Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Environ. Sci., 04 January 2023
Sec. Environmental Economics and Management
Volume 10 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fenvs.2022.1019904

Engaging faith-based communities in pro-environmental behavior using soft regulations: The case of single-use plastics

www.frontiersin.orgDan Kaufmann1 www.frontiersin.orgAriel Tikotsky2 www.frontiersin.orgTanhum Yoreh3 www.frontiersin.orgAnat Tchetchik4*
  • 1Sapir Academic College, Ashkelon, Israel
  • 2School of Business Administration Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel
  • 3School of the Environment, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 4Department of Geography and Environment, Faculty of Social Sciences, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel

The consumption of single-use plastics, such as disposable tableware (DTW), conveys a high benefit-cost ratio for consumers while having large environmental externalities. To encourage consumers to reduce their use of DTWs, governments could use small and non-coercive changes in people’s decision-making environments (nudges). This study focuses on the Israeli ultra-Orthodox communities a secluded population group that grows much faster- and consumes much more DTW than the rest of the Israeli population. Employing a quasi-representative sample (N = 450) of this population, this study conducted a discrete-choice experiment that presents the respondent with alternative options to reduce DTW. Two kinds of Nudges–framing and social norms–were utilized. The effectiveness of these Nudges in promoting PEB among faith-based communities has received little attention in previous studies. As another contribution to the literature, this paper also integrates latent constructs such as the respondents’ environmental attitudes and level of conservativeness. 46% of the respondents chose to opt-out whereas 29%, 14%, and 11% chose ‘1-day’, ‘2-days’, and ‘3-days’ per week avoiding DTW, respectively. Social norms, framings, and environmental attitudes had a significant mediating effect, with framing being associated with the highest effect on intentions to reduce DTW, i.e., a willingness to give up 0.31 USD per family member per month, compared to 0.07 USD for an increase in the description of the social norm. The results suggest that Nudges can enhance policies aimed at encouraging pro-environmental behavior among faith-based communities.

1 Introduction

The global disposable tableware (DTW) market is growing rapidly. In 2019, it was valued at 28.7 billion USD, and is predicted to reach 32.8 billion USD by the end of 2026 (MarketResearch.com, 2021). DTW are characterized by high functionality at low cost. They are intended to be used only once before they are thrown away. Because of their extremely low recyclability, DTW contaminate the natural environment (Wagner, 2017) and have become a significant source of damage, resulting in enormous environmental externalities (Cornago et al., 2021). In the North Pacific Ocean, the Great Pacific Garbage Patch, which is composed largely of plastic waste, exemplifies this problem; it has been estimated to be double the size of Texas (Lebreton et al., 2018). Various policies have been implemented to reduce the consumption of single-use plastic (SUP) in general and DTW in particular, ranging from soft policies (e.g., education) to command-and-control and market-based vehicles (e.g., taxation, fees, bans, and extended producer responsibility schemes). Thus far, these policies have yielded limited success, probably because of the lack of sufficient alternatives to DTW. This study focuses on the ultra-Orthodox Jewish population in Israel, a community whose SUP consumption is high, as compared to the broader society in Israel (Israel Central Bureau of Statistics, 2018), which, in turn, is the second biggest per capita consumer of SUP in the Mediterranean (World Wildlife Fund for Nature, 2020). Further, ultra-Orthodox Jews are the fastest-growing demographic in the developed world as well as in Israel (Cahaner and Malach, 2021). Ultra-Orthodox communities currently make up 12.9% of the population in Israel. Due to the high birth rate among ultra-Orthodox Jews (Freedman, 2020) their population is projected to grow at 4% per annum to two million by 2033, accounting for 16% of the Israeli population (Cahaner and Malach 2021). Even though they are not the current majority, projections indicate that when grouped with other Orthodox Jews, they are slated to become the largest Jewish demographic in Israel and the United States (Felman and Adkins, 2018; Cahaner and Malach 2021). Moreover, this population group has been known for its significant electoral power and gains, thus playing a large role in politics (Freedman, 2020). If ultra-Orthodox communities continue to consume single-use plastics at the current rate, this will have disastrous outcomes for the local environment, and put enormous strain on waste management systems.

Given the high consumption of DTW among the ultra-orthodox sector in Israel and their increased political power, legislation that bans or taxes disposable tableware may not be effective over the long term, which warrants the utilization of ‘soft’ regulatory measures. Indeed, various policies have been implemented to motivate faith-based communities to adopt PEB, such as education (e.g., Webb and Hayhoe, 2017), influencing their value frameworks (Smith and Pulver, 2009), and engaging faith leaders as role models to influence believers to adopt PEB (Yoreh, 2010; Tsimpo and Wodon, 2016). However, studies of the realized impact remain rare (Taylor et al., 2016). Given that consumption decisions are influenced by various factors, including perceptions of social norms (e.g., Steg and Vlek, 2009) and the high conformity to norms in faith-based communities, this study harnesses two nudges drawn from behavioral economics to explore their effect on the willingness to reduce consumption of DTW among ultra-Orthodox communities in Israel. The first nudge is a descriptive Social Norm (what other people in the community are doing in terms of using DTW). The second nudge is Framing (Defazio et al., 2021; Mertens et al., 2022) which in this study is the description of the hazards of DTW use in terms of health vs cleanliness and damage to the purity of sacred land.

This study provides the first evidence of the effectiveness of these nudges in promoting PEB among members of the ultra-Orthodox community. In addition, this study finds a mediating effect of latent variables such as pro-environmental attitudes and level of conservativeness.

The paper proceeds as follows: chapter 2 provides a Literature review and lay down the hypotheses. Chapter 3 describes the Methods including the study design, empirical model, data collection process, and analytical approach. Chapter 4 provides the statistical and econometric results. Chapter 5 provides policy implications and limitations.

2 Literature review and hypotheses

The following chapter provides the reader with a clear synthesis of the literature and the conceptual framework from which this study is drawn. The chapter starts with a review of PEB among faith-based communities and the challenges it conveys and continues with the utilization of Nudges to promote PEB in these communities, including emphasizing the contribution of this study. The chapter concludes with the research hypotheses.

2.1 Nudges

Nudges are small, non-coercive changes in people’s decision-making environments that affect their decision-making without substantial monetary incentives (Thaler and Sunstein, 2008). For example, defaulting employees into saving for retirement (while providing them an easy and highly salient way to override this default) can considerably increase the savings rate (Madrian and Shea, 2001; Thaler and Benartzi, 2004). Similarly, nudging people by providing them with information about a social norm (either descriptive - what others do—or injunctive - what is the right thing to do; Lede and Meleady, 2019) increases their inclination to adhere to that norm (e.g., Shang and Croson, 2009). Also prompting people with simple timely reminders can increase their uptake of services (e.g., Berliner Senderey et al., 2020). Another type of nudge is framing - namely, presenting information related to a decision (e.g., its potential outcomes) in a certain manner rather than another, so as to affect the decision-maker’s preferences (Tversky and Kahneman, 1986). A good example of framing was provided by Wilson (2006), who partnered with evangelical Christian leaders, in discussing environmental protection in terms of morality and ethics. For an extensive review of these and other types of Nudges please refer to Münscher et al. (2016) and Szaszi et al. (2018). Because of the high benefit-cost ratio of nudges, as compared to other policy tools, e.g., material incentives that include vouchers and tokens, (Yoeli et al., 2013; Benartzi et al., 2017; Chetty et al., 2014; Rogers and Feller, 2018; Mertens et al., 2022), governments and public institutions have increasingly used nudges to promote various policy goals in domains such as health, tax compliance, consumer protection and retirement savings (e.g., Halpern, 2015; OECD, 2017; Carlsson et al., 2019; Ruggeri, 2021) as well in promoting PEB (e.g., Schubert, 2017; Akbulut-Yuksel and Boulatoff, 2021). For example, “green nudges” (i.e., nudges that aim at promoting environmentally benign behavior) can be used to promote energy and water conservation (e.g., Allcott, 2011; Yoeli et al., 2013; Bhanot, 2021), recycling behaviors (e.g., Zhang and Wang, 2020) or food waste reduction (e.g., Kallbekken and Sælen, 2013; Vidal-Mones et al., 2022).

