS vs. control group) on the association of each EC-HOME subscale with each of the six continuous SDQ subscale scores. First, we fit a model removing nutrition group as a predictor and then freely estimating all parameters for both nutrition groups. Next, a second model was fit constraining all paths between the EC-HOME subscales and the SDQ subscale to be equal across nutrition groups, and a chi-square goodness of fit was performed to determine whether the constrained model significantly worsened model fit. If the unconstrained model was supported, the Wald test with cross-group equality constraints for each regression path was used to test for treatment group difference in path coefficients.

Figure 2
Flowchart illustrating the influence of various factors on the SDQ Subscale Score. Factors include Caregiver Responsivity, Negative Behavior Acceptance, Physical Safety, Learning Materials, and Academic & Language Stimulation, all with red arrows pointing to the SDQ Subscale Score. Nutrition Group has a blue arrow pointing to it. Covariates such as Data Collector, Child Age, Gender, and others are in a separate box with a black arrow pointing to the SDQ Subscale Score.

Figure 2. Conceptual figure of model paths illustrating the study aims. Conceptual figure of path models illustrating the study aims. The blue path corresponds to Aim 1, the red paths correspond to Aim 2, and green paths correspond to Aim 3. Covariances removed to aid in model clarity. Nutrition Group was dummy-coded (1 = LNS, 0 = Control). Six models fit for each of the six SDQ subscale scores. The blue path was removed for multiple group comparison testing for Aim 3.

For all models, we assessed model fit with the chi-square likelihood ratio statistic and these fit index criteria: RMSEA < 0.08, SRMR < 0.08, CFI > 0.90, and TLI > 0.90 (56). It should be noted that SEM analyses with large sample sizes yield the smallest confidence intervals around parameter estimates; subsequently, the likelihood-ratio test becomes overpowered and more likely to reject models due to small differences in residuals despite the tested model being well-specified (57). Hence, the RMSEA, SRMR, CFI, and TLI indices are more discerning for model fit assessment with our sample size. Full information maximum likelihood (FIML) was used to estimate missing data values. We conducted post hoc power analyses and report this information in the Supplementary Table S6.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Table 2 shows the participants' demographic characteristics, EC-HOME subscale raw scores, and the raw scores of the SDQ subscales in the LNS and the control groups. Independent samples t-tests found that participants in the LNS group were from households with a lower mean asset score than the control group (p = 0.02). There were no other significant differences between the two groups (p's ≥ 0.05). Of note, group differences were approaching significance for severe maternal depressive symptoms (total scores ≥ 12), SDQ Peer Relationship Problems, and Total Difficulties. Mothers in the LNS group had a marginally greater prevalence of severe depressive symptoms at baseline, with 25.1% of mothers in the LNS group reporting severe depressive symptoms and 20.4% of mothers in the control group reporting severe depressive symptoms [χ2(1) = 2.80, p = 0.09]. Overall, these findings suggest that the nutritional groups did not substantively differ on background characteristics, despite attrition. For the SDQ subscales, the control group had marginally higher Peer Relationship Problems (p = 0.05) and Total Difficulties scores (p = 0.07).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison of participant characteristics of the LNS group vs. the control group.

Table 3 documents the prevalence of children's social-emotional difficulties above the SDQ clinical cut-offs on the four SDQ problem subscales, the prevalence of low scores on the prosocial scale, and the chi-square test for equality of proportions to test for significant differences in prevalence between the two groups. The LNS group had significantly lower prevalences of conduct problems and total difficulties scores in the clinically concerning range compared to the control group. Finally, we performed zero-order correlations between participants' demographic characteristics, EC-HOME subscale raw scores, and SDQ subscale raw score for the entire sample to identify covariances to include for hypothesis testing. Supplementary material contains a complete description of zero-order correlations (Supplementary Table S4).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Prevalence of children with clinical cut-off scores for social-emotional strengths and difficulties.

Aims 1 and 2: examine how the home environment and early-life nutritional supplementation predict social-emotional outcomes

We fit six path models to examine how the five EC-HOME factors (Aim 1) and nutritional supplementation (Aim 2) predict children's SDQ scores (emotional problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, total difficulties, and prosocial behavior; see Table 4). We fit all six path models with the covariances shown in Supplementary Figure S2, which reflect the significant correlations between participant demographics (Supplementary Table S4). Model fit and unstandardized path estimates for EC-HOME factors predicting SDQ scores for all six models are presented in Table 4. Complete model parameter estimates for all six models are presented in Supplementary Table S5. Holistically, the comparative and absolute fit indices for all six models indicate that our models had good fit despite the significant likelihood-ratio test.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Unstandardized path estimates for early-life nutritional supplementation and home environment factors predictive of Ghanaian children's social-emotional strengths & difficulties.

For Aim 1, we found that children who received LNS had fewer total difficulties (β = −0.064, p = 0.023), fewer peer relationship problems (β = −0.07, p = 0.03), and tended to have fewer conduct problems (β = −0.06, p = 0.07) at age 4–6 years. These associations are aligned with our previous work that found children who received LNS had significantly lower total difficulties' z-scores compared to children in the control group (40). Nutritional supplementation was not significantly associated with children's emotional problems and hyperactivity/inattention, nor was it associated with children's prosocial behavior (p's > 0.32).

In support of Aim 2, distinct and specific aspects of the home environment were uniquely associated with children's SDQ scores at age 4–6, particularly in relation to social-emotional difficulties. More responsive caregiving was associated with fewer conduct problems (β = −0.10, p = 0.001), less hyperactivity/inattention (β = −0.09, p = 0.017), fewer total difficulties (β = −0.09, p = 0.003), and greater prosocial behavior (β = 0.14, p < 0.001). Caregivers who were more accepting of their children's age-appropriate negative behavior (i.e., used less punitive discipline) had children with fewer emotional (β = −0.12 p < 0.001), conduct (β = −0.12, p = 0.001), hyperactivity/inattention (β = −0.11, p < 0.001), and peer relationship problems (β = −0.163, p < 0.001) and fewer total difficulties (β = −0.205, p = 0.001). Children with more learning materials also had fewer conduct problems (β = −0.06, p = 0.04), fewer total difficulties (β = −0.07, p = 0.04), and greater prosocial behavior (β = 0.08, p = 0.03). Children with greater academic and language stimulation had less hyperactivity/inattention (β = −0.08, p =0.03), but contrary to our expectations, more emotional problems (β = 0.09, p = 0.002).

Several of the demographic covariates also were significantly associated with children's SDQ scores. For example, mothers who reported more depression or less agency also reported more difficulties and less prosocial behavior for their children. Older children had better adjustment than younger children. Girls had more emotional problems and prosocial behavior than boys, but fewer hyperactivity/inattention problems.

