Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 30 July 2025

Sec. Nutritional Epidemiology

Volume 12 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnut.2025.1605016

This article is part of the Research TopicEating Behavior and Chronic Diseases: Research Evidence from Population Studies, Volume IIView all 12 articles

Dietary adherence and cognitive performance in older adults: insights from a nationwide survey in China

Yameng FanYameng Fan1Yan Yu
Yan Yu2*Jiao TanJiao Tan1Yonghong MaYonghong Ma1Ke Men
Ke Men1*
  • 1School of Public Health, Xi’an Medical University, Xi’an, China
  • 2School of Public Health, Xi’an Jiaotong University, Xi’an, China

Background: Dietary guidelines, integrating the latest scientific evidence with traditional dietary culture, are regularly issued to provide dietary recommendations and promote health. However, the relationship between adherence to dietary guidelines and cognitive health has yet to be fully elucidated. In this study, we specifically examine the association between adherence to the Dietary Guidelines for Chinese (DGC) and cognitive performance among older adults.

Methods: We analyzed data from the China Health and Nutrition Survey (CHNS) (2011 and 2015), including participants aged ≥60 years. Adherence to the DGC was assessed using the Chinese Healthy Eating Index (CHEI). Cognitive performance was evaluated across the following three domains: (1) memory (immediate and delayed recall), (2) attention/executive function (counting backward from 20), and (3) processing speed (serial 7 subtraction). A composite z-score was derived to represent global cognition. Multivariate logistic regression and restricted cubic spline models were used to assess associations.

Results: Among 2,174 older adults, higher CHEI scores were significantly associated with better performance at counting backward (attention/executive function, p < 0.05), serial 7 subtraction (processing speed, p < 0.01), and global cognition (p < 0.01). A significant interaction effect was observed between the CHEI and nationality in relation to cognitive performance (pinteraction <0.05). Among the 17 CHEI components, only adherence to the recommended intake of fish and seafood (OR: 0.55; 95% CI: 0.37, 0.82) was independently linked to better cognitive performance. Sensitivity analyses excluding individuals with diabetes or overweight or obesity, and treating cognitive performance as a continuous outcome yielded consistent results (p < 0.05).

Conclusion: Greater adherence to the DGC was associated with better cognitive performance, particularly in processing speed and attention/executive function. Promoting adherence to dietary guidelines may be a viable strategy for preserving cognitive health in aging populations.

Introduction

With the advances in modern medicine and economy, population aging has become a global phenomenon. According to its seventh population census conducted in 2020, China has 264 million people aged 60 years and above, accounting for 18.7 percent of its population of 1.4 billion (1). In this context, age-related degeneration, especially cognitive decline among the elderly, has become a major public health concern.

To an extent, cognitive decline heralds the development of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and different types of dementia. Although the occurrence of dementia is closely related to age, it is not an inevitable consequence of aging. Around 40% of worldwide dementia could theoretically be prevented or delayed based on the 2020 report of the Lancet Commission (2). Accordingly, it is imperative to identify the potentially modifiable risk factors to promote cognitive health.

Diet, as a modifiable factor, is closely related to daily life. Accumulating evidence suggests that individual foods or nutrients, such as fruit, fish, vitamins and polyunsaturated fatty acids, are associated with cognitive performance among the elderly (36). Considering the cooperation and antagonism between nutrients and foods, contemporary research examining the link between diet and cognition has gradually transitioned into assessments of overall diet quality.

Diet quality is typically assessed using either guideline-based scoring systems [e.g., Chinese Dietary Guidelines Index (CDGI), Chinese Diet Quality Index (CDQI), and Chinese Healthy Eating Index (CHEI)] or pattern-based indices [e.g., Mediterranean Diet (MED) and Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH)]. Previous studies have primarily focused on diet quality assessed by the MED pattern, but this dietary pattern may not be appropriate for non-Mediterranean countries, such as the U.S., Denmark, and China (7). Investigating the associations between adherence to national dietary guidelines and cognitive performance in local populations holds greater practical significance. However, such research remains relatively limited and controversial (8, 9). Compared to existing Chinese diet quality indices, the CHEI offers three distinctive advantages: (1) it incorporates a more comprehensive range of food components, (2) it is designed as a continuous scoring system, and (3) it is the first index to quantify food consumption using standardized portion sizes as recommended in the DGC. These features enhance its validity for assessing dietary quality in Chinese populations.

Based on the data from the China Health and Nutrition Survey (CHNS) for 2011 and 2015, we examined whether alignment with the Dietary Guidelines for Chinese (DGC) measured using the CHEI, was related to cognitive performance among the Chinese elderly.