2.2 Pro-environmental behavior among faith-based communities

The literature increasingly discusses the potential and need for religious communities to advance environmental action. In their article published in Science, Amel et al. (2017) addressed climate change activism. They argued that faith communities can drive change as they bring people together through shared values and rituals. Faith communities have considerable social capital that enables them to enact change broadly and swiftly (Veldman et al., 2013). Religious environmental organizations attempt to motivate change by influencing the value frameworks of religious communities (Smith and Pulver 2009). Religious environmental activists reference fundamental ideas and values within their faith as guiding their PEB (McKay et al., 2013; Bomberg and Hague 2018; Hancock, 2018; Nilan, 2020; Koehrsen, 2021). To encourage action, initiatives such as Interfaith Power and Light’s Cool Congregation program1 and Faith and the Common Good’s Greening Sacred Spaces program2 highlight the economic incentives of greening places of worship. Some scholars argue that motivating Pro-Environmental Behavior (PEB) in faith-based communities is challenging; while some faith-based groups have been active in the climate activism movement (Zemo and Nigus, 2021), others have received media attention for their outright denial of climate change (Haluza-DeLay, 2014).

Nevertheless, the engagement of faith-based communities in PEB is often overlooked, both in academia and practice (Taylor et al., 2016). There are very few studies that address PEB among faith-based groups. For example, Rice (2006) investigated the relationship between PEB and religiosity among Islamic citizens of Cairo. This study found that religious teachings and religiosity were associated with PEB, through the notions of health and cleanliness, thus supporting the presence of an Islamic environmental ethic. In South Africa it was found that respondents perceived environmental stewardship as a dimension of Christian stewardship (Le Roux, 2012). In Israel there has also been little study concerning the environmental behaviors of Israeli minorities including faith-based groups. Most of the research has been conducted by Yoreh (2010, 2011, 2019a, b), who explored attitudes toward the environment, wastefulness, and recycling patterns among ultra-Orthodox communities in Israel and Canada. He argues that the existence of a prohibition against wastefulness (bal tashḥit; Deuteronomy 20:19–20) in Judaism might lead one to assume that observant Jews minimize wastefulness to a great extent. However, there is a considerable gap between how the prohibition’s parameters are generally interpreted by religious communities and how they are understood by environmentalists (Yoreh, 2019a). Nonetheless, Yoreh’s studies revealed an interesting paradox: on the one hand, wastefulness is considered commensurate with idolatry, and on the other hand, many observant Jews associate environmentalism with nature worship, resulting in a disinterest in environmental issues that can yield wasteful behavior. Therefore, it is difficult to predict what type of intervention would be effective. This study contributes to the existing literature by providing rigorous, empirical-based evidence, regarding the willingness of faith-based, ultra-Orthodox communities, to engage in unwasteful behavior (reducing DTW consumption) employing soft regulation measures.

2.3 Employing nudges to promote PEB among faith-based communities

As mentioned in 2.1, motivating faith-based communities to adopt PEB is more challenging. For example, Hagman et al. (2015) showed that people were less inclined to approve of pro-social nudges that minimize externalities than pro-self nudges (that focus on private welfare). According to Pe’er et al. (2019) this gap is less clear in faith-based social minorities. While several soft measures, such as education, influencing the value orientation, and using role models (e.g., Feldman and Meseley, 2003; Haigh, 2010; Moyer, 2015; Webb and Hayhoe, 2017; Lakhan, 2018) have been implemented to influence faith-based communities to adopt PEB, no nudges have been employed among these communities specifically to reduce consumption of DTW. Moreover, no studies have empirically studied the Nudges: social norms and framing in the context of faith-based communities PEB. We, therefore, focus on these Nudges. These nudges were proven to be effective, practical, and ethical in influencing people’s intentions and behaviors, including pro-environmental ones combined with the practicality and the ethicality of using them (Schubert, 2017; Grecksch, 2021; Homar and Knezevic Cvelbar, 2021; Xu et al., 2022). In Hummel and Maedche’s (2019) meta-analyses of the effectiveness of nudges, it was found that social norms interventions had an average effect size of 29%, while Mertens et al. (2022) in a similar meta-analysis, found that such nudges had an effect size (Cohen’s d) of 0.36. In the latter meta-analysis, the nudge category of information translation (to which framing belongs) yielded an effect size (Cohen’s d) of 0.28. In both meta-analyses, these kinds of nudges were found to be less effective than defaults - 87% and 0.62, respectively (Hummel and Maedche, 2019; Mertens et al., 2022). Yet, as noted by Perry et al. (2021) a stronger focus on the robust evaluation of the contribution of social norms to pro-environmental behaviors could lead to more effective pro-environmental initiatives.‏ Also, social norms and framing are inexpensive to implement and do not necessitate the introduction of changes in the consumer’s physical environment, or the use of technology (compare this to sending timely text messages, for example). Importantly, they are also less intrusive than nudges that are considered to be more effective, such as defaults.

2.4 Hypotheses

Based on prior evidence from the nudge literature (e.g., Schubert, 2017; Homar and Knežević Cvelbar, 2021; Pelletier and Sharp, 2008), the first hypothesis is as follows:

H1: The two kinds of nudges (social norms and framing) influence participants to express increased intentions to reduce their use of DTW.

In more individualistic cultures, personal attitudes play a more important role than in determining people’s social behavior. In contrast, in collectivist cultures (such as the ultra-Orthodox Jewish culture) the opposite is true (Triandis and Gelfand, 1998). Thus, the second hypothesis is as follows:

H2: The social norms nudge is more effective on people who are more collectivist compared to those who are less so.

Studies have shown that, unlike liberals, conservatives are not swayed by moral framing of environmental issues. Instead, conservative-leaning communities gravitate towards language such as cleanliness and purity (Feinberg and Willer, 2012; Yoreh, 2020). This is consistent with findings in ultra-Orthodox communities where cleanliness is championed as a top priority (Hershkovitz and Uval, 1998; Yoreh, 2010). As such, it is expected that the most conservative a community is, the more they will be motivated towards PEB under the framing that emphasizes that DTW harms the holy land’s purity and cleanliness, which leads to H3:

H3: Religious framing that stresses values of cleanliness/purity will be more effective in motivating PEB among non-modern, ultra-Orthodox, participants than in modern ultra-Orthodox participants.

The last Hypothesis is based on the considerable literature that suggests that environmental attitudes and knowledge can predict behavioral intention (e.g., Liu et al., 2020).

H4: Environmental attitudes can predict pro-environmental behaviors, and this effect can be moderated by knowledge.

Finally, given the altruistic nature of the ultra-orthodox community, the following research question is raised: Which of the following would be more effective in increasing intentions to reduce the use of DTW: receiving a small monetary reward for oneself, receiving the award for one’s children, or donating the reward to one’s religious community?

Figure 1 illustrates the hypothesized relationships between the nudges and the mediating factors.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The hypothesized relationships between the Nudges and the moderating factors. The oval variables represent the latent constructs, dashed arrows represent mediating effect.

