Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 05 October 2023
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Adolescence Mental Health in School and University View all 31 articles

Can adolescents' subjective wellbeing facilitate their pro-environmental consumption behaviors? Empirical study based on 15-year-old students

  • 1The Co-innovation Center for Social Governance of Urban and Rural Communities in Hubei Province, Zhongnan University of Economics and Law, Wuhan, Hubei, China
  • 2School of Business Administration, Zhongnan University of Economics and Law, Wuhan, Hubei, China
  • 3School of Accounting, Zhongnan University of Economics and Law, Wuhan, Hubei, China

Purpose: To address the challenge of declining pro-environmental behaviors in adolescence, this paper uses the theoretical foundations for subjective wellbeing to verify the influence of three latent dimensions of subjective wellbeing (life satisfaction, positive emotions, and negative emotions) on adolescents' pro-environmental consumption behaviors (PECBs). Furthermore, we explore the moderating effects of nations and regions in the relationship between subjective wellbeing and PECBs in adolescents.

Method: Based on the international data from the Programme for International Student Assessment 2018 (PISA 2018), we construct a unique dataset that includes 57,182 samples related to the subjective wellbeing and PECBs of 15-year-old students from eight countries/economies. Specially, we employ an ordered probit model to test our hypotheses.

Findings: Both adolescents' life satisfaction and positive emotions can significantly improve their PECBs, while there is a significant negative association between negative emotions and PECBs. At the nation's level, adolescents' life satisfaction and negative emotions in developed countries/economies significantly impact PECBs. In contrast, the positive emotions of adolescents in developing countries/economies have more substantial effects on PECBs. At the regional level, the impact of all three dimensions of adolescents' subjective wellbeing on PECBs is more significant in rural than urban areas.

Originality/value: This paper provides a new perspective for understanding adolescents' PECBs from the insights of subjective wellbeing. Previous studies have examined the effects of life satisfaction or happiness on PECBs in adults. This paper examines the impact of subjective wellbeing on adolescents' PECBs from life satisfaction, positive emotions, and negative emotions, which suggests that promoting adolescents' subjective wellbeing can be an effective strategy for encouraging PECBs. From a comparative research perspective, we further analyze the differences between the nations at different levels of development, the rural and urban areas, providing a valuable reference for policymakers and practitioners in promoting pro-environmental behaviors among adolescents.

1. Introduction

Adolescents and youth are the largest generations in history, at over 1.8 billion (1). As the un claimed, “it is impossible to achieve the Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs) by 2030 without the active participation of the largest generation of changemakers” (2). The choices adolescents make today—or the lack of them—will have a profound impact on the planet and on our survival as a species (3). However, numerous studies have shown that adolescents' interest in environmental protection presents a decreasing trend in adolescence relative to childhood and adulthood (412). How to promote adolescents' pro-environmental behaviors, then, is of increasing concern to policy-makers and educators (1316).

Pro-environmental consumption behaviors (PECBs) are actions taken by individuals that prioritize environmental sustainability in their consumer choices (17, 18). A large body of prior literature on the influence of adolescent PECBs has emphasized the impact of external factors, such as family and peer relationships (1924), social norms (25, 26), nature or socio-spatial distance (5, 2730), and environmental knowledge (3133). Other scholars have investigated the influence of internal factors, for instance, adolescents' personality traits (34, 35) and environmental concerns (32, 3639). It is thus clear that previous literature has focused on objective predictors that influence adolescents' PECBs, while the potential influence of adolescents' subjective feelings on PECBs remains to be explored (18, 40).

Research in positive psychology suggests that those with higher levels of subjective wellbeing are more willing to give more money or time to engage in socially beneficial behavior, while those with lower levels of subjective wellbeing are often under urgent pressure to make the modest lives and are overwhelmed by the need to defend the public interests (4143). But is this the case in the field of PECBs?

Some scholars suggest that happier people are more willing to pay more or change their consumption behaviors to protect the environment (17, 18, 40, 42, 4448). However, the result has been only empirically analyzed in adults. Adolescence is a critical stage of human development between childhood and adulthood (49). Numerous studies have revealed a declining trend in interest in environmental issues among adolescent groups (8, 9, 11, 12, 25, 50), a phenomenon known as“time out”(12) or “adolescent dip” (11). Can, then, adolescents' subjective wellbeing promote their PECBs as well as adults?