Methods

Study population

The data used in this study were publicly available from the CHNS,1 which is an ongoing cohort study that was initiated in 1989 and followed up in 1991, 1993, 1997, 2000, 2004, 2006, 2009, 2011, and 2015 (the latest open-access date). The methodological framework and operational protocols of the CHNS have been extensively documented (10). Briefly, the CHNS employs a multistage random cluster sampling approach to select representative individuals from 15 provinces (municipalities or autonomous regions) across China. A primary objective of the CHNS is to assess the nutritional, socioeconomic, and health status of the Chinese population.

Our analysis used data from the two most recent available waves (2011 and 2015) to explore the associations between adherence to the DGC and cognitive performance. Dietary and baseline information was collected from the 2011 wave (as a baseline), and cognitive performance was assessed using the 2015 wave. The following exclusions were applied to the 3,754 subjects aged 60 years or older at the baseline: (1) those with missing dietary data or extreme energy intake (<800 or >6,000 kcal/day for males and <600 or >4,000 kcal/day for females); (2) those with mental/psychiatric disorders, intellectual disability, or neurological disorders; (3) those lost to follow-up in the 2015 wave; (4) those with missing cognitive data; and (5) those with missing covariate data. Subsequently, a total of 2,174 participants were included in the final analysis (Figure 1). As shown in Supplementary Table S1, participants completing follow-up were significantly younger (67.8 vs. 70.9 years), more likely to live in rural site, less likely to have diabetes, and had higher energy intake compared to those lost to follow-up. No other significant differences were observed between the groups.

Figure 1
Flowchart detailing the selection process of elderly participants in the CHNS 2011 study. Starting with 3754 participants, 125 were excluded for missing dietary data, 31 for mental/psychiatric disorders, 1072 for follow-up loss, 94 for missing cognitive data, and 259 for missing covariates, resulting in a final sample of 2174 participants.

Figure 1. Flow diagram of the study sample selection.

Dietary assessment and the calculation of the CHEI

Dietary intake was assessed using three consecutive 24-h dietary recalls at the individual level and a food inventory weighing method at the household level. Daily energy and nutrient intakes were estimated using the China food composition databases (2002 and 2004 versions). Proportions of condiments (such as cooking oils, sodium, and added sugars) at the household level were allocated to individuals based on their calculated energy intake at the individual level.

The CHEI was developed and validated by Yuan et al. (11, 12) to assess adherence to the DGC. The CHEI comprises 17 components (food groups or nutrients), including 12 adequacy components (total grains, whole grains and mixed beans, tubers, total vegetables, dark vegetables, fruit, dairy, soybeans, fish and seafood, poultry, eggs, and seeds and nuts) and 5 moderation components (red meat, cooking oil, sodium, added sugar, and alcohol). After converting gram values to standard portion (SP) units, the scoring of these components was calculated per 1,000 kcal of energy density, with the exception of alcohol (expressed as an absolute weight) and added sugars (expressed as a percentage of energy). The detailed scoring criteria for individual components are presented in Supplementary Table S2. Individual component scores were independently calculated and aggregated to generate a composite score, which ranged from 0 to 100 points. Elevated scores reflected stronger adherence to the DGC.

Assessment of cognitive performance

A subset of items from the modified Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS-m) was administered to individuals in the CHNS who were 55 years of age or older. These items included immediate and delayed recall of a 10-word list (20 points), counting backward from 20 (2 points), and serial 7 subtraction (5 points).

1. Immediate and delayed recall of a 10-word list: This item was used to evaluate the cognitive subdomain of memory. Participants were asked to memorize 10 unrelated words within 2 min for the immediate recall test and to recall these words at the end of the session for the delayed recall test. The score was based on the total number of correctly recalled words.

2. Counting backward from 20: This item was used to assess the cognitive subdomain of attention/executive function. Participants were asked to count backward from 20 to 1. Scores were assigned as follows: 2 points for a correct response on the first attempt, 1 point for a correct response on the second attempt, and 0 points for incorrect responses on both attempts.

3. Serial 7 subtraction: This item was designed to evaluate the cognitive subdomain of processing speed. Participants were requested to execute five successive subtractions of 7 starting from 100. One point was awarded for each correct subtraction.

Global cognitive performance was calculated using aggregating z-scores [(test score − mean score)/standard deviation (SD)] for the above items. Elevated values for all items corresponded with superior cognitive performance. The lowest quartile of z-scores based on age group (60–69, ≥70) was defined as poor cognitive performance, which was consistent with previously used methods (13, 14).