3 Methods

3.1 Study design

Participants were presented with a survey questionnaire, beginning with a series of questions that asked them to estimate their DTW use patterns, including the drivers (barriers) underlying the use (minimizing the use) of DTW. The survey continued in the following parts: 1) a labeled discrete choice experiment (DCE) in which the participants were presented with four menus, each consisting of four mutually exclusive, alternatives for DTW reduction. The alternatives used in menus were labeled and included: 1. Avoiding DTW use 1 day each week, 2. Avoiding DTW usage 2 days each week, 3. Avoiding DTW use 3 days each week, or 4. Refraining from decreasing the use of DTW (opt-out). The participants were asked to indicate their preferred alternatives in each menu. Based on H1 (the effect of the framing nudge, i.e., the description of the hazards related to DTW use), prior to receiving the choice tasks, the participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions, i.e., two information framings regarding the damage caused by DTW and a no-information condition (see section b in 2.1.2 for details about the two information framings). Table 1 describes the attributes, and attributes’ levels, that describe the alternatives in the DCE. In general, the design attributes are as follows: (1). Community participation: an attribute that provides information about a descriptive social norm (based on the literature review to formulate H1) (2). The expected decrease in environmental damage in terms of reduced risk of pollution of drinking water. This is the environmental reward for the effort embedded in letting go of DTW (once, twice, or three times per week). Environmental rewards or benefits have been used widely in studies applying choice models to understand preferences towards pro-environmental behavior (e.g., Loomis and Gascoigne, 2018; Shan et al., 2019; Niskanen et al., 2021). 3. Symbolic monetary compensation. Reducing DTW may incur extra costs above and beyond the time and effort required (water, detergents, and the purchase of a dishwasher), therefore this attribute is included. This financial attribute is also important to calculate participants’ willingness to accept (WTA) for the effort embedded in their choices of alternatives, or for changes in the attributes of the alternatives. 4. Compensation beneficiary. This attribute was included given that ultra-Orthodox communities adhere to altruism in the form of charity or mutual aid (Berman, 2000; Malchi and Ben-Porat, 2018). The decisions about the attributes and their levels were also aided through in-depth interviews with marketing experts who specialize in the Israeli UO consumer segment and work with Askaria Ltd (Market Research Institute). After finalizing the first draft of the experimental design a pilot study was conducted among the staff of Askaria Market Research Institute. The pilot aimed at understanding the relevance of the attributes and their levels, and the cognitive burden associated with completing the choice tasks. The pilot was also required to obtain values for the efficient Bayesian design.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Attributes and their levels employed in the design.

Prior to conducting the choice tasks, the participants received the following brief explanation: “Below are four programs aimed at reducing the use of DTW. Each program contains three alternatives, among which you can choose the one that best suits you. Please note: There is no connection between the programs: for each of them, separately, you will be asked to choose the best alternative in your opinion.”

To generate the final experiment, a Bayesian D-error efficient design was employed using Ngene software. Twenty different choice tasks (i.e., scenarios) were generated and assigned via the algorithm into five blocks of four scenarios each. The choice tasks were designed to include implicit trade-offs between the attributes and their respective levels. An example of a choice scenario is provided in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. An example of a choice scenario.

The other parts of the survey included: 1) items aimed at unveiling the participants’ main media sources of influence; 2) items aimed at measuring collectivism; 3) items that measured the participants’ knowledge of the environmental harm caused by plastic pollution; 4) an abbreviated version of the revised New Ecological Paradigm Scale (NEP) (Dunlap et al., 2000). The NEP is a highly utilized known scale to measure Environmental Attitudes (EAs), it reflects beliefs about human-environment relationships and the perceived vulnerability of nature to human influences. Despite the emergence of other scales that measure EAs and despite problems associated with the NEP, such as unidimensional structure, (Bernstein and Szuster, 2018), no other measure has become the obvious heir to its throne and the NEP scale is still widely used (Mónus, 2021) as manifested in the numerous recent studies utilizing the NEP (e.g. Lou et al., 2022; Abedi Sarvestani and Shahraki, 2022; Hwang et al., 2020; Matsiori, 2020; Somerwill and Wehn, 2022; Tchetchik et al., 2021; Tchetchik et al., 2020). In the pilot conducted prior to the survey (detailed below in 2.2) the abbreviated NEP (10 of 15 items) was found to be less demanding while providing psychometric results equivalent to the conventional NEP scale (Cordano et al., 2003). Finally, items concerning socio-demographics and background were included.

3.1.2 Nudges

The effectiveness of two kinds of Nudges was tested:

a. The social norm nudge was operationalized by informing the participants that, based on prior research, a certain portion of families in their community were predicted to join the DTW use reduction plan. Three prospective participation rates were used 20%, 50%, or 80%.

b. The framing nudge concerned the damage associated with using DTW. One frame emphasized the fact that waste from DTW is harmful to the “Holy Land”. The other frame focused on DTWs as a source of health hazards caused by the presence of plastic particles in drinking water and food. This type of framing - emphasis framing (Druckman, 2001) emphasizes a subset of considerations, in order to steer people to focus on these considerations when constructing their preferences

3.2 Empirical model

3.2.1 Discrete choice model

To elicit the participants’ preferences towards reducing the use of DTW, a random utility-based, structural discrete-choice framework, was applied. This framework incorporates consumer heterogeneity and latent constructs. The inclusion of latent constructs leads to a more realistic representation of the choice process and better explanatory power (Ben-Akiva et al., 2002). Following the estimation procedure employed by Tchetchik et al. (2020), the choice data generated from the discrete-choice experiment were analyzed sequentially. First, confirmatory, and exploratory factor analyses, as well as latent class analysis were conducted for the latent variables. Second, the predicted values of the latent variables were internalized into the choice model. The choice model was then analyzed using the mixed-logit model with normal random distributed parameters.

3.3 Data collection

The generating process of the experimental design designated each of the five blocks to be assigned to a minimum of 35 participants, which resulted in a sample size of at least 35 × 5 = 175 participants. 450 ultra-Orthodox adults were sampled4. Since each participant was presented with four menus, each of which included four alternatives, the total number of choices was 7,200.

3.3.1 Sample characteristics

A list of the demographic variables of the 450 participants and their descriptive statistics are provided in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Sample’s descriptive statistics.

3.4 Analytical approach

The behavioral model is tested and validated by using a mixed-logit model with latent variables. The mixed-logit model is designed to capture the unobserved heterogeneity in the preferences of participants. It accounts for random taste variations and can incorporate correlations in unobserved factors over choice alternatives (Colombo et al., 2009). Preference heterogeneity can be explained by observed variables (i.e., age and income) and latent variables, such as latent psychological traits (i.e., environmental attitudes). According to Ben-Akiva et al. (2002, P1), “the incorporation of psychological factors leads to a more behaviorally realistic representation of the choice process, and consequently, better explanatory power“. The likelihood function formulation is presented in Supplementary Figure S1B. The (latent) variable environmental attitudes were included in the choice model since it has been indicated as a predictor of pro-environmental behavior (Bamberg and Möser, 2007). Other potential moderating factors which were included, are respondents’ own knowledge regarding the damage caused by DTW (operationalized via the items: plastic pollutes the air, plastic pollutes water sources, plastic pollutes the soil and harms agricultural produce, plastic waste creates damage to the appearance of the landscape), their level of conformism (versus individuality) and their level of conservativeness, the latter was operationalized via the extent to which they use the Internet and social media (Shahzalal and Hassan, 2019).