In response to a growing call for research, several scholars have explored whether adolescents' increased happiness or life satisfaction would fare better in PECBs (51). Nonetheless, two gaps in the existing literature still need to be addressed.

On the one hand, although a growing body of literature has focused on the relationship between subjective wellbeing and PECBs, subjective wellbeing is regarded as a one-dimensional structure in the existing literature (18). Subjective wellbeing, wellbeing, happiness, life satisfaction, and positive emotions are often used interchangeably (40, 44, 45). In reality, there is a large consensus that life satisfaction and emotions are two fundamental building blocks of subjective wellbeing (41, 52, 53). In this vein, subjective wellbeing includes both an affective component (positive and negative emotions) and a cognitive component (one's judgment of one's overall life satisfaction or satisfaction with specific domains of one's life) (14, 43, 52). That is, subjective wellbeing contains three related but independent components: life satisfaction, positive emotions, and negative emotions (43, 5456). Previous research has focused on the effects of life satisfaction or positive emotions on PECBs, neglecting the impact of negative emotions. Moreover, extant research suggests negative emotions or unhappiness can also predict pro-social behaviors (43). Hence, this paper will examine whether subjective wellbeing is associated with adolescents' PECBs along three dimensions: life satisfaction, positive emotions, and negative emotions to address the first research gap.

On the other hand, the existing research that has been conducted usually focuses on one country or region, and there is a lack of international and cross-regional comparative studies. Moreover, extensive research has focused on developed countries, whereas research on those in developing countries has yet to be explored, especially the comparative studies during different development levels of nations. In addition, comparative studies of PECBs in adolescents in different areas, such as urban and rural areas, remain a puzzle.

To shed light on these puzzles, using a sample of 57,182 adolescents across eight countries/economies, we explore whether adolescents' PECBs are associated with all of the three components of subjective wellbeing by the ordered probit model. Furthermore, we explore the differences between developing and developed countries/economies, and between rural and urban areas.

This paper is structured as follows. Section 2 presents the literature overview of subjective wellbeing and PECBs and proposes the hypotheses. In Sections 3, we describe the data sources, variables, and empirical model. Section 4 reports the general results and the mechanism analysis of the moderating effects. In Section 5, we further discuss the findings. Finally, conclusions are drawn in Section 6.

2. Literature review

2.1. Adolescents' PECBs

Adolescents' lifetimes, in particular, will face the environmental consequences of actions taken or not taken today. Their active engagement in understanding the state of our environment (and the importance of protecting it) is critical to achieving sustainable development globally (3). PECBs are the human behaviors that mitigate various environmental issues, including climate change, environmental pollution, and the loss of biodiversity (5759). Precisely, PECBs comprise the committing of acts that benefit the ecological environment (e.g., recycling, use of non-toxic substances, changing purchasing behaviors) and the omission of actions that harm it (e.g., avoiding air travel, reducing waste production, minimizing energy consumption) (17, 60, 61).

For adolescents, United Nations Environment Programme (UNEP) suggests they can influence the market by buying or not buying (3). PECBs and public environmental protection activities are the main ways adolescents protect the environment. PECBs are more related to the private lifestyle of adolescents. Nevertheless, adolescents may not have sufficient exposure or knowledge of issues related to environmental degradation, and therefore may not see the relevance of these issues to their daily lives (27, 62). More specifically, as students, adolescents are surrounded by personal and academic pressures such as grades, social life, and extracurricular activities, which makes it difficult for them to devote more money or time to environmental issues. The decline in interest in environmental issues among adolescents is a tremendous challenge for the earth's future that may require a more effective approach to address (3, 63).

2.2. Life satisfaction and PECBs in adolescents

Life satisfaction is a subjective measure of one's overall sense of fulfillment and contentment with their life (41, 45, 54). It is people's evaluation of their life as a whole, including their past, present, and future (64, 65). Previous studies have shown that life satisfaction significantly impacts PECBs (43, 45). Wang and Kang (45) argued that people with higher life satisfaction performed better in PECBs. Ones with high life satisfaction often have a strong sense of pro-society values, including a concern for social and environmental issues (43). This value system can lead them to choose environmentally friendly products and services, even if they cost more or are less convenient (40, 45).