Covariates

Sociodemographic variables such as age, gender (male and female), nationality (Han and minority), location (urban site and rural site), education level (middle school and below, and high school and above), and income (inflation-adjusted household income per capita, categorized into tertiles as low, middle, and high) were collected.

Lifestyle variables included participants who had smoked at least one cigarette per day, who were defined as smokers, and participants who had consumed alcohol in the past year, who were defined as drinkers. Leisure sedentary time was assessed based on the total time spent sitting in a week (excluding time spent sleeping).

Body measurements, were obtained, including weight (kg) and standing height (cm). Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight divided by the square of height.

Following a 10-min seated rest period, blood pressure was measured according to standardized protocols. Three sequential readings were recorded, with the mean systolic (SBP) and diastolic (DBP) blood pressure values used for the analysis. Hypertension was operationally defined as follows: (1) average SBP ≥140 mmHg, (2) average DBP ≥90 mmHg, or (3) self-reported use of physician-prescribed antihypertensive medications. Diabetes was defined based on a physician’s diagnosis (self-reported).

Statistical analysis

The characteristics of the study population were described as the mean ± SD for continuous variables and the number (percentage) for categorical variables. The CHEI was categorized into quartiles (quartile 1: <25th percentile; quartile 2: ≥25 to 50th percentiles; quartile 3: ≥50 to 75th percentiles; and quartile 4: ≥75th percentiles). Differences in the characteristics between groups were tested using a one-way analysis of variance for continuous variables and a chi-square test for categorical variables. The relationships between the CHEI and cognitive performance were evaluated using binary logistic regression models. We considered the median CHEI value within each quartile to be a continuous variable to examine the trends associated with ascending quartiles of the CHEI. In Model 1, adjustments were made for age, gender, and nationality. In Model 2, additional adjustments were incorporated for location, energy intake, drinking status, smoking status, and leisure sedentary time. Model 3 was further adjusted for education, income, BMI, hypertension, and diabetes. Restricted cubic splines with knots at the 5th, 50th, and 95th percentiles of the exposure distribution were applied to examine to explore the dose–response relationships between the CHEI (as a continuous variable) and poor cognitive performance based on the SAS macro “%RCS_Reg” (15). Sensitivity analyses were performed by excluding participants with (1) diabetes or (2) overweight or obesity, and (3) treating cognitive performance as a continuous outcome. Furthermore, we performed stratified analyses to determine if these associations varied according to different factors, such as age group (60–69 and ≥70), gender (male and female), or nationality (Han and other minorities). For exploratory purposes, we investigated the relationship between CHEI components (dichotomized as meeting or not meeting the DGC) and poor cognitive performance to determine if statistically significant associations exited that could be ascribed to particular components. STATA version 16.0 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, United States) was employed for data management and the main analyses. SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, United States) was used for the dose–response curve analyses. A two-tailed p-value <0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.

Results

Descriptive statistics

The baseline characteristics of participants across the CHEI quartiles are shown in Table 1. Participants with a higher CHEI were more likely to be Han and non-smokers; live in urban site; have diabetes; have a higher BMI, education level, and income; and have increased leisure sedentary time (p < 0.05). We did not observe any significant disparities for age, gender, energy intake, drinking status, or hypertension across the quartiles of the CHEI.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of the overall target population according to CHEI quartilesa.

CHEI and poor cognitive performance

As shown in Table 2, the highest CHEI quartile in Model 1 was significantly linked to decreased odds of poor cognitive performance (OR: 0.38; 95% CI: 0.27, 0.53; p < 0.001) compared to the lowest CHEI quartile. The relationship between the CHEI and poor cognitive performance was attenuated, but remained significant, after additional adjustments in Model 2 (OR: 0.48; 95% CI: 0.34, 0.68; p < 0.001) and Model 3 (OR: 0.60; 95% CI: 0.42, 0.85; p < 0.01). A significant linear trend was observed, indicating that the odds of poor cognitive performance significantly decreased with an increase in CHEI quartiles (ptrend <0.01). A continuous variable analysis revealed that a 1-SD increment in the CHEI was significantly linked to decreased odds of poor cognitive performance (p < 0.01).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals of the CHEI for poor performancea.

A dose–response relationship between the CHEI and poor cognitive performance was observed. As shown in Figure 2, the CHEI was negatively associated with poor cognitive performance in a linear manner (poverall association = 0.0161; pnonlinear association = 0.2520).

Figure 2
Graph displaying the odds ratio (OR) and 95% confidence interval (CI) for CHDI, with a red line indicating trends and dashed lines for CI. Overall association p-value is 0.0161, and the nonlinear association p-value is 0.2520. The OR decreases as CHDI increases.