Aim 3: examine if nutritional supplementation modifies the associations between home environment and children's social-emotional outcomes

Multi-group comparison analysis showed that nutritional supplementation moderated the associations between the home environment and children's hyperactivity/inattention and total difficulties at age 4–6 years (Table 5). Allowing the EC-HOME paths to vary between nutritional supplementation group significantly improved model fit relative to constraining EC-HOME paths to be equal for both groups when predicting children's hyperactivity/inattention [χ2(5) = 15.71, p = 0.01] and when predicting children's total difficulties [χ2(5) = 13.26, p = 0.02]. The chi-square goodness of fit tests were non-significant for all other models (p's > 0.40).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Chi-square goodness of fit tests for multi-group comparison of all models.

Nutrition group moderates the associations between the physical environment, academic stimulation, and children's hyperactivity/inattention

The Wald test with cross-group equality constraints for each EC-HOME regression path demonstrated significant group differences for the physical environment [Δχ2(1) = 10.73, p = 0.001] and for academic and language stimulation path coefficients [Δχ2(1) = 4.95, p = 0.03] predicting children's hyperactivity/inattention. Living in a safer physical environment was associated with less hyperactivity/inattention among children who received LNS (β = −0.16, p = 0.009), whereas the physical environment was not associated with hyperactivity/inattention for the control group (β = 0.08, p = 0.06; Figure 3A). To assist with understanding the nature of the moderating influence of physical safety on the predictive associations of nutrition group with hyperactivity/inattention, Figure 4A displays the mean SDQ hyperactivity/inattention scores for children in the LNS vs. control groups, living in the context of less vs. more physically safe homes. Children in the LNS group who lived in physically safer homes had the fewest hyperactivity/inattention problems.

Figure 3
Two path diagrams with panel A (Hyperactivity & Inattention and Panel B (Total Difficulties). Each diagram displays five home environment predictors - Caregiver Responsivity, Negative Behavior Acceptance, Physical Safety, Access to Learning Materials, and Academic & Language Stimulation - influence children's SDQ Subscale Scores. Coefficients for LNS and Control are printed beside each arrow. Bold path visually emphasize predictors that were meaningfully different between LNS and Control. Panel A bold paths: Physical Safety (LNS −0.33**; Control 0.17†) and Academic & Language Stimulation (LNS 0.06; Control −0.17**). Panel B bold path: Physical Safety (LNS −0.87**; Control 0.23).

Figure 3. Multi-group comparison path models: associations between home environment and children's hyperactivity/inattention and total difficulties are moderated by nutritional supplementation. N = 962. Path models of associations between home environment factors and Ghanaian children's hyperactivity/inattention (A) and total difficulties (B), controlling for child age, child gender, mother age, maternal education, household assets index, and data collectors. Multi-group comparison found that the associations between home environment factors and total difficulties and hyperactivity/inattention vary by nutritional supplement group. Bolded, solid lines reflect paths that were significantly different between groups. Values are unstandardized regression estimates. Covariates and covariances not presented to aid with interpretability. Missing data is estimated with Full Informaiton Maximum Likelihood.p < 0.10, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

Figure 4
Three bar charts with error bars and a legend mapping Control (light gray) and LNS (dark gray). Y axis shows SDQ mean scores; x axis categories are Low vs High for the HOME subscale. Panel A (Hyperactivity/Inattention by Physical Safety): in LNS, the High safety bar is lower than Low; Control bars are similar. Panel B (Hyperactivity/Inattention by Academic & Language Stimulation): at Low stimulation, Control is higher than LNS; at High stimulation, groups are similar. Panel C (Total Difficulties by Physical Safety): both groups are lower at High safety, with little between group difference.

Figure 4. SDQ hyperactivity/inattention and total difficulties mean scores by nutritional supplementation group and HOME subscale scores. Mean SDQ scores by nutritional supplementation group and HOME subscale scores. Error bars show the 95% confidence interval around the mean. (A) SDQ hyperactivity/inattention mean scores for LNS and control children living in Low Physical Safety (scores <3) and High Physical Safety (scores ≥3) homes. (B) SDQ hyperactivity/inattention mean scores for LNS and control children living in Low Stimulation (scores <6) and High Stimulation (scores ≥6) homes. (C) SDQ total difficulties mean scores for LNS and control children living in Low Physical Safety (scores <3) and High Physical Safety (scores ≥3) homes.

Additionally, less academic stimulation was associated with more hyperactivity/inattention among children in the control group (β = −0.12, p = 0.004); but this association was non-significant for children who received LNS (β = 0.04, p = 0.46; Figure 3A). To assist with understanding the nature of the moderating influence of academic and language stimulation on the predictive associations of nutrition group with children's hyperactivity/inattention, Figure 4B displays the mean SDQ hyperactivity/inattention scores for children in the LNS vs. control groups, living in the context of less vs. more academically stimulating homes. Children in the control group who experienced less academic and language stimulation had the most hyperactivity/inattention problems. The Wald test was non-significant for all other EC-HOME variables (p's > 0.15), suggesting that those path weights did not significantly differ between nutritional supplementation groups.

Nutrition group moderates the associations between the physical environment and children's total difficulties

The Wald test with cross-group equality constraints for each EC-HOME regression path showed significant group differences for the physical environment predicting children's total difficulties [Δχ2(1) = 10.64, p = 0.001]. Living in a safer physical environment was associated with fewer total difficulties among children who received LNS (β = −0.18, p = 0.001), whereas the physical environment was not associated with total difficulties for the control group (β = 0.04, p = 0.25; Figure 3B). To assist with understanding the nature of the moderating influence of physical safety on the predictive associations of nutrition group with SDQ problem scores, Figure 4C displays the mean SDQ total problem scores for children in the LNS vs. control groups, living in the context of less vs. more physically safe homes. Children in the LNS group who lived in physically safer homes had the fewest total difficulties. The Wald test was non-significant for all other EC-HOME variables (p's > 0.09), suggesting that those path weights did not significantly differ between nutritional supplementation groups.

Discussion

Children in LMICs face compounded risks, including undernutrition and environmental adversity, both of which elevate the risk for social-emotional difficulties (1, 3, 9). This study examined the main and interactive effects of early-life nutritional supplementation and specific aspects of home environment quality on Ghanaian children's social-emotional functioning. Overall, our findings indicate that early-life nutritional supplementation with small-quantity lipid-based nutrient supplements and multiple aspects of supportive home environments each have a protective effect on social-emotional outcomes, underscoring the importance of both for reducing social-emotional difficulties. Independent associations of LNS and home environment quality were stronger and more prevalent than interactive effects. Yet there also was evidence that early-life LNS enhanced the degree to which growing up in physically safe homes was associated with better psychological adjustment and protected Ghanaian children from hyperactivity/inattention problems associated with experiencing less academic and language stimulation. Hence, our study builds upon previous research by identifying how early-life nutritional supplementation and specific aspects of the home environment relate to specific social-emotional outcomes.