Moreover, individuals with high life satisfaction may be more likely to be influenced by social norms that promote PECBs. Ouyang et al. (44) suggested that people might feel responsible for their communities and want to be seen as environmentally responsible and socially conscious. In this vein, ones may support companies that prioritize sustainable practices and ethical sourcing even if they may pay more (40). In contrast, those less satisfied with their lives are likely to be preoccupied with improving their situation, which can overshadow concerns about social issues (18). Therefore, we propose the following hypothesis:

H1a. Adolescents' life satisfaction is significantly and positively correlated with their PECBs.

2.3. Positive emotions and PECBs in adolescents

Positive emotions are subjective experiences that are typically pleasant and enjoyable. These emotions include feelings such as happiness, joy, contentment, gratitude, and hope (55, 56). Research has shown that cultivating positive emotions can benefit individuals and society; for example, positive emotions can motivate people to engage in pro-environmental behaviors that protect the natural environment and preserve its resources (40, 44, 66, 67).

Notably, the influence of positive emotions on PECBs has been identified in the literature. Yuan (68), Briones et al. (69) suggested that positive emotions were associated with greater self-control and regulation, which could motivate people to act in ways that align with their values and beliefs. This could also translate into PECBs, such as recycling or reducing energy consumption. In the opinions of Meinhold and Malkus (32), Kaida and Kaida (70), people who experience positive emotions may be more likely to conform to social norms related to sustainability, especially if those norms are reinforced by positive feedback from others. Therefore, they may be more likely to consider the environmental impact of their actions and choose PECBs. Besides, people with more positive emotions usually have a stronger sense of altruism and reciprocal cooperation (44). This can lead to increased concern for the environment and a desire to engage in behaviors that benefit others, even if it requires personal sacrifice or inconvenience (40). Therefore, we propose the following hypothesis:

H1b. Adolescents' positive emotions are significantly and positively correlated with their PECBs.

2.4. Negative emotions and PECBs in adolescents

Negative emotions are subjective feelings that can cause one to be miserable and sad, such as hate, afraid, anger, jealousy, and sadness (56, 67). Negative emotions may dampen people's enthusiasm for life and reduce their confidence and self-esteem (71). In this context, negative emotions may stop one from thinking and behaving rationally and seeing situations from their proper perspective. Adolescence is a period of dramatic development. The frontal lobes have not entirely developed, which causes immature self-emotional management skills (72, 73). Adolescents in this period are more concerned with their inner feelings. When immersed in negative emotions, it is a big challenge for them to pay attention to social and public issues and take social responsibility. Erikson (74) referred to these irresponsible behaviors of adolescents as “adolescence moratoriums.”

Although research has suggested that negative emotions, through fear, anger, guilt, shame, etc., help to evoke environmentally responsible behaviors (67). In fact, using negative emotions to drive behavior change limits our attention to the short term. As Carter (67), Sheldon and Kasser (75) argued that negative emotions might lead to a focus on immediate needs and desires rather than long-term consequences. For example, someone feeling stressed or anxious may prioritize convenience over sustainability, choosing disposable products instead of reusable ones to save time and effort (76). Moreover, the negative emotions that come with psychological threats can motivate one to give more attention to external goals, which may cause lower PECBs (75, 76). Specifically, when people are threatened regarding survival, finances, or relationships, they are more inclined toward extrinsic goals such as financial success, popularity, and image than intrinsic goals such as personal growth, social norms, and community contributions (7578). Thus, we propose the following hypothesis:

H1c. Adolescents' negative emotions are significantly and negatively correlated with their PECBs.

2.5. Moderating effects: developing or developed countries/economies

Nations worldwide are usually divided into developed and developing countries/economies based on their economic development, wealth, and wellbeing. The level of development of a country or economy is linked to the long and healthy life, access to knowledge, and standard of living of its people, which are also significant predictors of PECBs (40, 79).

In developed countries/economies with high levels of material wealth and availability of resources, people might be more selective about the products they purchase (46, 47, 80). More specifically, individuals in developed countries/economies tend to have a higher living standard, enabling them to make income sacrifices to change their purchase behaviors for environmental sustainability (40, 42). Additionally, adolescents in developed countries have greater access to education, information, and media resources, which can increase their awareness and concern about environmental issues.