Figure 2. Dose–response relationship between the CHEI and poor cognitive performance. Models were adjusted for age, gender, nationality, location, energy intake, drinking status, smoking status, leisure sedentary time, education level, income, BMI, hypertension, and diabetes.

CHEI and different cognitive subdomains

Table 3 displays the associations between the CHEI and poor cognitive performance for different subdomains. Higher CHEI was associated with significantly lower fully adjusted odds of poor cognitive performance in the counting backward from 20 test (ptrend <0.05) and the serial 7 subtraction test (ptrend <0.01). No significant association was observed between the CHEI and immediate and delayed recall.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals of the CHEI for poor performance in different cognitive domainsa.

Interaction effects

As shown in Figure 3, a potential interaction effect was identified between the CHEI and nationality in relation to poor cognitive performance (pinteraction = 0.021). In the fully adjusted model, each 1-SD increment of the CHEI was linked to decreased odds of poor cognitive performance among the Han population (OR: 0.80; 95% CI: 0.70, 0.92), but not among other minority populations (OR: 1.12; 95% CI: 0.78, 1.62). No significant interaction effects were observed between the CHEI and age or gender (pinteraction >0.05).

Figure 3
Forest plot showing adjusted odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) for different subgroups: age (60–69, ≥70), gender (male, female), and nationality (Han, other). Overall adjusted OR is 0.84 (0.74, 0.95). Age subgroup shows ORs of 0.80 (0.66, 0.96) and 0.87 (0.73, 1.04). Gender subgroup shows ORs of 0.87 (0.72, 1.04) and 0.82 (0.69, 0.97). Nationality shows ORs of 0.80 (0.70, 0.92) and 1.12 (0.78, 1.62). Pinteraction values indicate interaction significance with a notable value of 0.021 for nationality.

Figure 3. Associations of 1 SD increase in the CHEI with poor cognitive performance stratified by demographic characteristics (age, gender, and nationality). Models were adjusted for age, gender (except for gender subgroup analysis), nationality (except for nationality subgroup analysis), location, energy intake, drinking status, smoking status, leisure sedentary time, education level, income, BMI, hypertension, and diabetes.

CHEI components and poor cognitive performance

Given the significant associations between the CHEI and poor cognitive performance, we conducted an exploratory analysis of the relationships between each CHEI component and poor cognitive performance. We adjusted for all covariates that were part of Model 3, along with other components. Of the 17 components, only adherence to the recommended consumption of fish and seafood was significantly associated with better cognitive performance (OR: 0.55; 95% CI: 0.37, 0.82) (Figure 4).

Figure 4
Forest plot showing adjusted odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals for various food groups. Significant associations are seen for fish and seafood (0.55, p = 0.003). Other food groups show non-significant results, with confidence intervals crossing one.

Figure 4. Associations of the CHEI components with poor cognitive performance. Models were adjusted for age, gender, energy intake, nationality, location, drinking status, smoking status, leisure sedentary time, education level, income, BMI, hypertension, diabetes, and other components.

Sensitivity analyses

The association between the CHEI and poor cognitive performance did not appreciably change in the sensitivity analyses after excluding participants who suffered from diabetes (n = 2,026; Supplementary Table S3) or overweight or obesity (n = 1,200; Supplementary Table S4). When treating cognitive performance as a continuous outcome (using composite z-scores), the association remained statistically significant in both linear regression (Supplementary Table S5) and dose–response analyses (Supplementary Figure S1), consistent with our primary categorical analysis.

Discussion

In this study, we observed that compliance with the DGC, assessed using the CHEI, was associated with better cognitive performance among older Chinese adults (except for the memory subdomain). A negative relationship between the CHEI and poor cognitive performance was only observed among the Han nationality. Exploratory analyses based on CHEI components revealed a significant association between adherence to the recommended consumption levels of fish and seafood and superior cognitive performance, which may be attributed to the rich content of omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids in fish and seafood, especially docosahexaenoic acid (DHA).