4 Results

The distribution of the answers to the question: how many meals per week do you use disposable tableware is described in Figure 3. Notably, 61% acknowledged that they used DTW between meals to a very large extent.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. the distribution of the number of meals per week in which the respondents use DTW.

When respondents were asked how hard it would be to forgo the use of DTW, 35% of the participants answered that it would be difficult, and an additional 40% answered that it would be very difficult. High and significant correlations (0.61–0.80) were found between the variables of the number of meals with DTW, using DTW between meals, and how hard it would be to not use DTW.

When the participants were asked about reasons for using DTW, saves effort emerged as the most important reason, which increased as the extent of use increased (see Figure 4). The low cost and ease of storage of DTW were ranked second and third in importance, respectively. Saving water was the least important reason at all use levels. Other reasons mentioned for using DTW were that in comparison to non-disposable tableware, DTW save time (3%), are more aesthetical (2%), more hygienic (2%), save time (3%), and safer for children (1%).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. The importance of the different reasons to use DTW (on a 1-7 scale) by weekly usage.

Looking at the distribution of choices in the choice task, 46% chose the opt-out option (not interested in reducing DTW) whereas 29%, 14%, and 11% chose the 1-day, 2-day, and 3 days a week avoiding DTW, respectively.

Looking further at the differences in the choice distribution, it is found that respondents from the Lithuanian strand chose the opt-out option significantly more than the other two strands (Hasidic and Sephardi) i.e., 50% compared to 41% and 46%, respectively (p < 0.005). Also, 53% and 49% of the respondents with the highest income levels (above average and much above average) chose to opt-out which is significantly higher than the three lower income levels, 43%, 44% and 44%, respectively (p < 0.005). When comparing the choice distribution across the different, framing conditions, no significant differences are found (see Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Choice distribution across the different framing conditions.

Next, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted for the NEP scale. The results revealed two factors: eco-crisis (i.e., believing that an ecological crisis was imminent) and climate denial (the belief that nature’s balance was strong enough to cope with anthropogenic effects, and that the ecological crisis has been greatly exaggerated). Employing the -PNORM-command in Stata 17.0, the distribution of these two factors in the population follows a normal one (see Supplementary Figure S1A ). However, the mean value of the factor climate denial (2.78 [0 0.045]) is significantly higher (p < 0.001 in a t-test) than the mean value of the Eco-crisis factor (2.55 [0.04]). These factors were tested in the econometric analysis, the mixed-logit model via interactions with the different alternative specific constants and the alternative attributes to examine whether, and in what way (i.e., via each attribute is it operationalized) and to what extent environmental attitudes affect the intention to consume less DTW.

Following the Latent Class Analysis (LCA) regarding sources of exposure to news and other content, the participants were assigned to three mutually exclusive clusters: participants who extensively used digital social media (“least conservative”); participants who extensively use printed media and use the Internet for email only (“moderately conservative”) and participants who do not have access to the Internet at all (“most consecutive”). The results of the analysis are provided in Supplementary Figure S1A. A PCA on the conformism/individuality scale did not yield sufficient internal consistency (alpha Cronbach was low). Therefore, only one item from this four-item scale, was used. Finally, respondents who received no framing were presented with four items to elicit their own knowledge regarding the damage caused by DTW. A PCA resulted in one significant factor (alpha Cronbach = 0.9).

4.1 Econometric model results

4.1.1 Notation

To make this model more reader-friendly, Table 3 provides the notations and assumptions are presented in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Notations and assumptions used in the econometric analyses.

Table 4 reports the results of the five mixed-logit models. In these models, a binary dependent variable equaled one if an alternative was chosen and 0 otherwise. The main independent variables were the attributes of the alternatives to DTW avoidance, and the information framing conditions. Model 1) consisted of the main effects (i.e., the choice attributes), including the alternative-specific constants (ASC), which reveal the ceteris paribus preferences for avoiding DTW for 1 day, 2 days, and 3 days per week (the opt-out alternative was the reference). Models (2)–5) included variables that represented the participants’ environmental attitudes, informational conditions, and sources of knowledge. In each model, the log-likelihood and McFadden’s Pseudo R2 is provided. The latter equals 27% in model (5), which was sufficiently high for a cross-sectional survey. The ASC parameters were assigned random parameters to determine their heterogeneity. The model was specified in a preference space rather than a willingness to pay (WTP) space.5 Hence, the coefficient of the payment vehicle (i.e., compensation) was assigned as a fixed parameter. The right-hand column in Table 4 provides the WTP values based on the results of model (5). Following Hensher et al. (2005), WTP estimates were calculated by dividing the choice attribute coefficient by the compensation coefficient.6 The interpretations of the results of model 5) and the corresponding WTP/A values are discussed below.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Results of the mixed-logit model.

The compensation coefficient is positive, as expected by economic theory. Similarly, the coefficient of risk reduction is positive, i.e., DTW avoidance plans that offer higher risk reduction of plastic pollution, are more likely to be chosen. Participants are willing to give up 0.33 USD per household member per month to choose a plan that enables higher risk reduction. Both coefficients, payment children and payment self, are positive (the reference option is a donation to the participant’s religious institution) with the coefficient of payment children being significantly larger than the payment self-coefficient (i.e., participants prioritized their children’s welfare over their own; they are willing to forsake 0.39 USD if the compensation is directed to their children’s saving accounts, and only 0.30 USD if the compensation is directed to their own accounts, compare to the reference option). The coefficient of community participation is positive. For each percentage increase in the share of community members that are expected to join a certain reduction plan, the participant is willing to give up 0.07 USD. However, the higher the participants are ranked on the individualism score, the less they will be influenced by social norms; their willingness to give-up compensation will decrease by 0.02 USD.

Regarding the information conditions, compared with participants who did not receive any information (i.e., the reference condition), those who received the Holy Land cleanliness framing and mostly relied on printed media (“moderately conservative”), were more likely to choose the 3 days avoidance program. Respondents who receive this framing and who do not use the Internet at all (“more conservative”) are less likely to choose 3 days of DTW avoidance. They will require an additional compensation of 0.94 USD to participate in this plan. Respondents who are greatly exposed to social media (“least conservative”) and who received health damage framing will be more likely to choose 3 days of DTW avoidance; that is, they will be willing to give up 0.31 USD. However, the participants who received the health damage framing and who are highly exposed to printed media (“moderately conservative”) or do not use the Internet at all (“more conservative”) will be less likely to choose this program and will require an additional compensation of 0.30 USD and 0.28 USD, respectively. Regarding the participants who did not receive any information, the effects of their perceptions of the damage caused by plastic pollution were assessed. These perceptions were positively associated with the choice of avoiding using DTW 3 days per week and the willingness to give up 0.18 USD per household member per month.

Finally, concerning the ASCs, the coefficient of 1-day DTW avoidance did not differ significantly from zero. However, its S.D. was significant, implying that the preferences are equally distributed around zero. The coefficients of two- and 3-day avoidance indicated that the participants required additional compensation of 1.23 USD and 1.99 USD, respectively (ceteris paribus). However, the results showed high heterogeneity around these preferences: 21% of the respondents had a positive preference for 2-day avoidance whereas nearly 9% of the respondents had a positive preference for 3-day avoidance (the distribution of the preferences appears in Figure in Supplementary Figure S1C).