Conversely, in developing countries/economies where poverty, inequality, and environmental degradation are pressing issues, individuals' subjective wellbeing may be more tied to stable access to basic needs of survival (46, 80). They may not be more selective in their consumption behaviors, and their financial constraints may limit their choices (41). Furthermore, adolescents in developing countries might lack exposure to such information, so their awareness and interest in environmental problems may be lower. Therefore, the impact of adolescents' subjective wellbeing on their PECBs should differ between developing and developed countries/economies due to different economic and social development levels. Based on the above analysis, we propose the following hypotheses:

H2a. Compared to the group of developing countries/economies, the effect of adolescents' life satisfaction on PECBs is more substantial in developed countries/economies.

H2b. Compared to the group of developing countries/economies, the effect of adolescents' positive emotions on PECBs is more substantial in developed countries/economies.

H2c. Compared to the group of developing countries/economies, the effect of adolescents' negative emotions on PECBs is more substantial in developed countries/economies.

2.6. Moderating effects: the rural or urban areas

Just as there are significant differences in levels of development between countries, there are essential differences between rural and urban areas in terms of population density, economic activity, infrastructure, lifestyle, education, health care, and the environment (81, 82).

In rural areas, communities have stronger social connections and a sense of community compared to urban areas. Ouyang et al. (44) argued that residents in rural areas lived in a society of acquaintances, and participation in environmental protection helped to strengthen their social capital and network of relationships. Besides, rural adolescents may have a closer relationship with their local environment and natural surroundings than urban adolescents (30). Specifically, rural adolescents may be more involved in farming, gardening, or outdoor activities. In contrast, the barriers to opportunities for adolescents in urban areas to connect with nature while enjoying high industrialization and modernization levels are increasing. Solano-Pinto et al. (51) have empirically tested whether there is a reciprocal influence between children's wellbeing and connectivity with nature which may promote their PEBs. Thus, the effect of adolescents' subjective wellbeing on PECBs should be more pronounced among adolescents in rural areas than in urban areas. Based on the above analysis, we propose the following hypotheses:

H3a. Compared to the group in the urban area, the effect of adolescents' life satisfaction on PECBs is more substantial in rural regions.

H3b. Compared to the group in the urban area, the effect of adolescents' positive emotions on PECBs is more substantial in rural regions.

H3c. Compared to the group in the urban area, the effect of adolescents' negative emotions on PECBs is more substantial in rural regions.

3. Methodology

3.1. Data

The data for this paper comes from two different sources. One is the individual dimension data, 2018 Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA 2018), from Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD). The other is the national dimension data, the human development index (HDI), from the United Nations Development Programme (UNDP).

The questionnaire from PISA 2018 is a comprehensive and rigorous international standardized assessment for adolescents, with both the multi-item measurements of subjective wellbeing and PECBs (14). PISA 2018, the triennial assessment launched in 1997 by OECD, is the latest wave of surveys. PISA 2018 assesses the extent to which 15-year-old students have acquired knowledge and skills to meet real-life challenges near the end of their compulsory education, conducted in eighty countries/economies worldwide. Unfortunately, the relevant questions for subjective wellbeing and PECBs are surveyed in only eight countries/economies in PISA 2018. Excluding invalid responses, 57,182 samples constitute the dataset for this study.

3.2. Variables

3.2.1. Dependent variable: PECBs

In the PISA 2018 student questionnaire, three question items are related to PECBs (see Table 3). Respondents are asked whether they have participated in PECBs in the questionnaire. We assign a value to each question answer. “No” is given a matter of “0”; “yes” is given a value of “1”.

3.2.2. Independent variable: subjective wellbeing

Subjective wellbeing is the core set of explanatory variables with three dimensions (see Table 3). Life satisfaction is measured by the four-point Likert scale of 10 question items, ranging from 1 (Not at all satisfied) to 4 (Totally satisfied). Positive emotions are measured by the four-point Likert scale of five question items, ranging from 1 (Never) to 4 (Always). Negative emotions are measured by the four-point Likert scale of four question items, ranging from 1 (Never) to 4 (Always). All three scales are tested reliability and validity. Cronbach's α for these scales are all >0.8, indicating that the reliability of scales is acceptable (see Table 1). The KMO value and Bartlett's test of sphericity show satisfactory results (Life satisfaction: KMO = 0.917; χ2 = 246,570.980, p < 0.001; positive emotions: KMO = 0.853; χ2 = 113,166.304, p < 0.001; negative emotions: KMO = 0.750; χ2 = 156,573.700, p < 0.001), which indicate the suitability of factor analysis. Factor loadings (FL) of each item above 0.5 indicate good construct validity (see Table 1). Both the average variance extracted (AVE) of positive emotions and negative emotions are above 0.50, while the AVE of life satisfaction is 0.482 (see Table 1). However, the composition reliability (CR) values of the three constructs are all above 0.60 (see Table 1), demonstrating the convergent validity is acceptable (83).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Reliability and validity of the measurement scale of subjective wellbeing.