Previous studies have explored the relationship between diet quality and cognitive health among older adults, but most of them have focused on diet quality assessed using the MED pattern (16, 17). The traditional Chinese dietary pattern differs from the MED pattern in terms of the food composition and cooking methods. The diet-quality scoring system based on the MED pattern only considers the relative intake levels of food or nutrients (below or above the median of the target population) rather than the actual intake levels, which may limit the practical guiding significance. In this study, we evaluated diet quality based on national dietary guidelines to more accurately reflect the real eating habits of the local population (e.g., food preferences, cooking methods, etc.). In addition, several cognition-specific indices, such as the Mediterranean-Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay (MIND) diet and the Oriental Intervention for Enhanced Neurocognitive Health (ORIENT) diet, have been developed and shown to exert significant neuroprotective effects (18, 19). Unlike these dietary patterns, which primarily emphasize neuroprotective foods, the CHEI provides a more comprehensive evaluation of overall dietary quality by assessing 17 components—including both adequacy and moderation metrics—thereby facilitating the capture of multiple health outcomes beyond cognition. More importantly, the CHEI’s alignment with China’s national dietary recommendations enhances its public health relevance, making it easier to translate into clinical and policy interventions for the Chinese population. Future studies incorporating both guideline-based and cognition-specific indices may further clarify their complementary roles across different populations.

Research on diet quality based on national dietary guidelines and associations with cognitive outcomes is relatively limited and has primarily been conducted in Western countries. Adherence to dietary guidelines has been associated with better cognitive performance, both cross-sectionally (in the Australia and U.S.) (9, 20) and longitudinally (over 15 years in the Netherlands and 11 years in the U.S.) (21, 22). To the best of our knowledge, there are two studies exploring the association between CHEI and cognitive health among the Chinese population. In the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Study (CLHLS), a focused analysis of the mediating role of psychological balance and depressive symptoms between CHEI and cognitive function among rural older adults was conducted (23). Compared with this study, our research specifically enrolled older adults from both urban and rural sites. We conducted a comprehensive investigation of the relationship between the CHEI and cognitive function, including: (1) main association analysis, (2) cognitive subdomain analysis, (3) CHEI components-specific analysis, (4) stratified analysis, and (5) sensitivity analysis. Another study from CHNS (2004 & 2006) suggested that better diet quality assessed by CHEI improves cognitive performance in Chinese adults aged 55+ (24). Compared with this earlier work, our research possesses four key methodological improvements: (1) utilization of the latest open-access CHNS data (2011 & 2015) focusing on Chinese older adults aged ≥60 years; (2) standardized cognitive assessment using z-scores rather than raw scores; (3) comprehensive adjustment for potential confounders, including ethnicity and energy intake; and (4) rigorous additional analyses examining both cognitive subdomains and CHEI dietary components. Most notably, we made three novel discoveries: first, adherence to the DGC showed domain-specific cognitive benefits. Second, the Han population exhibited significant positive associations that were not observed in ethnic minorities. Third, component-level analysis identified fish and seafood intake as the primary driver of these associations.

We observed that adherence to the DGC was related to processing speed and attention/executive function cognitive subdomains, but not to memory. Surprisingly, our previous study based on National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) data also discovered that compliance with the 2015–2020 Dietary Guidelines for Americans (DGA), as evaluated using the Healthy Eating Index-2015 (HEI-2015), was linked to better processing speed and executive function, but no associations were observed for memory in U.S. adults aged 60 years and above (25). Another study involving 819 older adults in Australia indicated that adherence to the MED was only related to better visuospatial cognition and not to any other cognitive subdomains (26). These findings indicate that particular healthy eating patterns might only confer advantages to certain specific subdomains of cognition. Future studies should incorporate multiple cognitive tests to comprehensively and thoroughly assess cognitive performance.

Our results are distinctively demonstrated that compliance with the DGC was positively correlated with cognitive performance for the Han nationality, but not for other minorities. Although existing research has yet to fully clarify why the link between diet and cognitive function varies across nationalities, potential factors leading to such national disparities may include dietary habits, food availability, education levels, traditional culture, and media influence. Previous studies have reported that meal behavior and dietary intake differed greatly in different Chinese nationalities (27, 28). Moreover, research has shown that the apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε2 allele exhibits a protective effect against cognitive impairment exclusively in the Han, but not in the Hui—the largest minority population in China—suggesting distinct nationality-specific risks for cognitive impairment (29). Another contributing factor could be insufficient sample sizes in minority group analyses, reducing statistical reliability. For example, a protective effect of adherence to the MED on cognition was only observed in non-Hispanic White Americans, who had a larger sample size, but not in minority groups with smaller sample sizes, such as non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic/Latino, or Asian Americans (30). Conversely, in a study with a large sample of Hispanic/Latino Americans, significant associations between diet and cognition were observed (31). Overall, different diet strategies might thus be emphasized for different Chinese nationalities.