5 Concluding remarks

Policymakers at all levels aim to mitigate anthropogenic pressure on the environment through various means, including regulations. However, the effectiveness and efficiency of many measures, such as command-and-control and market-based tools, are limited. Clearly, this goal will only be realized with the cooperation of businesses and consumers (Byerly et al., 2018; Ali et al., 2022). At the same time, there is an increasingly large number of consumers who are religiously affiliated. In fact, by 2050, the number of people affiliated with a religion is expected to grow from 5.8 billion in 2010 to 8.1 billion (Grim and Connor, 2015). Thus, it is essential to identify ways of motivating these populations to adopt PEB in general and to reduce the consumption of DTW in particular. This study advances the understanding of the relationships between soft intervention measures (nudges) and the desired environmental behavior concerning the consumption of single-use plastics among a population that heavily uses them. It is essential for enabling policymakers, government officials, NGOs, and others to design better policy initiatives.

The results of the current study provide evidence that simple and non-coercive changes in believers’ decision-making architectures can decrease their intentions to use DTW without imposing drastic policy measures such as high taxation. Specifically, the findings showed that social norms and information framing that communicated health hazards promoted a shift toward reducing DTW use. Similarly, the findings indicated that a framing nudge that depicted the use of DTWs as harming the Holy Land increased the tendency to avoid DTW consumption. This effect was moderated by the participants’ level of conservativeness, a factor that was measured by their exposure to external (digital) sources of knowledge.

The findings suggest that less conservative respondents who use the Internet and Social Networks as their main knowledge sources are more responsive to the health damage framing. In contrast, respondents who are moderately conservative (i.e., they use mainly printed media but use the Internet for emails) are more responsive to the holy-land purity framing. These findings provide empirical evidence of the nuanced nature through which “green nudges” (in the form of framing) affect ultra-Orthodox Jews based on their level of conservativeness, as manifested by their use of the Internet and social media. From a policy perspective, this finding is important as it emphasizes the need to use different nudges to different types of communities or even to sub-communities (Soman and Hossain, 2021).

In addition, for respondents who received no framing (the reference condition), it was found that previous knowledge about the damage caused to the environment and health was associated with a higher probability of avoidance from DTW. This stresses the ongoing need to effectively communicate these risks via various media channels. Clearly, exploring alternative ways to deliver pro-environmental messages is crucial in faith-based communities, which ignore or are less exposed to the Internet. The results of this study emphasize that knowledge about the hazards of SUP to the environment can overcome the high convenience of using them even in very large families were washing the dishes after every meal is indeed an unpleasant task. The fact that respondents in the survey preferred to save the money they will receive from avoiding DTW for their children and not for themselves also expressed their concerns about the future generation which is in line with the motivation to limit their use of DTW.

The results of this study also help substantiate, in the context of DTW use, previous findings: First, participants who ranked high on the climate denial factor were less likely to engage in DTW reduction efforts. Second, the findings showed that social norms had a positive impact on intentions to reduce DTW use. As can be expected, for individualists, this impact is less pronounced.

According to the Israeli Household Expenditure Survey (Israel Central Bureau of Statistics, 2018), annual expenditure per family on disposable tableware is led by the National Religious (religiously observant but not rejecting modern practices) and ultra-Orthodox (a highly religious and very conservative population in terms of modern practices) sectors. The results of the current study may also serve as a reference to a massive command-and-control measure that was recently implemented in the same context of reducing the use of DTW, namely, a new tax on DTW that entered into force in November 2021. The taxation of DTW in Israel faced strong opposition from representatives of the ultra-Orthodox community who viewed this tax as political action while ignoring the argument that this taxation is motivated by the need to protect the environment. Therefore, any changes in the government structure will put this piece of legislation in danger. As shown, the use of DTW is strongly motivated by convenience factors. This study points that softer regulation may lead to similar and more stable results.

Finally, the contribution of this research goes beyond the boundaries of its specific context, as it provides an empirical framework to explore behavioral change when behavior is very persistent and where responsiveness to nudges has been acknowledged to be unclear.

5.1 Limitations

The results provide the first evidence of the effectiveness of soft regulations in changing religious believers’ intentions to use DTW; however, additional research is needed in this area. First, a future study should investigate the effectiveness of nudges in influencing the actual consumption of DTW. Second, additional studies should be conducted on additional faith-based communities to increase the external validity of the findings of the current study. Because the main motivation for using DTW is to save time and effort, these efforts should be accompanied by presenting a viable alternative to environmentally harmful DTW, such as bio-degradable tableware at affordable prices.

Third, this study explores which “soft” regulatory measures will be more effective in achieving a substantial reduction in the consumption of single-use plastics. Nevertheless, the implications of the expected changes in the demand on the supply side, and therefore on the industry of single-use plastics, are not considered. This is a limitation of the study as the demand for the product always plays the most critical role in any industry (Dey et al., 2022a). Understanding expected variations in the demand for single-use plastics among these communities has crucial implications for manufacturers. It is essential for the SCM model to ensure the sustainable development of society (Sarkar and Bhuniya, 2022). For example, fluctuating market demand affects holding cost; in the case of low demand, the holding cost is reduced by limiting stock (Dey et al., 2022b). This is also the case when considering whether to invest in structural innovation of greener products (refers to products with improved design and functionality but without using new parts) or in improved innovation (which uses at least one new part) to produce the new innovative product (Sarkar et al., 2022) (Agrawal, 2008; Carlsson et al., 2019; Loomis and Gascoigne, 2018).

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusion of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

DK: conceptualization, data collection; ATik: trial design, trial execution, and writing; TY: trial design, writing, and review; ATch: methodology, trial design, data analysis, writing, and review.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fenvs.2022.1019904/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1https://www.coolcongregations.org/.

2https://www.faithcommongood.org/greening_sacred_spaces.

3Based on an exchange rate of 1 USD = NIS 3.521 in November 2019 when the survey was conducted.

4The sample was representative in the following important dimensions: gender, belongingness to a sub-sector within the ultra-orthodox population, residence (in ultra-orthodox-dominant areas versus other areas), and region of residence.

5In general, these approaches are equivalent. However, because convenient distributions Normal or log-normal) are usually specified, there are different implications when they are placed on WTPs rather than on coefficients (Train and Weeks, 2005).

6The WTP/A values shown in Table 4 were originally in NIS but were converted to USD (USD 1 = NIS 3.521) in November 2019 when the survey was conducted.

References

Abedi Sarvestani, A., and Shahraki, M. R. (2022). Analysis of forest guard staff’s attitudes towards the new environmental paradigm case study of golestan province. Geogr. Environ. Plan. 33 (1), 1–18.

Google Scholar

Agrawal, S. K. (2008). Faith-based ethnic residential communities and neighbourliness in Canada. Plan. Pract. Res. 23 (1), 41–56. doi:10.1080/02697450802076431

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akbulut-Yuksel, M., and Boulatoff, C. (2021). The effects of a green nudge on municipal solid waste: Evidence from a clear bag policy. J. Environ. Econ. Manag. 106, 102404. doi:10.1016/j.jeem.2020.102404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ali, S. M., Ahmed, S., Ahmed, H. N., Sharmin, A., and Rahman, R. (2022). Reducing plastic pollutants through catalyzing consumer roles: A novel application of fuzzy total interpretive structural modeling. J. Clean. Prod. 335, 130327. ‏. doi:10.1016/j.jclepro.2021.130327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allcott, H. (2011). Social norms and energy conservation. J. Public Econ. 95 (9-10), 1082–1095. doi:10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amel, E., Manning, C., Scott, B., and Koger, S. (2017). Beyond the roots of human inaction: Fostering collective effort toward ecosystem conservation. Science 356 (6335), 275–279. doi:10.1126/science.aal1931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bamberg, S., and Möser, G. (2007). Twenty years after hines, hungerford, and tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 27 (1), 14–25. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Akiva, M., Walker, J., Bernardino, A. T., Gopinath, D. A., Morikawa, T., and Polydoropoulou, A. (2002). “Integration of choice and latent variable models,” in Perpetual motion: Travel behaviour research opportunities and application challenges, 431–470. ‏.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benartzi, S., Beshears, J., Milkman, K. L., Sunstein, C. R., Thaler, R. H., Shankar, M., et al. (2017). Should governments invest more in nudging? Psychol. Sci. 28 (8), 1041–1055. doi:10.1177/0956797617702501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berliner Senderey, A., Kornitzer, T., Lawrence, G., Zysman, H., Hallak, Y., Ariely, D., et al. (2020). It’s how you say it: Systematic A/B testing of digital messaging cut hospital no-show rates. PloS one 15 (6), e0234817. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0234817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, E. (2000). Sect, subsidy, and sacrifice: An economist's view of ultra-orthodox Jews. Q. J. Econ. 115 (3), 905–953. doi:10.1162/003355300554944