Additionally, we test the discriminant validity. Table 2 shows that the square root of the AVE for each construct is all greater than its correlation with the other variables, demonstrating the discriminant validity of the measures (18).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The discriminant validity of the measures.

3.2.3. Moderating variables: the nation and region

In this paper, the samples are drawn from eight countries/economies: Bulgaria, Hong Kong, Ireland, Mexico, Panama, Serbia, Spain, and United Arab Emirates (the UAE). In order to test the moderating effect of the level of development of countries or economies, we classify them as developing countries/economies and developed countries/economies. The HDI, produced by UNDP since 1990, consists of three components: a long and healthy life, access to education, and a decent standard of living (84). The HDI covers both economic and social indicators, replacing the single GDP per capita measurement system and providing a more comprehensive and scientific picture of the nation's level of development. The HDI ranges from 0 to 1, with higher values being better. The four subgroups are as follows: less than 0.550 for low development; between 0.550 and 0.699 for medium development; 0.700 and 0.799 for high development; and ≥0.800 for extremely high development.

To explore the impact of the level of national development on the PECBs of 15-year-olds, we set the data collection period from birth to the year the adolescents were surveyed, i.e., 2004 to 2018. We take the average value of the HDI for each country or economy over a total of 15 years, from 2004 to 2018. The results show that Hong Kong, Ireland, Spain, and the UAE have a mean HDI above 0.800 for these 15 years, while Bulgaria, Mexico, Panama, and Serbia have HDIs between 0.700 and 0.799. According to existing studies (84), HDI above 0.800 is considered the developed countries or economies, while the rest are developing countries or economies. Therefore, in this paper, the group with a high HDI is regarded as a developing country/economy and set a value of 0. The group with a very high HDI is considered a developed country/economy and assigned a value of 1.

As for the regions, in PISA 2018, the communities in which students' schools are located are divided into five categories by population (see Table 3). We further divide them into the rural (Assign a value of 0) and urban areas (Assign a value of 1).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Descriptive statistics.

3.2.4. Control variables

The individual demographic characteristics, such as gender and grade, are controlled. Moreover, according to Ouyang et al. (44), we further hold for the effect of their environmental knowledge. Table 3 shows the above variables' descriptive statistics in detail.

3.3. Empirical model

Given that the dependent variable PECBs is an ordered variable, according to Ouyang et al. (44), Wang and Kang (45), we use an ordered probit model to test the effect of subjective wellbeing on PECBs. The model is as follows:

PECBsi=β0+β1Subjectivewellbeingi+β2Xi+εi     (1)

where i represents the individual. PECBsi, the dependent variable, stands for the PECBs of individual i. Subjectivewellbeingi, the set of the core explanatory variables, represents the subjective wellbeing of individual i. Xi is the set of control variables. Lastly, εi is an error term.

4. Results

4.1. General results

The regression results of the effect of the three dimensions of subjective wellbeing on PECBs in adolescents are reported in Table 4. The results show that adolescents' life satisfaction and positive emotions are significantly positively associated with their PECBs, while negative emotions are significantly negatively associated with their PECBs. Thus, H1a, H1b, and H1c are all verified.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Benchmark regression.

Furthermore, at the individual level, the grade level, gender, and environmental knowledge of adolescents all significantly impact PECBs. The research sample for this paper is 15-year-old school students of the same age and in grades ranging from 7th to 12th. The results show that the higher the grade level of the adolescents, the better their performance on PECBs. At the same time, there is a significant positive correlation between adolescents' environmental knowledge and PECBs. The findings indicate that the promotion of environmental knowledge and academic knowledge helps adolescents to develop sustainable consumption behaviors that are conducive to environmental protection, which is consistent with Carmi et al. (85), Latif et al. (86), Liu et al. (87), Meyer (88). Moreover, some studies have shown that females show stronger environmental attitudes and behaviors than males (8991). However, our results show that girls perform weaker than boys in PECBs, which is consistent with the findings of Vicente-Molina et al. (92).