Early-life nutritional supplementation promotes distinct aspects of Ghanaian children's social-emotional functioning

We found that children who received LNS exhibited fewer peer relationship problems and total difficulties, and tended to have fewer conduct problems, while controlling for home quality and background characteristics. Additionally, children who received LNS had a lower prevalence of clinically concerning conduct problems and total social-emotional difficulties, compared to the children in the control group. These findings underscore the potential for nutritional supplementation to reduce social-emotional difficulties, possibly by supporting brain and emotional development through sustained nutrient availability during sensitive developmental periods (19, 22, 58, 59). As posited by Ocansey and colleagues (40), better social-emotional functioning for children in the LNS group may stem from the essential fatty acids and iron present in the supplement that they consumed from ages 6 to 18 months. These nutrients are crucial for neurodevelopmental processes—such as synaptogenesis, myelination, and dopaminergic signaling—that support emotion regulation and executive functioning (19, 60, 61).

The LNS supplement contained 0.5 g of α-linolenic acid (ALA)—a precursor to docosahexaenoic acid (DHA)—and 4.6 g of linoleic acid, and we previously found that mothers who received LNS in our study had significantly higher breastmilk ALA at 6 months postpartum compared to mothers in the IFA and MMN conditions (49). Both animal and human research has found DHA deficiency to negatively impact brain regions related to emotion regulation, with subsequent adverse impacts on learning, behavioral, and emotional outcomes (6164).

Additionally, iron deficiencies during the 6- to 24-month postnatal period can detrimentally alter structural and functional brain development (e.g., dopaminergic pathways, myelination, dendritic growth) (65), contributing to increased risk for social, emotional, and attentional problems in young children (60, 6669). It is interesting that LNS supplementation was not associated specifically with children's hyperactivity/inattention as DHA and iron deficiency have been consistently associated with both concurrent and prospective attentional and hyperactivity problems measured years later (64, 65, 68, 69); however, many of these studies measured attentional and hyperactivity problems exclusively without considering other social-emotional and behavioral comorbidities. Further research is needed to understand the biological mechanisms underlying how early-life LNS supplementation relates to some—but not all—social-emotional outcomes.

Other possible biological mechanisms underlying how LNS may influence children's social-emotional development include changes to the gut-brain axis or epigenetic changes. Emerging research on the gut–brain axis suggests that, in the first 1,000 days of life, shifts in the intestinal microbiota feed back to the central nervous system via bi-directional neural, endocrine, and immune routes, altering the synthesis and turnover of key neurotransmitters (70), which may subsequently influence pathophysiological risk for social-emotional difficulties and related mental health disorders (71). Alternatively, growing evidence suggests that early-life adequacy of folic acid and B vitamin micronutrients—which were present in the LNS supplement—may influence DNA methylation and gene expression with downstream influence on mental health across the lifespan (72). Elucidating these pathways will require future studies that integrate nutritional interventions with microbiome, epigenomic, and neuroimaging measures.

Distinct aspects of the home environment are uniquely related to Ghanaian children's social-emotional functioning

Overall, we found that multiple aspects of a higher-quality home environment—particularly caregiver's behavior—were associated with better social-emotional functioning across a range of outcomes. Caregivers who showed more emotional warmth and verbal responsivity toward their children had children with fewer problems with conduct, hyperactivity/inattention, and overall social-emotional functioning. Additionally, their children showed greater prosociality, a developmental asset associated with academic, physical, and mental wellbeing learning (10, 11). Highly responsive Ghanaian parents model empathy, positive communication, and emotional support for their children and may have more positive parent-child relationships, all of which may help children develop effective emotion regulation and social skills underlying social-emotional competence (73, 74). Ghanaian cultural norms place a strong emphasis on communal interdependence and respect for elders (75); thus, caregiver responsivity may also model and reinforce cooperative social norms and thereby promote children's social-emotional development. These findings are consistent with large-scale, cross-cultural parenting research (76) as well as burgeoning Ghanaian socialization research (77, 78) that suggest caregiver responsivity and warmth to be a “universally beneficial” parenting practice that facilitates children's social-emotional competence.

Similarly, caregivers who demonstrated greater acceptance of children's age-appropriate expressions of negative affect and behaviors and who used less punitive discipline in response to children's negative behavior had children with fewer emotional problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, and total social-emotional difficulties. Almost all the Negative Behavior Acceptance items described caregivers' use of corporal punishment in response to children's misbehavior (e.g., “parents neither slaps nor spanks child during visit”). There is robust meta-analytic and cross-cultural parenting research demonstrating that corporal punishment relates to children's increased short- and long-term risk for behavioral, social, and emotional problems (79, 80), and our findings suggest that Ghanaian parents who refrain from disciplining their children with corporal punishment are also mitigating their children's social-emotional risk. It is possible that, in households with high Negative Behavior Acceptance scores, those parents are using low-power disciplinary strategies, such as inductive reasoning, explaining behavioral expectations, and consistent enforcement of clear rules. These low-power disciplinary strategies provide children with an understanding of the consequences of their behavior and the rationale for why rules are necessary, helping them to internalize social and behavioral norms (81). Indeed, Ghanian parents' greater use of low-power discipline has been related to their children being less aggressive, less depressed, less anxious, and having less peer conflict (77, 78). Nutritional interventions that incorporate parent training should target parents' disciplinary practices.

Regarding the educational climate of the household, children's greater access to learning materials was associated with more favorable social-emotional outcomes whereas the associations of academic and language stimulation within the home to these outcomes were mixed. Children with greater access to learning materials in the home had fewer conduct problems, fewer total difficulties, and greater prosocial behavior, suggesting that educational resources in the household may promote children's positive social-emotional development. Access to learning materials may reduce children's conduct problems and bolster prosocial behaviors by fostering cognitive stimulation, providing fun and interactive activities to practice self-regulation, and promoting positive parent–child interactions through shared activities (e.g., puzzles, story books).

Surprisingly, higher academic and language stimulation was associated with both increased emotional problems and decreased hyperactivity/inattention, suggesting that while academically stimulating home environments may support children's attentional skills, they might also add stress affecting children's emotion regulation. This finding is somewhat inconsistent with research conducted in high-income countries (82) where academically stimulating home environments appear to promote young children's self-regulation (8385) and cooperative, positive peer interactions (86, 87), while also being associated with children's reduced hyperactivity and problem behaviors (84, 88). Research conducted in high-, middle-, and low-income countries suggests that parental academic pressure contributes to children's heightened stress, anxiety, and mental health risk (89, 90). Parents' excessive academic expectations may undermine children's self-esteem, leading them to internalize feelings of failure or shame if they fail to meet parental standards. Alternatively, children with heightened emotional problems may elicit more academic support from their parents. Future research should aim to disentangle the direction of the association.