Better cognitive performance was observed in those who aligned with the recommended intake of fish and seafood. A meta-analysis incorporating 21 prospective cohorts revealed that each additional weekly serving of fish (approximately 105 g) was associated with a 5% reduction in the risk of dementia (32). Findings from two large prospective cohort studies, the Nurses’ Health Study and the Health Professionals Follow-up Study in the U.S., indicated that a daily increase to three servings of fish was associated with a 7% lower risk of subjective cognitive decline (33). A similar finding was also reported in China. A cohort study based on the CHNS demonstrated that daily fish consumption of at least 100 g was related to better cognitive performance among adults aged 65 years and above in China, suggesting a potential protective role of fish in cognitive health (6). The specific mechanisms underlying the protective benefits of fish consumption on cognitive health may be related to the rich content of omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids in fish, including alpha-linolenic acid (ALA), eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA), and DHA. In particular, DHA, which is widely distributed in neural tissues, is a critical component of synaptic membranes. It plays a beneficial role in regulating neurogenesis, neuronal membrane permeability, synaptic plasticity, and neurotransmission (34). DHA has the capacity to mitigate both inflammation and oxidative stress within the central nervous system, thereby protecting neurons and inhibiting apoptosis (35, 36).

Our research has several strengths. For example, the CHNS data used are widely representative of mainland China. This study is also the first to demonstrate that adherence to the DGC differentially correlates with specific cognitive subdomains. The association between fish consumption and cognition in older adults was explored after controlling for other food components. Several limitations should also be noted. First, although this prospective study excluded individuals with mental/psychiatric disorders, intellectual disabilities, or neurological diseases, the absence of baseline cognitive assessments and relatively short follow-up period precluded definitive causal inferences. Second, while the three 24-h dietary recalls represent a validated assessment method, they may inadequately reflect long-term dietary patterns, potentially attenuating true diet-cognition associations. Additionally, although baseline dietary data are commonly used in nutritional epidemiology, this approach cannot account for dietary changes during follow-up. Repeated dietary measurements in future studies could mitigate this limitation. Finally, compared to those lost to follow-up, retained participants were younger, had lower diabetes prevalence, and higher energy intake, which may affect the generalizability of our findings.

Conclusion

We observed that adherence to the DGC was linked to global cognitive performance as well as specific cognitive subdomains such as processing speed and attention/executive function among older Chinese adults. Considering both the potential significance and the constraints of this study, future research should verify these results using larger prospective cohort studies.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/china.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill and the National Institute for Nutrition and Health, Chinese Center for Disease Control and Prevention. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

YF: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft. YY: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. JT: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft. YM: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft. KM: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was funded by Scientific Research Foundation of Xi’an Medical College (Program No. 2023BS26), Scientific Research Program Funded by Education Department of Shaanxi Provincial Government (Program No. 24JK0644), and Natural Science Basic Research Program of Shaanxi (Program No. 2025JC-YBQN-1102).

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all participants and investigators for the support of data collection on CHNS.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2025.1605016/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

References

1. Tu, W, Zeng, X, and Liu, Q. Aging tsunami coming: the main finding from China’s seventh national population census. Aging Clin Exp Res. (2022) 34:1159–63. doi: 10.1007/s40520-021-02017-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Livingston, G, Huntley, J, Sommerlad, A, Ames, D, Ballard, C, Banerjee, S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. (2020) 396:413–46. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Huang, L, Zhao, C, Gao, M, Tao, Y, Chen, X, Chen, H, et al. Associations of vegetable and fruit intake with cognitive function and its decline: two longitudinal studies. J Nutr Health Aging. (2024) 28:100223. doi: 10.1016/j.jnha.2024.100223

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Djuricic, I, and Calder, PC. Beneficial outcomes of omega-6 and omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids on human health: an update for 2021. Nutrients. (2021) 13:2421. doi: 10.3390/nu13072421

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Watson, J, Lee, M, and Garcia-Casal, MN. Consequences of inadequate intakes of vitamin A, vitamin B12, vitamin D, calcium, iron, and folate in older persons. Curr Geriatr Rep. (2018) 7:103–13. doi: 10.1007/s13670-018-0241-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Qin, B, Plassman, BL, Edwards, LJ, Popkin, BM, Adair, LS, and Mendez, MA. Fish intake is associated with slower cognitive decline in Chinese older adults. J Nutr. (2014) 144:1579–85. doi: 10.3945/jn.114.193854

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Da Silva, R, Bach-Faig, A, Raido Quintana, B, Buckland, G, Vaz De Almeida, MD, and Serra-Majem, L. Worldwide variation of adherence to the Mediterranean diet, in 1961–1965 and 2000–2003. Public Health Nutr. (2009) 12:1676–84. doi: 10.1017/S1368980009990541