4.2. Moderating effects

We further examine whether the impact of adolescents' subjective wellbeing on PECBs differs between developing and developed countries/economies. At the same time, we test the significance of the differences between the three groups. The results show that the differences between all three groups are significant at the 0.001% level (see Empirical p-value in Table 5). The results show that the positive impact of increased life satisfaction on adolescents' PECBs is more substantial in developed countries/economies compared (β = 0.173 , p < 0.001) to developing countries/economies (β = 0.047, p < 0.01). Thus, H2a is verified.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Moderating effect of the nation.

The effect of negative emotions on PECBs also confirms this result. The significant inhibitory effect of negative emotions on PECBs is more strongly manifested in developed countries/economies (β = −0.092, p < 0.001). Nevertheless, the impact of negative sentiment on PECBs is not significant in developing countries/economies. Thus, H2c is verified. Notably, on the dimension of positive emotions, adolescents' subjective wellbeing in developing countries/economies (β = 0.120, p < 0.001) has more substantial positive impacts on their PECBs than developed countries/economies (β = 0.069, p < 0.001). Thus, H2b is not verified.

On the other hand, we explore the moderating effect of region on adolescents' subjective wellbeing on their PECBs. Table 6 shows that the significant effects of subjective wellbeing on PECBs are all stronger for adolescents in rural areas than in urban areas. Moreover, we test the significance of the differences between the three groups. The results show that the differences between all three groups are significant at the 0.001% level (see Empirical p-value in Table 6). Thus, H3a, H3b, and H3c are all verified.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Moderating effect of region.

5. Discussion

The results of this paper show that life satisfaction and positive emotions are significantly associated with greater PECBs. In contrast, negative emotions are associated considerably with lower PECBs in adolescents, even after controlling for environmental knowledge and sociodemographic variables. There are consistently significant correlations between the three dimensions of subjective wellbeing and PECBs in developed countries/economies. However, in developing countries/economies, life satisfaction and positive emotions significantly affect PECBs, while negative emotions do not correlate substantially with PECBs. Interestingly, the significant impact of adolescents' subjective wellbeing on PECBs is more substantial in developing countries/economies than in developed countries/economies. More specifically, in developed countries/economies, adolescents' life satisfaction and negative emotions significantly impacted their PECBs more than in developing countries/economies. However, the positive sentiment of adolescents in developing countries/economies has a more significant impact on PECBs than in developed countries/economies.

Furthermore, life satisfaction, positive emotions, and negative emotions are all significantly associated with PECBs in rural and urban areas. Notably, all these significant correlations are more substantial in rural than urban areas. The results indicate that adolescents in rural areas may have more opportunities to participate in community-based activities and get close to nature, which promotes sustainability and environmental protection. This can give them a sense of purpose and fulfillment, which may further reinforce their commitment to pro-environmental behaviors.

5.1. Theoretical implications

These findings contribute to the existing literature in several ways. Firstly, our study offers new ideas for approaching the challenge of declining environmental behavior in adolescents (11, 12). Our results confirm that subjective wellbeing, as an excellent predictor of PECBs, is not only in adults but also in adolescents, which builds on previous research examining the association between subjective wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviors in adults (18, 40, 44, 45). Secondly, our results also provide new evidence for developing the theory of subjective wellbeing. Building on the relationship between the three dimensions of subjective wellbeing (life satisfaction, positive emotions, and negative emotions) on pro-social behavior in adults by Kushlev et al. (43), our results further confirm that all three components of subjective wellbeing are significantly correlated with PECBs in adolescents. Last but not least, this paper broadens positive psychology from a comparative perspective. The significant differences between developed and developing nations, and between rural and urban areas, provide essential and concrete references for policymakers to enhance PECBs for adolescents.

5.2. Practical implications

The finding that subjective wellbeing positively influences PECBs has important practical implications for promoting sustainable consumption and environmental protection. This paper contributes to understanding the psychological mechanisms underlying environmentally friendly behaviors. It suggests that promoting subjective wellbeing can be an effective strategy for encouraging PECBs in adolescents. Our findings also highlight the importance of fostering subjective wellbeing as a critical component of sustainable consumption policies and programs. By improving adolescents' subjective wellbeing, policymakers and practitioners can create an environment that fosters pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors, ultimately leading to more sustainable lifestyles and a healthier planet.