Our findings align with previous research that emphasizes the protective role of supportive caregiver behaviors in fostering social-emotional competence (9194) and the effectiveness of nutritional interventions when paired with stimulating households (3032, 95). Meta-analytic evidence emphasizes the cumulative, additive benefits of integrating both nutrition and responsive caregiving to promote children's social-emotional, cognitive, and physical development (95, 96). That is, both adequate early-life nutrition and high-quality home environments facilitate children's wellbeing, and interventions that integrate both are particularly well-suited to support children's development. Our results indicate that integrated interventions targeting caregiver responsivity and reducing punitive discipline may be particularly effective as both were robustly associated with multiple social-emotional outcomes. Early-life nutritional interventions that include caregiver training may be a cost-effective approach for promoting children's social-emotional competence.

Finally, demographic background characteristics—specifically, child age, child gender, mother's age, maternal depression, and maternal agency—were associated with children's social-emotional outcomes in ways that were highly consistent with prior developmental research. Older children had fewer social-emotional problems across multiple domains while also showing more prosocial behavior, consistent with prior work highlighting age-related increases in emotion regulation and social-emotional competence (73, 97). Boys had more hyperactivity/inattention, but fewer emotional problems and less prosocial behavior compared to girls, consistent with extant literature showing gender differences in social-emotional development for children in LMICs (98). Children with older mothers showed fewer hyperactive/inattentive behaviors and total difficulties, suggesting that greater maternal age may confer some protective benefits.

Notably, mothers with fewer depressive symptoms or who were more self-efficacious had children with fewer social-emotional difficulties across all measured domains and had children who showed greater prosocial behavior. This is consistent with extant research in high-, middle-, and low-income countries demonstrating robust associations between maternal mental health and children's development (99, 100). Considering the intertwined nature of maternal mental health, responsivity, and punitive discipline (99101), integrated nutritional interventions that also address maternal mental health and caregiving behaviors may optimize both maternal and child social-emotional outcomes. Importantly, meta-analytic evidence suggests that parenting interventions in LMICs aimed at increasing supportive, responsive caregiving did not improve maternal depression, suggesting that parenting behavior and maternal mental health should be treated as distinct targets for interventions in LMICs (95).

Early-life nutritional supplementation moderates the effects of household physical safety and academic stimulation

Our findings indicate that early-life nutritional supplementation with LNS can play a moderating role in the association between specific home environmental risks and child social-emotional development. In our previous work, we found that children who received LNS were buffered against the adverse effects of overall lower-quality home environments (40), and the present study builds upon this work by identifying which distinct aspects of the home environment quality and which specific social-emotional outcomes are modified by LNS. For children receiving LNS, safer physical environments were associated with fewer difficulties and less hyperactivity/inattention, while household physical safety did not predict outcomes in the control group. Previous studies conducted in North America and Europe have shown that poor housing quality is associated with children's social-emotional difficulties (102, 103), and contributes to psychological distress, mood disorders, and behavioral dysregulation in school-age children from low- and middle-income families (104106). Our findings suggest that Ghanaian children who received LNS benefitted from physically safer home environments by exhibiting fewer social-emotional problems and were more like their peers in high-income countries. By contrast, children in the control group did not show this same effect, having comparable social-emotional problems regardless of their home's physical safety. This suggests that early-life nutritional supplementation with LNS may enhance the protective effects of a physically safe home.

Interestingly, children who received LNS were buffered against academically under-stimulating households as lower academic stimulation was associated with increased hyperactivity/inattention problems only among children in the control group. As previously discussed, there is robust evidence demonstrating that academic stimulation in the household and children's hyperactivity and attentional problems are negatively associated (82, 84, 88). These findings suggest that nutritional supplementation may differentially protect children against specific environmental risks, particularly physical and academic aspects of their surroundings, potentially reducing the impact of certain adverse conditions on social-emotional outcomes.

Limitations, strengths, and conclusion

This study was not without limitations. First, the internal consistencies for the SDQ Conduct and Peer Relationship Problems subscales were quite low, which can indicate poor construct reliability (107); however, lower alphas can occur in exploratory research and novel contexts (108, 109). Systematic review of the application and validation of the SDQ in Africa found substantial heterogeneity in alphas between studies, ranging from 0.24 to 0.73 (4), and our alphas fall within that range. Additionally, the mean inter-item correlations for all SDQ subscales exceeded the recommended cut-offs (110). Second, the trial was partially double-blinded as participants were not blinded to whether they received LNS sachets or MMN/IFA tablets during the main trial as the LNS sachets looked different from the MMN/IFA tablets. The MMN/IFA tablets were identical in appearance. The field staff conducting home visits at follow-up and data analysts were blinded. The lack of blinding for mothers may have biased their SDQ responses about their children's behavior as mothers in the LNS group were aware that they were receiving the novel nutritional supplement, which may have led them to have greater expectations for their child's development and may have impacted how they perceived their child's behavior. However, our previous work found no differences between LNS and control groups in parents' perceptions of the positive and negative impacts of the nutritional supplement on children's development at follow-up, suggesting that parents in both the LNS and control groups had equally high expectations (111). Third, two subscales of the newly adapted EC-HOME inventory—physical environment safety and negative behavior acceptance—had only three and four respective test items (see Supplementary material S1, S2), which may lead to low test discrimination and more vulnerability to measurement error.

Despite these limitations, this study has several strengths that enhance its contributions to the literature on child development in LMICs. First, the use of a randomized controlled trial (RCT) design minimizes selection bias and provides robust evidence for causal inferences about the effects of early-life nutritional supplementation. Second, this study also characterized Ghanaian children's home environments by applying factor analysis on test items of a widely used tool, the EC-HOME inventory, in a sample of more than 900 children. The newly adapted EC-HOME inventory showed better internal consistencies of the five new subscales (ranged from 0.43 to 0.79) than those of the original eight subscales (ranged from 0.14 to 0.69). This cultural adaptation of the EC-HOME inventory to the Ghanaian context ensures that the measures used are reliable and reflective of the local household factors influencing child development, enhancing the validity of the findings.