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Chen, X, Liu, Z, Sachdev, PS, Kochan, NA, Brodaty, H, and O'Leary, F. Association of adherence to the Australian Dietary Guidelines with cognitive performance and cognitive decline in the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study: a longitudinal analysis. J Nutr Sci. (2021) 10:e86. doi: 10.1017/jns.2021.44

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Li, PF, McEvoy, MA, McKiernan, S, Schofield, PW, MacDonald-Wicks, LK, and Patterson, AJ. Diet quality and cognitive performance in Australian adults aged 55–85 years: a cross-sectional analysis of the hunter community study cohort. Nutrients. (2021) 13:909. doi: 10.3390/nu13030909

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Zhang, B, Zhai, FY, Du, SF, and Popkin, BM. The China Health And Nutrition Survey, 1989–2011. Obes Rev. (2014) 15:2–7. doi: 10.1111/obr.12119

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Yuan, Y, Li, F, Wu, H, Wang, Y, Chen, J, He, G, et al. Evaluation of the validity and reliability of the Chinese Healthy Eating Index. Nutrients. (2018) 10:114. doi: 10.3390/nu10020114

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Yuan, Y, Li, F, Dong, R, Chen, J, He, G, Li, S, et al. The development of a Chinese Healthy Eating Index and its application in the general population. Nutrients. (2017) 9:977. doi: 10.3390/nu9090977

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Dong, X, Li, S, Chen, J, Li, Y, Wu, Y, and Zhang, D. Association of dietary omega-3 and omega-6 fatty acids intake with cognitive performance in older adults: National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011–2014. Nutr J. (2020) 19:25. doi: 10.1186/s12937-020-00547-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Wang, R, Wang, W, Hu, P, Zhang, R, Dong, X, and Zhang, D. Association of dietary vitamin D intake, serum 25(OH)D3, 25(OH)D2 with cognitive performance in the elderly. Nutrients. (2021) 13:3089. doi: 10.3390/nu13093089

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Desquilbet, L, and Mariotti, F. Dose-response analyses using restricted cubic spline functions in public health research. Stat Med. (2010) 29:1037–57. doi: 10.1002/sim.3841

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Garcia-Casares, N, Gallego Fuentes, P, Barbancho, MA, Lopez-Gigosos, R, Garcia-Rodriguez, A, and Gutierrez-Bedmar, M. Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment and Mediterranean diet. A systematic review and dose–response meta-analysis. J Clin Med. (2021) 10:4642. doi: 10.3390/jcm10204642

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Coelho-Junior, HJ, Trichopoulou, A, and Panza, F. Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between adherence to Mediterranean diet with physical performance and cognitive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. (2021) 70:101395. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2021.101395

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Huang, L, Tao, Y, Chen, H, Chen, X, Shen, J, Zhao, C, et al. Mediterranean-dietary approaches to stop hypertension intervention for neurodegenerative delay (MIND) diet and cognitive function and its decline: a prospective study and meta-analysis of cohort studies. Am J Clin Nutr. (2023) 118:174–82. doi: 10.1016/j.ajcnut.2023.04.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Chen, L, Zhong, W, Chen, H, Zhou, Y, Ran, W, He, Y, et al. ORIENT diet: a potential neuroprotective dietary pattern for Chinese stroke high-risk population. J Am Med Dir Assoc. (2025) 26:105331. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2024.105331

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Ye, X, Scott, T, Gao, X, Maras, JE, Bakun, PJ, and Tucker, KL. Mediterranean diet, healthy eating index 2005, and cognitive function in middle-aged and older Puerto Rican adults. J Acad Nutr Diet. (2013) 113:276–281.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2012.10.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Nooyens, ACJ, Yildiz, B, Hendriks, LG, Bas, S, van Boxtel, MPJ, Picavet, HSJ, et al. Adherence to dietary guidelines and cognitive decline from middle age: the Doetinchem Cohort Study. Am J Clin Nutr. (2021) 114:871–81. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqab109

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Wengreen, HJ, Neilson, C, Munger, R, and Corcoran, C. Diet quality is associated with better cognitive test performance among aging men and women. J Nutr. (2009) 139:1944–9. doi: 10.3945/jn.109.106427