More specifically, in developed countries/economies, more attention should be paid to the positive emotions of adolescents. In comparison, in developing countries/economies, more attention should be paid to the life satisfaction of adolescents and the impact of negative emotions on PECBs. Additionally, focusing on PECBs for adolescents in urban areas is essential. Policymakers, schools, and families should create more opportunities for young people to connect with nature and participate in environmental activities.

5.3. Limitations and further research

The limitations should also be considered when interpreting the results of this paper. Firstly, our study uses cross-sectional data and could not provide any evidence of causality. There are few international research projects on adolescents' subjective wellbeing and pro-environmental behavior. We look forward to more long-term follow-up projects to examine the causal relationship and long-term effects between adolescents' subjective wellbeing and PECBs. The second limitation concerns the measurement of adolescent PECBs used in this paper are self-reported rather than actual pro-environmental behaviors. Moreover, all PECBs are measured on dichotomous yes or no scales. Future research would benefit from a more detailed classification and more accurate frequency measurements on PECBs. Last but not the least, the idiosyncratic attributes inherent to various nations, encompassing elements like political structures, societal contexts, and cultural legacies, exert a profound impact on the behaviors exhibited by adolescents. Consequently, a meticulous examination that takes into account the unique characteristics of each nation becomes of paramount importance, thereby highlighting a pivotal trajectory for forthcoming research undertakings. Last but not least, the distinctive attributes inherent to various countries/economies, encompassing elements like political structures, societal contexts, and cultural legacies, profoundly impact adolescents' behaviors. Consequently, a meticulous examination that considers the unique characteristics of each country/economy becomes paramount, thereby highlighting a pivotal trajectory for forthcoming research undertakings.

6. Conclusions

In conclusion, our study confirms that all three components of subjective wellbeing, life satisfaction, positive emotions, and negative emotions, are significantly associated with adolescents' PECBs. Moreover, the effect of adolescent subjective wellbeing on PECBs differs substantially between developed and developing countries/economies, and between rural and urban areas. The findings of this paper provide new insight into the challenge of declining pro-environmental behaviors in adolescence.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://www.oecd.org/pisa/data/2018database/.

Author contributions

MZ: conceptualization, formal analysis, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing, visualization, funding acquisition, and project administration. WZ: formal analysis, data curation, and visualization. YS: conceptualization, formal analysis, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing, funding acquisition, and supervision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2023.1184605/full#supplementary-material

References

1. UN. 10 Things You Didn't Know About the World's Population. Available online at: https://www.un.org/youthenvoy/2015/04/10-things-didnt-know-worlds-population/ (accessed September 26, 2023).

Google Scholar

2. UN. Youth SDG Dashboard. Available online at: https://www.un.org/youthenvoy/youth-sdg-dashboard/ (accessed September 26, 2023).

Google Scholar

3. UNEGEO-6 for Youth. (2021) Available online at: https://www.unep.org/resources/geo-6-youth (accessed September 26, 2023).

Google Scholar

4. Negev M, Saggy G, Garb Y, Salzherg A, Tal A. Evaluating the environmental literacy of israeli elementary and high school students. J Environm Educ. (2008) 39:3–20. doi: 10.3200/JOEE.39.2.3-20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Uitto A, Boeve-de Pauw J, Saloranta S. Participatory school experiences as facilitators for adolescents' ecological behavior. J Environm Psychol. (2015) 43:55–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Uitto A, Juuti A, Lavonen J, Byman R, Meisalo V. Secondary school students' interests, attitudes and values concerning school science related to environmental issues in Finland. Environm Educ Res. (2011) 17:167–86. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2010.522703

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Uitto A, Saloranta S. The relationship between secondary school students' environmental and human values, attitudes, interests and motivations. Procedia Soc Behav Sci. (2010) 9:1866–72. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.12.415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Wray-Lake L, Metzger A, Syvertsen AK. Testing multidimensional models of youth civic engagement: Model comparisons, measurement invariance, age differences. Appl Dev Sci. (2017) 21:266–84. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2016.1205495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Wray-Lake L, Syvertsen AK, Flanagan CA. Developmental change in social responsibility during adolescence: an ecological perspective. Dev Psychol. (2016) 52:130. doi: 10.1037/dev0000067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Otto S, Kaiser FG. Ecological behavior across the lifespan: why environmentalism increases as people grow older. J Environm Psychol. (2014) 40:331–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Olsson D, Gericke N. The adolescent dip in students' sustainability consciousness—Implications for education for sustainable development. J Environ Educ. (2016) 47:35–51. doi: 10.1080/00958964.2015.1075464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Kahn PH, Kellert SR. Children and Nature: Psychological, Sociocultural, Evolutionary Investigations. (2002) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