To our knowledge, this is one of the first studies investigating the moderating effect of a nutrition intervention on the association between specific aspects of household quality and both social-emotional strengths and difficulties for children living in an LMIC. By exploring how specific home environment factors interact with early-life nutritional supplementation, this study offers valuable insights into which environmental resources can be incorporated into the design of nutritional interventions, allowing for a more tailored approach to enhancing social-emotional outcomes in LMICs. Future research using a more detailed measure of children's physical environment and a larger sample size of children living in low-quality physical environments may further clarify the interaction of nutrition with the physical environment and several aspects of social-emotional development. A follow-up study of this cohort is needed to investigate whether Ghanaian children's social-emotional difficulties and the relationship between home environment and social-emotional development found in this study persist through school-age and young adulthood. Interventions aiming to reduce punitive discipline, increase caregiver responsiveness, and ensure safe physical environments, alongside nutrition, hold promise for improving social-emotional outcomes and advancing the United Nations' Sustainable Development Goals (UN SDGs) related to health and wellbeing (UN SDG 2, SDG 3). Integrated approaches that align health and psychosocial support can address children's complex developmental needs and may be more effective than isolated interventions alone. Future interventions should consider multipronged approaches that address both nutritional needs and home environment quality to promote early child development and wellbeing while mitigating against environmental risks.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of California, Davis. Ethics Committee for the College of Basic and Applied Sciences of the University of Ghana, Ghana Health Service Ethical Review Committee. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

LP: Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology. HY: Writing – original draft, Conceptualization, Formal analysis. MD: Writing – review & editing. SA-A: Writing – review & editing, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision. AG: Writing – review & editing, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision. KD: Writing – review & editing, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision. SK: Writing – review & editing. BO: Writing – review & editing. CA: Writing – review & editing, Data curation. EP: Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision. PH: Writing – review & editing, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was funded by grants from the National Institutes of Health (R01HD099811 & R03HD104875) and the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation (OPP49817) to the University of California, Davis, and the University of Ghana. The findings and conclusions contained within are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect positions or policies of the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation.

Conflict of interest

MD was employed by McKing Consulting Corporation.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2025.1571677/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Black MM, Walker SP, Fernald LCH, Andersen CT, DiGirolamo AM, Lu C, et al. Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. Lancet. (2017) 389:77–90. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31389-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, Lozoff B, Wasserman GA, Pollitt E, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. (2007) 369:145–57. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60076-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. McCoy DC, Peet ED, Ezzati M, Danaei G, Black MM, Sudfeld CR, et al. Early childhood developmental status in low- and middle-income countries: national, regional, and global prevalence estimates using predictive modeling. PLoS Med. (2016) 13:e1002034. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hoosen N, Davids EL, de Vries PJ, Shung-King M. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) in Africa: a scoping review of its application and validation. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. (2018) 12:6. doi: 10.1186/s13034-017-0212-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Pedersen GA, Smallegange E, Coetzee A, Hartog K, Turner J, Jordans MJD, et al. A systematic review of the evidence for family and parenting interventions in low- and middle-income countries: child and youth mental health outcomes. J Child Fam Stud. (2019) 28:2036–55. doi: 10.1007/s10826-019-01399-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Mao C, Shen Z, Long D, Liu M, Xu X, Gao X, et al. Epidemiological study of pediatric nutritional deficiencies: an analysis from the global burden of disease study 2019. Nutr J. (2024) 23:44. doi: 10.1186/s12937-024-00945-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Stevens GA, Finucane MM, Paciorek CJ, Flaxman SR, White RA, Donner AJ, et al. Trends in mild, moderate, and severe stunting and underweight, and progress towards MDG 1 in 141 developing countries: a systematic analysis of population representative data. Lancet. (2012) 380:824–34. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60647-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lu C, Cuartas J, Fink G, McCoy D, Liu K, Li Z, et al. Inequalities in early childhood care and development in low/middle-income countries: 2010–2018. BMJ Global Health. (2020) 5:e002314. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2020-002314

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Daelmans B, Black MM, Lombardi J, Lucas J, Richter L, Silver K, et al. Effective interventions and strategies for improving early child development. BMJ. (2015) 351:h4029. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h4029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Hastings PD. Utilizing the study of prosocial development to support the well-being of children and youths with pediatric health conditions. In:Bornstein MH, Shah PE, , editors Handbook of pediatric psychology, developmental-behavioral pediatrics, and developmental science. American Psychological Association (2025). doi: 10.1037/0000413-011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N. Empathy, prosocial behavior, and positive development in schools. In:Furlong MJ, Gilman R, Huebner ES, , editors. Handbook of Positive Psychology in Schools, 2nd ed. New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group (2014). p. 82–98.

Google Scholar

12. Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry. (2000) 177:534–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.177.6.534

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Keilow M, Sievertsen HH, Niclasen J, Obel C. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire and standardized academic tests: reliability across respondent type and age. PLoS ONE. (2019) 14:e0220193. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220193

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Mellins CA, Xu Q, Nestadt DF, Knox J, Kauchali S, Arpadi S, et al. Screening for mental health among young south african children: the use of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ). Glob Soc Welf. (2018) 5:29–38. doi: 10.1007/s40609-018-0110-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Wolf RT, Jeppesen P, Gyrd-Hansen D, Group TCS, Oxholm AS. Evaluation of a screening algorithm using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire to identify children with mental health problems: a five-year register-based follow-up on school performance and healthcare use. PLoS ONE. (2019) 14:e0223314. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0223314

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Black MM, Dewey KG. Promoting equity through integrated early child development and nutrition interventions. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2014) 1308:1–10. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12351

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Britto PR, Lye SJ, Proulx K, Yousafzai AK, Matthews SG, Vaivada T, et al. Nurturing care: promoting early childhood development. Lancet. (2017) 389:91–102. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31390-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Vaivada T, Gaffey MF, Bhutta ZA. Promoting early child development with interventions in health and nutrition: a systematic review. Pediatrics. (2017) 140:e20164308. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-4308

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Prado EL, Dewey KG. Nutrition and brain development in early life. Nutr Rev. (2014) 72:267–84. doi: 10.1111/nure.12102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Nelson CA, Gabard-Durnam LJ. Early adversity and critical periods: neurodevelopmental consequences of violating the expectable environment. Trends Neurosci. (2020) 43:133–43. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.01.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Tofail F, Fernald LC, Das KK, Rahman M, Ahmed T, Jannat KK, et al. Effect of water quality, sanitation, hand washing, and nutritional interventions on child development in rural Bangladesh (WASH Benefits Bangladesh): a cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet Child Adoles Health. (2018) 2:255–68. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30031-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Cusick SE, Georgieff MK. The role of nutrition in brain development: the golden opportunity of the “first 1000 days.” J Pediatr. (2016) 175:16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.05.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Schwarzenberg SJ, Georgieff MK, Committee Committee on Nutrition. Advocacy for improving nutrition in the first 1000 days to support childhood development and adult health. Pediatrics. (2018) 141:e20173716. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-3716

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Masten AS, Barnes AJ. Resilience in children: developmental perspectives. Children. (2018) 5:98. doi: 10.3390/children5070098