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, J., and Szuster, B. (2018). Beyond unidimensionality: Segmenting contemporary pro-environmental worldviews through surveys and repertory grid analysis. Environ. Commun. 12 (8), 1062–1076. doi:10.1080/17524032.2018.1504809

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhanot, S. P. (2021). Isolating the effect of injunctive norms on conservation behavior: New evidence from a field experiment in California. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 163, 30–42. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2018.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bomberg, E., and Hague, A. (2018). Faith-based climate action in christian congregations: Mobilisation and spiritual resources. Local Environ. 23 (5), 582–596. doi:10.1080/13549839.2018.1449822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byerly, H., Balmford, A., Ferraro, P. J., Hammond Wagner, C., Palchak, E., Polasky, S., et al. (2018). Nudging pro-environmental behavior: Evidence and opportunities. Front. Ecol. Environ. 16 (3), 159–168. ‏. doi:10.1002/fee.1777

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahaner, L., and Malach, G. (2021). Statistical report on ultra-orthodox society in Israel 2021. Jerusalem: The Israel Democracy Institute. [In Hebrew.].

Google Scholar

Carlsson, F., Gravert, C. A., Kurz, V., and Johansson-Stenman, O. (2019). “CeCAR working paper series No. 4,” in Nudging as an Environmental Policy Instrument (April 25, 2019) Available at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3711946 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3711946.

Google Scholar

Chetty, R., Friedman, J. N., Leth-Petersen, S., Nielsen, T. H., and Olsen, T. (2014). Active vs. passive decisions and crowd-out in retirement savings accounts: Evidence from Denmark. Q. J. Econ. 129 (3), 1141–1219. doi:10.1093/qje/qju013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, S., Hanley, N., and Louviere, J. (2009). Modeling preference heterogeneity in stated choice data: An analysis for public goods generated by agriculture. Agric. Econ. 40 (3), 307–322. ‏. doi:10.1111/j.1574-0862.2009.00377.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cordano, M., Welcomer, S. A., and Scherer, R. F. (2003). An analysis of the predictive validity of the new ecological paradigm scale. J. Environ. Educ. 34, 22–28. doi:10.1080/00958960309603490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornago, E., Börkey, P., and Brown, A. (2021). “Preventing single-use plastic waste: Implications of different policy approaches,” in OECD Environment Working Papers (OECD Publishing), 182. doi:10.1787/c62069e7-en

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Defazio, D., Franzoni, C., and Rossi-Lamastra, C. (2021). How pro-social framing affects the success of crowdfunding projects: The role of emphasis and information crowdedness. J. Bus. Ethics 171 (2), 357–378. doi:10.1007/s10551-020-04428-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dey, B. K., Park, J., and Seok, H. (2022a). Carbon-emission and waste reduction of a manufacturing-remanufacturing system using green technology and autonomated inspection. RAIRO-Oper. Res. 56 (4), 2801–2831. ‏. doi:10.1051/ro/2022138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dey, B. K., Yilmaz, I., and Seok, H. (2022b). A sustainable supply chain integrated with autonomated inspection, flexible eco-production, and smart transportation. Processes, 10(9), 1775.‏doi:10.3390/pr10091775

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Druckman, J. N. (2001). The implications of framing effects for citizen competence. Polit. Behav. 23 (3), 225–256. doi:10.1023/a:1015006907312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunlap, R. E., Van Liere, K. D., Mertig, A. G., and Jones, R. E. (2000). New trends in measuring environmental attitudes: Measuring endorsement of the new ecological paradigm: A revised nep scale. J. Soc. Issues 56, 425–442. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinberg, M., and Willer, R. (2012). The moral roots of environmental attitudes. Psychol. Sci. 24 (1), 56–62. doi:10.1177/0956797612449177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, D., and Meseley, L. (2003). Faith-based environmental initiatives in Appalachia: Connecting faith, environmental concern and reform. Worldviews. 7 (3), 227–252. doi:10.1163/156853503322709128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felman, A., and Adkins, L. E. (2018). Orthodox to dominate American jewry in coming decades as population booms. New York, NY: The Forward. Available at: https://forward.com/news/402663/orthodox-will-dominate-american-jewry-in-coming-decades-as-population/.

Google Scholar

Fleischer, A., and Sternberg, M. (2006). The economic impact of global climate change on mediterranean rangeland ecosystems: A space-for-time approach. Ecol. Econ. 59 (3), 287–295. ‏. doi:10.1016/j.ecolecon.2005.10.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freedman, M. (2020). Vote with your rabbi: The electoral effects of religious institutions in Israel. Elect. Stud. 68, 102241. doi:10.1016/j.electstud.2020.102241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelfand, M. J., and Realo, A. (1999). Individualism-collectivism and accountability in intergroup negotiations. J. Appl. Psychol. 84 (5), 721–736. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.84.5.721

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grecksch, K. (2021). Achieving water efficiency in the public sector through social norms. Front. Environ. Sci., 446.‏doi:10.3389/fenvs.2021.575583

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grim, B. J., and Connor, P. (2015). Changing religion, Changing economies. Future global religious and economic impact. Availableat: http://religiousfreedomandbusiness.org/changing-religion-and-changing-economies.

Google Scholar

Hagman, W., Andersson, D., Västfjäll, D., and Tinghög, G. (2015). Public views on policies involving nudges. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 6 (3), 439–453. doi:10.1007/s13164-015-0263-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haigh, M. (2010). Education for a sustainable future: Strategies of the new hindu religious movements. Sustainability 2 (11), 3500–3519. doi:10.3390/su2113500

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halpern, D. (2015). Inside the nudge unit: How small changes can make a big difference. WH Allen. Reprint edition.

Google Scholar

Haluza-DeLay, R. (2014). Religion and climate change: Varieties in viewpoints and practices. WIREs Clim. Change 5 (2), 261–279. ‏. doi:10.1002/wcc.268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hancock, R. (2018). Islamic environmentalism: Activism in the United States and great britain. New York, NY and Abingdon, UK: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Hensher, D. A., Rose, J. M., Rose, J. M., and Greene, W. H. (2005). Applied choice analysis: A primer. ‏: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Hershkovitz, S., and Uval, M. (1998). The haredi population in jerusalem: Needs and characteristics. Jerusalem: The Municipality of Jerusalem. (in Hebrew).

Google Scholar

Homar, A. R., and Knežević Cvelbar, L. K. (2021). The effects of framing on environmental decisions: A systematic literature review. Ecol. Econ. 183, 106950. doi:10.1016/j.ecolecon.2021.106950

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hummel, D., and Maedche, A. (2019). How effective is nudging? A quantitative review on the effect sizes and limits of empirical nudging studies. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 80, 47–58. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2019.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwang, Y. T., Moon, J., Lee, W. S., Kim, S. A., and Kim, J. (2020). Evaluation of firefly as a tourist attraction and resource using contingent valuation method based on a new environmental paradigm. J. Qual. Assur. Hosp. Tour. 21 (3), 320–336. ‏. doi:10.1080/1528008x.2019.1663464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Israel Central Bureau of Statistics (2018). Household expenditure survey of the CBS. (in Hebrew).