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Jiang, Z, Xu, Z, Zhou, M, Huijun, Z, and Zhou, S. The influence of healthy eating index on cognitive function in older adults: chain mediation by psychological balance and depressive symptoms. BMC Geriatr. (2024) 24:904. doi: 10.1186/s12877-024-05497-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Xu, Z, Chen, S, Guo, M, Zhang, T, Niu, X, Zhou, Y, et al. The impact of diet quality on cognitive ability of Chinese older adults: evidence from the China Health and Nutrition Survey (CHNS). BMC Geriatr. (2024) 24:55. doi: 10.1186/s12877-023-04630-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Fan, Y, Zhang, Y, Li, J, Liu, Y, Chang, H, Jiang, Y, et al. Association between healthy eating index-2015 and various cognitive domains in us adults aged 60 years or older: the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011–2014. BMC Public Health. (2021) 21:1862. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-11914-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Chen, X, Liu, Z, Sachdev, PS, Kochan, NA, O'Leary, F, and Brodaty, H. Dietary patterns and cognitive health in older adults: findings from the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. J Nutr Health Aging. (2021) 25:255–62. doi: 10.1007/s12603-020-1536-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Ge, K, Zhai, F, and Wang, Q. Effect of nationality on dietary pattern and meal behavior in China. Am J Clin Nutr. (1997) 65:1290S–4S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/65.4.1290S

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Liu, L, Liu, L, Ding, Y, Huang, Z, He, B, Sun, S, et al. Ethnic and environmental differences in various markers of dietary intake and blood pressure among Chinese Han and three other minority peoples of China: results from the WHO Cardiovascular Diseases and Alimentary Comparison (CARDIAC) Study. Hypertens Res. (2001) 24:315–22. doi: 10.1291/hypres.24.315

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Wang, Z, Ma, W, Rong, Y, and Liu, L. The association between apolipoprotein E gene polymorphism and mild cognitive impairment among different ethnic minority groups in China. Int J Alzheimer’s Dis. (2014) 2014:150628. doi: 10.1155/2014/150628

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Gu, Y, Guo, J, and Moshfegh, AJ. Race/ethnicity and gender modify the association between diet and cognition in U.S. older adults: National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2011–2014. Alzheimers Dement. (2021) 7:e12128. doi: 10.1002/trc2.12128

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Estrella, ML, Durazo-Arvizu, RA, Mattei, J, Mossavar-Rahmani, Y, Perreira, KM, Siega-Riz, AM, et al. Alternate healthy eating index is positively associated with cognitive function among middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos in the HCHS/SOL. J Nutr. (2020) 150:1478–87. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxaa023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Zhang, Y, Chen, J, Qiu, J, Li, Y, Wang, J, and Jiao, J. Intakes of fish and polyunsaturated fatty acids and mild-to-severe cognitive impairment risks: a dose-response meta-analysis of 21 cohort studies. Am J Clin Nutr. (2016) 103:330–40. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.124081

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Yeh, T, Yuan, C, Ascherio, A, Rosner, BA, Blacker, D, and Willett, WC. Long-term dietary protein intake and subjective cognitive decline in us men and women. Am J Clin Nutr. (2022) 115:199–210. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqab236

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Saleh, RNM, and Minihane, AM. Fish, n-3 fatty acids, cognition and dementia risk: not just a fishy tale. Proc Nutr Soc. (2022) 81:27–40. doi: 10.1017/S0029665121003700

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Chang, C, Wu, C, Wang, J, Li, J, Wang, Y, Lin, S, et al. DHA attenuated Japanese encephalitis virus infection-induced neuroinflammation and neuronal cell death in cultured rat neuron/glia. Brain Behav Immun. (2021) 93:194–205. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2021.01.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Cole, GM, Lim, GP, Yang, F, Teter, B, Begum, A, Ma, Q, et al. Prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: omega-3 fatty acid and phenolic anti-oxidant interventions. Neurobiol Aging. (2005) 26:133–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dietary guidance, CHEI, cognitive performance, older adults, CHNS

Citation: Fan Y, Yu Y, Tan J, Ma Y and Men K (2025) Dietary adherence and cognitive performance in older adults: insights from a nationwide survey in China. Front. Nutr. 12:1605016. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2025.1605016

Received: 02 April 2025; Accepted: 14 July 2025;
Published: 30 July 2025.

Edited by:

Fei Xu, Nanjing Municipal Center for Disease Control and Prevention, China

Reviewed by:

Amardeep Singh Virdi, Amity University Punjab, India
Luowei Chen, The Second Affiliated Hospital of Zhejiang University, School of Medicine, China
Jiatong Shan, National University of Singapore, Singapore

Copyright © 2025 Fan, Yu, Tan, Ma and Men. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yan Yu, eWFueXVwYXBlckB5ZWFoLm5ldA==; Ke Men, bWVua2VAeGl5aS5lZHUuY24=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.