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van Ijzendoorn MH. Differential susceptibility to rearing environment depending on dopamine-related genes: New evidence and a meta-analysis. Dev Psychopathol. (2011) 23:39–52. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000635

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. For better and for worse: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Curr Direct Psychol Sci. (2007) 16:300–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Ellis BJ, Boyce WT. Biological sensitivity to context. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2008) 17:183–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00571.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Slagt M, Dubas JS, Deković M, van Aken MAG. Differences in sensitivity to parenting depending on child temperament: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. (2016) 142:1068–110. doi: 10.1037/bul0000061

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Nores M, Barnett WS. Benefits of early childhood interventions across the world: (under) investing in the very young. Econ Educ Rev. (2010) 29:271–82. doi: 10.1016/j.econedurev.2009.09.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, Grantham-McGregor SM. Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth-stunted Jamaican children: prospective cohort study. Lancet. (2005) 366:1804–7. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67574-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Yousafzai AK, Rasheed MA, Rizvi A, Armstrong R, Bhutta ZA. Effect of integrated responsive stimulation and nutrition interventions in the Lady Health Worker programme in Pakistan on child development, growth, and health outcomes: a cluster-randomised factorial effectiveness trial. Lancet. (2014) 384:1282–93. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60455-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Yousafzai AK, Aboud F. Review of implementation processes for integrated nutrition and psychosocial stimulation interventions. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2014) 1308:33–45. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12313

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Dulal S, Prost A, Karki S, Saville N, Merom D. Characteristics and effects of integrated nutrition and stimulation interventions to improve the nutritional status and development of children under 5 years of age: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Global Health. (2021) 6:e003872. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2020-003872

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Keats EC, Das JK, Salam RA, Lassi ZS, Imdad A, Black RE, et al. Effective interventions to address maternal and child malnutrition: an update of the evidence. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. (2021) 5:367–84. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30274-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Dewey KG, Stewart CP, Wessells KR, Prado EL, Arnold CD. Small-quantity lipid-based nutrient supplements for the prevention of child malnutrition and promotion of healthy development: overview of individual participant data meta-analysis and programmatic implications. Am J Clin Nutr. (2021) 114:3S−14S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqab279

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Adu-Afarwuah S, Lartey A, Okronipa H, Ashorn P, Peerson JM, Arimond M, et al. Small-quantity, lipid-based nutrient supplements provided to women during pregnancy and 6 mo postpartum and to their infants from 6 mo of age increase the mean attained length of 18-mo-old children in semi-urban Ghana: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. (2016) 104:797–808. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.116.134692

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Adu-Afarwuah S, Lartey A, Dewey KG. Meeting nutritional needs in the first 1000 days: a place for small-quantity lipid-based nutrient supplements. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2017) 1392:18–29. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13328

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Prado EL, Adu-Afarwuah S, Arnold CD, Adjetey E, Amponsah B, Bentil H, et al. Prenatal and postnatal small-quantity lipid-based nutrient supplements and children's social–emotional difficulties at ages 9–11 y in Ghana: follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. (2023) 118:433–42. doi: 10.1016/j.ajcnut.2023.05.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Prado EL, Adu-Afarwuah S, Lartey A, Ocansey M, Ashorn P, Vosti SA, et al. Effects of pre- and post-natal lipid-based nutrient supplements on infant development in a randomized trial in Ghana. Early Hum Dev. (2016) 99:43–51. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2016.05.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Ocansey ME, Adu-Afarwuah S, Kumordzie SM, Okronipa H, Young RR, Tamakloe SM, et al. Prenatal and postnatal lipid-based nutrient supplementation and cognitive, social-emotional, and motor function in preschool-aged children in Ghana: a follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. (2019) 109:322–34. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy303

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Caldwell B, Bradley R. Home observation for measurement of the environment: administration manual. In:Tempe, AZ., , editor Family & Huamn Dynamics Research Institute. Arizona State University (2003).

Google Scholar

42. Totsika V, Sylva K. The home observation for measurement of the environment revisited. Child Adolesc Ment Health. (2004) 9:25–35. doi: 10.1046/j.1475-357X.2003.00073.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Arimond M, Zeilani M, Jungjohann S, Brown KH, Ashorn P, Allen LH, et al. Considerations in developing lipid-based nutrient supplements for prevention of undernutrition: experience from the International Lipid-Based Nutrient Supplements (iLiNS) Project. Matern Child Nutr. (2016) 11:31–61. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12049

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Adu-Afarwuah S, Lartey A, Zeilani M, Dewey KG. Acceptability of lipid-based nutrient supplements (LNS) among Ghanaian infants and pregnant or lactating women. Matern Child Nutr. (2010) 7:344–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-8709.2010.00286.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Klevor MK, Haskell MJ, Lartey A, Adu-Afarwuah S, Zeilani M, Dewey KG. Lipid-based nutrient supplements providing approximately the recommended daily intake of vitamin a do not increase breast milk retinol concentrations among Ghanaian women123. J Nutr. (2016) 146:335–42. doi: 10.3945/jn.115.217786

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Adu-Afarwuah S, Young RT, Lartey A, Okronipa H, Ashorn P, Ashorn U, et al. Supplementation during pregnancy with small-quantity lipid-based nutrient supplements or multiple micronutrients, compared with iron and folic acid, increases women's urinary iodine concentration in semiurban Ghana: a randomized controlled trial. Matern Child Nutr. (2018) 14:e12570. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12570

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Adu-Afarwuah S, Lartey A, Okronipa H, Ashorn P, Zeilani M, Baldiviez LM, et al. Impact of small-quantity lipid-based nutrient supplement on hemoglobin, iron status and biomarkers of inflammation in pregnant Ghanaian women. Matern Child Nutr. (2017) 13:e12262. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12262

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ocansey ME, Adu-Afarwuah S, Kumordzie SM, Okronipa H, Young RR, Tamakloe SM, et al. The association of early linear growth and haemoglobin concentration with later cognitive, motor, and social-emotional development at preschool age in Ghana. Matern Child Nutr. (2019) 15:e12834. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12834

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Oaks BM, Young RR, Adu-Afarwuah S, Ashorn U, Jackson KH, Lartey A, et al. Effects of a lipid-based nutrient supplement during pregnancy and lactation on maternal plasma fatty acid status and lipid profile: results of two randomized controlled trials. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. (2017) 117:28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.plefa.2017.01.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiat. (1997) 38:581–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Stone LL, Otten R, Engels RCME, Vermulst AA, Janssens JMAM. Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire for 4- to 12-year-olds: a review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2010) 13:254–74. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0071-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Cox JL, Chapman G, Murray D, Jones P. Validation of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) in non-postnatal women. J Affect Disord. (1996) 39:185–9. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(96)00008-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Schwarzer R, Jerusalem M. General self-efficacy scale. In:Weinman J, Wright S, Johnston M, , editors. Measures in health psychology: A user's portfolio causal and control beliefs. Windsor, United Kingdom: nferNelson (1995). p. 35–7. doi: 10.1037/t00393-000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. (2012) 48:1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Leys C, Delacre M, Mora YL, Lakens D, Ley C. How to classify, detect, and manage univariate and multivariate outliers, with emphasis on pre-registration. Int Rev Soc Psychol. (2019) 32:1–10. doi: 10.5334/irsp.289

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. 4th ed. New York: Guilford Publications (2015). 553 p.