Google Scholar

Kallbekken, S., and Sælen, H. (2013). ‘Nudging’ hotel guests to reduce food waste as a win–win environmental measure. Econ. Lett. 119 (3), 325–327. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2013.03.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koehrsen, J. (2021). Muslims and climate change: How Islam, Muslim organizations, and religious leaders influence climate change perceptions and mitigation activities. WIREs Clim. Change 12 (3), 1–19. doi:10.1002/wcc.702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakhan, C. (2018). The garbage gospel: Using the theory of planned behavior to explain the role of religious institutions in affecting pro-environmental behavior among ethnic minorities. J. Environ. Educ. 49 (1), 43–58. doi:10.1080/00958964.2017.1337701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Layton, D. F., and Brown, G. (2000). Heterogeneous preferences regarding global climate change. Rev. Econ. Statistics 82 (4), 616–624. doi:10.1162/003465300559091

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Roux, C. S. (2012). The role of the christian church in promoting environmental stewardship: A case study of the uniting reformed church in Southern Africa, 129. BOB.

Google Scholar

Lebreton, L., Slat, B., Ferrari, F., Sainte-Rose, B., Aitken, J., Marthouse, R., et al. (2018). Evidence that the great pacific garbage patch is rapidly accumulating plastic. Sci. Rep. 8 (1), 1–15. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-22939-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lede, E., and Meleady, R. (2019). Applying social influence insights to encourage climate resilient domestic water behavior: Bridging the theory-practice gap. WIREs Clim. Change 10, e562. doi:10.1002/wcc.562

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, P., Teng, M., and Han, C. (2020). How does environmental knowledge translate into pro-environmental behaviors?: The mediating role of environmental attitudes and behavioral intentions. Sci. total Environ. 728, 138126. ‏. doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.138126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loomis, J., and Gascoigne, W. (2018). Understanding agricultural producers' willingness to undertake self-monitoring of environmental outcomes: Results of a choice experiment with Colorado agricultural producers. J. Nat. Resour. Policy Res. 8 (1-2), 1–21. doi:10.5325/naturesopolirese.8.1-2.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lou, X., Li, L. M. W., Xia, W., and Zhu, Q. (2022). A meta-analysis of temporal shifts in environmental concern between 1994 and 2017: An examination of the new environmental paradigm. Anthropocene 3, 100335. doi:10.1016/j.ancene.2022.100335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madrian, B. C., and Shea, D. F. (2001). The power of suggestion: Inertia in 401 (k) participation and savings behavior. Q. J. Econ. 116 (4), 1149–1187. doi:10.1162/003355301753265543

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malchi, A., and Ben-Porat, G. (2018). Home and away: Volunteering among Ultra-Orthodox men in Israel. Int. J. Sociol. Soc. Policy 38, 411–425. doi:10.1108/ijssp-06-2017-0086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MarketResearch.com (2021). Disposable tableware market share, size global growth prospects, trends, industry analysis, key players and forecast to 2027. Avaialble at: https://www.marketwatch.com/press-release/disposable-tableware-market-share-size-global-growth-prospects-trends-industry-analysis-key-players-and-forecast-to-2027-2021-12-06.

Google Scholar

Matsiori, S. K. (2020). Application of the new environmental paradigm to Greece: A critical case study. Econ. Analysis Policy 66, 335–344. doi:10.1016/j.eap.2020.02.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKay, J. E., Mangunjaya, F. M., Dinata, Y., Harrop, S. R., and Khalid, F. (2013). Practise what you preach: A faith-based approach to conservation in Indonesia. Oryx 48 (1), 23–29. doi:10.1017/s0030605313001087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mertens, S., Herberz, M., Hahnel, U. J., and Brosch, T. (2022). The effectiveness of nudging: A meta-analysis of choice architecture interventions across behavioral domains. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 119 (1), e2107346118. doi:10.1073/pnas.2107346118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mónus, F. (2021). Environmental perceptions and pro-environmental behavior–comparing different measuring approaches. Environ. Educ. Res. 27 (1), 132–156. ‏. doi:10.1080/13504622.2020.1842332

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moyer, J. (2015). Faith-based sustainability in practice: Cases studies from Kenya. J. Study Relig. Nat. Cult. 9 (1), 42–67. doi:10.1558/jsrnc.v9i1.17758

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Münscher, R., Vetter, M., and Scheuerle, T. (2016). A review and taxonomy of choice architecture techniques. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 29 (5), 511–524. doi:10.1002/bdm.1897

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilan, P. (2020). Muslim youth environmentalists in Indonesia. J. Youth Stud. 24, 925–940. doi:10.1080/13676261.2020.1782864

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niskanen, O., Tienhaara, A., Haltia, E., and Pouta, E. (2021). Farmers’ heterogeneous preferences towards results-based environmental policies. Land Use Policy 102, 105227. doi:10.1016/j.landusepol.2020.105227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

OECD (2017). Tackling environmental problems with the help of behavioural insights. Paris: OECD Publishing. doi:10.1787/9789264273887-en

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pe’er, E., Feldman, Y., Gamliel, E., Sahar, L., Tikotsky, A., Hod, N., et al. (2019). Do minorities like nudges? The role of group norms in attitudes towards behavioral policy. Judgm. Decis. Mak., 14(1), 40.‏.

Google Scholar

Pelletier, L. G., and Sharp, E. (2008). Persuasive communication and proenvironmental behaviours: How message tailoring and message framing can improve the integration of behaviours through self-determined motivation. Can. Psychology/Psychologie Can. 49 (3), 210–217. doi:10.1037/a0012755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, G. L., Richardson, S. J., Harré, N., Hodges, D., Lyver, P. O. B., Maseyk, F. J., et al. (2021). Evaluating the role of social norms in fostering pro-environmental behaviors. Front. Environ. Sci. 160, 620125. doi:10.3389/fenvs.2021.620125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rice, G. (2006). Pro-environmental behavior in Egypt: Is there a role for Islamic environmental ethics? J. Bus. ethics 65 (4), 373–390. doi:10.1007/s10551-006-0010-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, T., and Feller, A. (2018). Reducing student absences at scale by targeting parents’ misbeliefs. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2 (5), 335–342. doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0328-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

K. Ruggeri (Editor) (2021). Psychology and behavioral economics: Applications for public policy. 2nd Edn. Routledge. doi:10.4324/9781003181873

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarkar, B., and Bhuniya, S. (2022). A sustainable flexible manufacturing–remanufacturing model with improved service and green investment under variable demand. Expert Syst. Appl. 202, 117154. ‏. doi:10.1016/j.eswa.2022.117154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarkar, B., Ullah, M., and Sarkar, M. (2022). Environmental and economic sustainability through innovative green products by remanufacturing. J. Clean. Prod. 332, 129813. ‏. doi:10.1016/j.jclepro.2021.129813

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schubert, C. (2017). Green nudges: Do they work? Are they ethical? Ecol. Econ. 132, 329–342. doi:10.1016/j.ecolecon.2016.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shahzalal, M. D., and Hassan, A. (2019). Communicating sustainability: Using community media to influence rural people’s intention to adopt sustainable behaviour. Sustainability 11 (3), 812. doi:10.3390/su11030812