Google Scholar

57. Satorra A, Saris WE. Power of the likelihood ratio test in covariance structure analysis. Psychometrika. (1985) 50:83–90. doi: 10.1007/BF02294150

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Cortés-Albornoz MC, García-Guáqueta DP, Velez-van-Meerbeke A, Talero-Gutiérrez C. Maternal nutrition and neurodevelopment: a scoping review. Nutrients. (2021) 13:3530. doi: 10.3390/nu13103530

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Reis ÁE de M, Teixeira IS, Maia JM, Luciano LAA, Brandião LM, Silva MLS, et al. Maternal nutrition and its effects on fetal neurodevelopment. Nutrition. (2024) 125:112483. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2024.112483

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Lozoff B. Early iron deficiency has brain and behavior effects consistent with dopaminergic dysfunction. J Nutr. (2011) 141:740S−6S. doi: 10.3945/jn.110.131169

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Innis SM. Dietary omega 3 fatty acids and the developing brain. Brain Res. (2008) 1237:35–43. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.08.078

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Carrié I, Clément M, de Javel D, Francès H, Bourre JM. Phospholipid supplementation reverses behavioral and biochemical alterations induced by n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acid deficiency in mice. J Lipid Res. (2000) 41:473–80. doi: 10.1016/S0022-2275(20)34486-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Carrié I, Smirnova M, Clément M, De JD, Francès H, Bourre JM. Docosahexaenoic acid-rich phospholipid supplementation: effect on behavior, learning ability, and retinal function in control and n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acid deficient old mice. Nutr Neurosci. (2002) 5:43–52. doi: 10.1080/10284150290007074

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Stevens LJ, Zentall SS, Abate ML, Kuczek T, Burgess JR. Omega-3 fatty acids in boys with behavior, learning, and health problems. Physiol Behav. (1996) 59:915–20. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02207-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. East P, Doom JR, Blanco E, Burrows R, Lozoff B, Gahagan S. Iron deficiency in infancy and neurocognitive and educational outcomes in young adulthood. Dev Psychol. (2021) 57:962–75. doi: 10.1037/dev0001030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Kim J, Wessling-Resnick M. Iron and mechanisms of emotional behavior. J Nutr Biochem. (2014) 25:1101–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2014.07.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Lozoff B. Iron deficiency and child development. Food Nutr Bull. (2007) 28:S560–71. doi: 10.1177/15648265070284S409

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Hagen J, Mollen E, Wolf AW. Poorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancy. Pediatrics. (2000) 105:E51. doi: 10.1542/peds.105.4.e51

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Lozoff B, Beard J, Connor J, Barbara F, Georgieff M, Schallert T. Long-lasting neural and behavioral effects of iron deficiency in infancy. Nutr Rev. (2006) 64:S34–43. doi: 10.1301/nr.2006.may.S34-S43

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Ratsika A, Codagnone MC, O'Mahony S, Stanton C, Cryan JF. Priming for life: early life nutrition and the microbiota-gut-brain axis. Nutrients. (2021) 13:423. doi: 10.3390/nu13020423

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Horn J, Mayer DE, Chen S, Mayer EA. Role of diet and its effects on the gut microbiome in the pathophysiology of mental disorders. Transl Psychiatry. (2022) 12:164. doi: 10.1038/s41398-022-01922-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Bekdash RA. Early life nutrition and mental health: the role of DNA methylation. Nutrients. (2021) 13:3111. doi: 10.3390/nu13093111

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Denham SA, Basset HH, Wyatt T. The socialization of emotional competence. In:Grusec JE, Hastings PD, , editors. Handbook of Socialization: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press (2015). p. 590–613.

Google Scholar

74. Laible DJ, Thompson RA, Froimson J. Early socialization: the influence of close relationships. In:Handbook Handbook of Socialization: Theory and Research 2nd, ed. New York, NY, US: The Guilford Press (2015). p. 35–59.

Google Scholar

75. Twum-Danso A. Reciprocity, respect and responsibility: the 3Rs underlying parent-child relationships in Ghana and the implications for children's rights. Int J Child Rights. (2009) 17:415–32. doi: 10.1163/157181809X430337

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Lansford JE, Godwin J, Al-Hassan SM, Bacchini D, Bornstein MH, Chang L, et al. Longitudinal associations between parenting and youth adjustment in twelve cultural groups: cultural normativeness of parenting as a moderator. Dev Psychol. (2018) 54:362–77. doi: 10.1037/dev0000416

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Salaam B, Mounts NS. International note: Maternal warmth, behavioral control, and psychological control: Relations to adjustment of Ghanaian early adolescents. J Adolesc. (2016) 49:99–104. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.03.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Salaam B. Cumulative contextual risk, mothers' and fathers' parenting, and adolescents' psychosocial problems in Ghana. J Res Adolesc. (2024) 34:69–84. doi: 10.1111/jora.12898

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Lansford JE, Sharma C, Malone PS, Woodlief D, Dodge KA, Oburu P, et al. Corporal punishment, maternal warmth, and child adjustment: a longitudinal study in eight countries. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2014) 43:670–85. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.893518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Gershoff ET. Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: a meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol Bull. (2002) 128:539–79. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.128.4.539

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Grusec JE, Davidov M. Analyzing socialization from a domain-specific perspective. In:Grusec JE, Hastings PD, , editors. Handbook of Socialization: Theory & Research. 2nd ed. New York: Guildford Publications (2014). p. 158–81.

Google Scholar

82. Van Voorhis FL, Maier MF, Epstein JL, Lloyd CM. The Impact of Family Involvement on the Education of Children Ages 3 to 8: A Focus on Literacy and Math Achievement Outcomes and Social-Emotional Skills. MDRC (2013). Available online at: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED545474 (Accessed November 13, 2024).

Google Scholar

83. Baker CE. Fathers' and mothers' home literacy involvement and children's cognitive and social emotional development: implications for family literacy programs. Appl Dev Sci. (2013) 17:184–97. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2013.836034

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Mistry RS, Benner AD, Biesanz JC, Clark SL, Howes C. Family and social risk, and parental investments during the early childhood years as predictors of low-income children's school readiness outcomes. Early Child Res Q. (2010) 25:432–49. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.01.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Rosen ML, Hagen MP, Lurie LA, Miles ZE, Sheridan MA, Meltzoff AN, et al. Cognitive stimulation as a mechanism linking socioeconomic status with executive function: a longitudinal investigation. Child Dev. (2020) 91:e762–79. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13315

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Hindman AH, Morrison FJ. Differential contributions of three parenting dimensions to preschool literacy and social skills in a middle-income sample. Merrill-Palmer Quart. (2012) 58:191–223. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2012.0012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

87. McWayne C, Hampton V, Fantuzzo J, Cohen HL, Sekino Y. A multivariate examination of parent involvement and the social and academic competencies of urban kindergarten children. Psychol Sch. (2004) 41:363–77. doi: 10.1002/pits.10163

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Fantuzzo J, McWayne C, Perry MA, Childs S. Multiple dimensions of family involvement and their relations to behavioral and learning competencies for urban, low-income children. School Psych Rev. (2004) 33:467–80. doi: 10.1080/02796015.2004.12086262

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Ang RP, Klassen RM, Chong WH, Huan VS, Wong IYF, Yeo LS, et al. Cross-cultural invariance of the Academic Expectations Stress Inventory: adolescent samples from Canada and Singapore. J Adolesc. (2009) 32:1225–37. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.01.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Eriksen IM. Class, parenting and academic stress in Norway: middle-class youth on parental pressure and mental health. Discourse. (2021) 42:602–14. doi: 10.1080/01596306.2020.1716690

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Bates JE, Pettit GS. Temperament, parenting, and social development. In:Grusec JE, Hastings PD, , editors. Handbook of Socialization: Theory and Research. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Publications (2015). p. 372–97.

Google Scholar

92. Grusec JE. Socialization processes in the family: social and emotional development. Annu Rev Psychol. (2010) 62:243–69. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131650

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Hastings PD. The socialization of emotion by parents: following Saarni's legacy. Eur J Dev Psychol. (2018) 15:694–710. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2018.1482210

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Hastings PD, Grady JS, Barrieau LE. Children's anxious characteristics predict how their parents socialize emotions. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (2018) 47:1225–38. doi: 10.1007/s10802-018-0481-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Jeong J, Pitchik HO, Yousafzai AK. Stimulation interventions and parenting in low- and middle-income countries: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. (2018) 141:e20173510. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-3510

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Jeong J, Franchett EE, Oliveira CVR de, Rehmani K, Yousafzai AK. Parenting interventions to promote early child development in the first three years of life: a global systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med. (2021) 18:e1003602. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003602

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, Eggum ND. Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children's maladjustment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2010) 6:495–525. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Bando R, Lopez-Boo F, Fernald L, Gertler P, Reynolds S. Gender differences in early child development: evidence from large-scale studies of very young children in nine countries. J Econ Race Policy. (2024) 7:82–92. doi: 10.1007/s41996-023-00131-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Lovejoy MC, Graczyk PA, O'Hare E, Neuman G. Maternal depression and parenting behavior: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2000) 20:561–92. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00100-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Herba CM, Glover V, Ramchandani PG, Rondon MB. Maternal depression and mental health in early childhood: an examination of underlying mechanisms in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet Psychiatry. (2016) 3:983–92. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30148-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Schuengel C, Oosterman M. Parenting self-efficacy. In:Bornstein MH, , editor. Handbook of Parenting. 3rd ed. London: Routledge (2019). p. 654–80. doi: 10.4324/9780429433214-19

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Bradley RH, Putnick DL. Housing quality and access to material and learning resources within the home environment in developing countries. Child Dev. (2012) 83:76–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01674.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Evans GW, Saltzman H, Cooperman JL. Housing quality and children's socioemotional health. Environ Behav. (2001) 33:389–99. doi: 10.1177/00139160121973043

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Bradley RH, Rothenberg WA. Children's physical home environment, national development, and early childhood development in 51 low- and middle-income countries. In: Parenting and Child Development in Low- and Middle-Income Countries. London: Routledge (2022). doi: 10.4324/9781003044925-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Evans GW, Wells NM, Moch A. Housing and mental health: a review of the evidence and a methodological and conceptual critique. J Soc Issues. (2003) 59:475–500. doi: 10.1111/1540-4560.00074

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Ferguson KT, Cassells RC, MacAllister JW, Evans GW. The physical environment and child development: an international review. Int J Psychol. (2013) 48:437–68. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2013.804190

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Cronbach LJ. Test “reliability”: its meaning and determination. Psychometrika. (1947) 12:1–16. doi: 10.1007/BF02289289

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Cho E, Kim S. Cronbach's coefficient alpha: well known but poorly understood. Organ Res Methods. (2015) 18:207–30. doi: 10.1177/1094428114555994

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Cortina JM. What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. J Appl Psychol. (1993) 78:98–104. doi: 10.1037//0021-9010.78.1.98

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Clark LA, Watson D. Constructing Validity: Basic Issues in Objective Scale Development. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association (2016). p. 187. doi: 10.1037/14805-012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Adams KP, Okronipa H, Adu-Afarwuah S, Arimond M, Kumordzie S, Oaks BM, et al. Ghanaian parents' perceptions of pre and postnatal nutrient supplements and their effects. Matern Child Nutr. (2018) 14:e12608. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12608

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

112. UNICEF West and Central Africa Regional Office. Regional Documentation on the Education and Skills Development of Adolescents in and Out of School in West and Central Africa. UNICEF (2023).

Google Scholar

Keywords: nutritional supplement, social-emotional development, home environment, low-and middle-income countries, integrated intervention, small-quantity lipid-based nutrient supplement

Citation: Partington LC, Yuan H, Demuyakor ME, Adu-Afarwuah S, Guyer AE, Dewey KG, Kumordzie SM, Oaks BM, Arnold CD, Prado EL and Hastings PD (2025) Early-life nutritional supplementation protects against home environmental risks in Ghanaian children's social-emotional development. Front. Nutr. 12:1571677. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2025.1571677

Received: 05 February 2025; Accepted: 21 July 2025;
Published: 20 August 2025.

Edited by:

Olutosin Ademola Otekunrin, University of Ibadan, Nigeria

Reviewed by:

Daniel Edem Kpewou, University for Development Studies, Ghana
Alejandro Soto-Ospina, Corporación Universitaria Lasallista, Colombia
Maria Micheliana Da Costa Silva, Universidade Federal de Viçosa, Brazil

Copyright © 2025 Partington, Yuan, Demuyakor, Adu-Afarwuah, Guyer, Dewey, Kumordzie, Oaks, Arnold, Prado and Hastings. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lindsey C. Partington, bGNwYXJ0aW5ndG9uQHVjZGF2aXMuZWR1

Deceased

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.