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shan, J., Li, J., and Xu, Z. (2019). Estimating ecological damage caused by green tides in the yellow sea: A choice experiment approach incorporating extended theory of planned behavior. Ocean Coast. Manag. 181, 104901. doi:10.1016/j.ocecoaman.2019.104901

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shang, J., and Croson, R. (2009). A field experiment in charitable contribution: The impact of social information on the voluntary provision of public goods. Econ. J. 119 (540), 1422–1439. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0297.2009.02267.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A., and Pulver, S. (2009). Ethics-based environmentalism in practice: Religious- environmental organizations in the United States. Worldviews. 13, 145–179. doi:10.1163/156853509x438580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soman, D., and Hossain, T. (2021). Successfully scaled solutions need not be homogenous. Behav. Public Policy 5 (1), 80–89. doi:10.1017/bpp.2020.24

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somerwill, L., and Wehn, U. (2022). How to measure the impact of citizen science on environmental attitudes, behaviour and knowledge? A review of state-of-the-art approaches. Environ. Sci. Eur. 34 (1), 18–29. ‏. doi:10.1186/s12302-022-00596-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steg, L., and Vlek, C. (2009). Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: An integrative review and research agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 29 (3), 309–317. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szaszi, B., Palinkas, A., Palfi, B., Szollosi, A., Aczel, B., and Szollosi, A. (2018). A systematic scoping review of the choice architecture movement: Toward understanding when and why nudges work. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 31 (3), 355–366. doi:10.1002/bdm.2035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, B., Van Wieren, G., and Zaleha, B. (2016). The greening of religion hypothesis (part two): Assessing the data from Lynn White, Jr, to Pope Francis. J. Study Relig. Nat. Cult. 10 (3), 306–378. doi:10.1558/jsrnc.v10i3.29011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tchetchik, A., Zvi, L. I., Kaplan, S., and Blass, V. (2020). The joint effects of driving hedonism and trialability on the choice between internal combustion engine, hybrid, and electric vehicles. Technol. Forecast. Soc. Change 151, 119815. ‏. doi:10.1016/j.techfore.2019.119815

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tchetchik, A., Kaplan, S., and Blass, V. (2021). Recycling and consumption reduction following the COVID-19 lockdown: The effect of threat and coping appraisal, past behavior and information. Resour. Conservation Recycl. 167, 105370. ‏. doi:10.1016/j.resconrec.2020.105370

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaler, R. H., and Benartzi, S. (2004). Save more tomorrow™: Using behavioral economics to increase employee saving. J. Political Econ. 112 (S1), S164–S187. doi:10.1086/380085

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaler, R. H., and Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health. Wealth, Happiness 6, 14–38.

Google Scholar

Train, K. (2003). Discrete choice methods with simulation. Cambridge University Press First edition.

Google Scholar

Train, K., and Weeks, M. (2005). Discrete choice models in preference space and willingness-to-pay space. In Applications of simulation methods in environmental and resource economics (pp. 1–16). Springer, Dordrecht.‏.

Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C., and Gelfand, M. J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. J. personality Soc. Psychol. 74 (1), 118–128. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsimpo, C., and Wodon, C. (2016). Faith affiliation, religiosity, and attitudes towards the environment and climate change. Rev. Faith Int. Aff. 14 (3), 51–64. doi:10.1080/15570274.2016.1215850

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1986). Rational choice and the framing of decisions. J. Bus. 59 (4), S251–S278. doi:10.1086/296365

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veldman, R. G., Szasz, A., and Haluza-DeLay, R. (2013). How the world's religions are responding to climate change. Taylor and Francis. ‏.

Google Scholar

Vidal-Mones, B., Diaz-Ruiz, R., and Gil, J. M. (2022). From evaluation to action: Testing nudging strategies to prevent food waste in school canteens. Waste Manag. 140, 90–99. doi:10.1016/j.wasman.2022.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, T. P. (2017). Reducing single-use plastic shopping bags in the USA. Waste Manag. 70, 3–12.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, B. S., and Hayhoe, Doug (2017). Assessing the influence of an educational presentation on climate change beliefs at an evangelical christian college. J. Geoscience Educ. 65 (3), 272–282. National Association of Geoscience Teachers. doi:10.5408/16-220.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, E. O. (2006). The creation: An appeal to save life on earth. New York, NY: Norton.

Google Scholar

World Wildlife Fund for Nature (2020). The blue recovery of the mediterranean: The sea holds the future of the whole region. Available at: https://www.wwf.mg/?364330/The-Blue-Recovery-of-the-Mediterranean-the-sea-holds-the-future-of-the-whole-region.

Google Scholar

Xu, Y., Du, J., Khan, M. A. S., Jin, S., Altaf, M., Anwar, F., et al. (2022). Effects of subjective norms and environmental mechanism on green purchase behavior: An extended model of theory of planned behavior. Front. Environ. Sci., 39.‏, 779629 doi:10.3389/fenvs.2022.779629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoeli, E., Hoffman, M., Rand, D. G., and Nowak, M. A. (2013). Powering up with indirect reciprocity in a large-scale field experiment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 110 (2), 10424–10429. doi:10.1073/pnas.1301210110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoreh, T. (2010). Ultra-Orthodox recycling narratives: Implications for planning and policy. J. Enterprising Communities People Places Glob. Econ. 4 (4), 323–345. doi:10.1108/17506201011086129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoreh, T. (2011). “Involuntary simplicity: A case study of haredi consumption patterns in Canada and Israel,” in From antiquity to the post-modern world: Contemporary jewish studies in Canada. Editors D. Maoz, and A. Gondos (UK: Cambridge, 235–237.

Google Scholar

Yoreh, T. (2019a). Consumption, wastefulness, and simplicity in Ultra-Orthodox communities. Stud. Jud. Humanit. Soc. Sci. 2 (2), 137–152.

Google Scholar

Yoreh, T. (2019b). Rethinking jewish approaches to wastefulness. Rev. Rabbin. Jud. 22 (1), 31–45. doi:10.1163/15700704-12341350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoreh, T. (2020). Broadening the tent: Moral language and conservative environmentalism. Berkley Forum: Berkley Center for Religion, Peace and World Affairs. Available at: https://berkleycenter.georgetown.edu/responses/broadening-the-tent-moral-language-and-conservative-environmentalism (Accessed October 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Zemo, K. H., and Nigus, H. Y. (2021). Does religion promote pro-environmental behaviour? A cross-country investigation. J. Environ. Econ. Policy 10 (1), 90–113. doi:10.1080/21606544.2020.1796820

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Z., and Wang, X. (2020). Nudging to promote household waste source separation: Mechanisms and spillover effects. Resour. Conservation Recycl. 162, 105054. doi:10.1016/j.resconrec.2020.105054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: single-use plastics (SUP), disposable tableware, green nudge, discrete choice experiment (DCE), faith-based communities, pro-environmental behavior (PEB), ultra-orthodox

Citation: Kaufmann D, Tikotsky A, Yoreh T and Tchetchik A (2023) Engaging faith-based communities in pro-environmental behavior using soft regulations: The case of single-use plastics. Front. Environ. Sci. 10:1019904. doi: 10.3389/fenvs.2022.1019904

Received: 15 August 2022; Accepted: 07 November 2022;
Published: 04 January 2023.

Edited by:

Syed Mithun Ali, Bangladesh University of Engineering and Technology, Bangladesh

Reviewed by:

Rekha Guchhait, Yonsei University, South Korea
Bikash Koli Dey, Hongik University, South Korea

Copyright © 2023 Kaufmann, Tikotsky, Yoreh and Tchetchik. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anat Tchetchik, anat.tchetchik@biu.ac.il

Download