13. Education A.f.P. Health Position Paper. (2015).

Google Scholar

14. OECD. PISA 2018 Assessment and Analytical Framework. Paris: OECD Publishing. (2019).

Google Scholar

15. UNECE. UNECE Strategy for Education for Sustainable Development. UNECE: Vilnius. (2005).

Google Scholar

16. WHO. Health in 2015, From MDGs, Millennium Development Goals to SDGs, Sustainable Development Goals. (2015). Available onlline at: https://www.who.int/data/gho/publications/mdgs-sdgs (accessed September 26, 2023).

Google Scholar

17. Lange F, Dewitte S. Measuring pro-environmental behavior: review and recommendations. J Environ Educ. (2019) 63:92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.04.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Nguyen HV, Le M, Pham C, Cox S. Happiness and pro-environmental consumption behaviors. J Econ Dev. (2022). doi: 10.1108/JED-07-2021-0116. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Collado S, Staats H. Sancho normative influences on adolescents' self-reported pro-environmental behaviors: the role of parents and friends. Environ Behav. (2017) 51:288–314. doi: 10.1177/0013916517744591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Jia F, Yu H. Action, communication, and engagement: How parents “ACE” Children's pro-environmental behaviors. J Environm Psychol. (2021) 74:101575. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Scopelliti M, Barni D, Rinallo E. My parents taught&hellip;green was my growth! the role of intergenerational transmission of ecological values in young adults&rsquo; pro-environmental behaviors and their psychosocial mechanisms. Int J Environm Res Public Health. (2022) 19:31670. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19031670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Grønhøj A, Thøgersen J. Action speaks louder than words: The effect of personal attitudes and family norms on adolescents' pro-environmental behaviour. J Econ Psychol. (2012) 33:292–302. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2011.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Wallis H, Loy LS. What drives pro-environmental activism of young people? A survey study on the fridays for future movement. J Environm Psychol. (2021) 74:101581. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101581

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Grønhøj A, Thøgersen J. Why young people do things for the environment: The role of parenting for adolescents' motivation to engage in pro-environmental behaviour. J Environm Psychol. (2017) 54:11–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Krettenauer T, Lefebvre J. Beyond subjective and personal: endorsing pro-environmental norms as moral norms. J Environm Psychol. (2021) 76:101644. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101644

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Pearce H, et al. Motivating children to become green kids: The role of victim framing, moral emotions, and responsibility on children's pro-environmental behavioral intent. J Environm Commun. (2021) 15:969–85. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2021.1934056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Krettenauer T, Wong W, Jia F, Yao Y. Connectedness with nature and the decline of pro-environmental behavior in adolescence: a comparison of Canada and China. J Environm Commun. (2020) 71:101348. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Busse M, Menzel S. The role of perceived socio-spatial distance in adolescents' willingness to engage in pro-environmental behavior. J Environm Commun. (2014) 40:412–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Xia W, Li LMW. Multilevel evidence for the parent-adolescent dyadic effect of familiarity with climate change on pro-environmental behaviors in 14 societies: moderating effects of societal power distance and individualism. Environ Behav. (2022) 54:1097–132. doi: 10.1177/00139165221129550

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Barbaro N, Pickett SM. Mindfully green: Examining the effect of connectedness to nature on the relationship between mindfulness and engagement in pro-environmental behavior. Pers Individ Dif. (2016) 93:137–42. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.05.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Sarrasin O, Henry J, Masserey C, Henry J. The relationships between adolescents&rsquo; climate anxiety, efficacy beliefs, group dynamics, and pro-environmental behavioral intentions after a group-based environmental education intervention. Youth. (2022) 2:422–40. doi: 10.3390/youth2030031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Meinhold JL, Malkus AJ. Adolescent environmental behaviors: can knowledge, attitudes, and self-efficacy make a difference? Environ Behav. (2005) 37:511–32. doi: 10.1177/0013916504269665

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Correia E, Sousa S, Viseu C, Leite J. Using the theory of planned behavior to understand the students' pro-environmental behavior: a case-study in a Portuguese. HEI Int J Sustainab Higher Educ. (2022) 23:1070–89. doi: 10.1108/IJSHE-05-2021-0201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Poškus MS. What works for whom? investigating adolescents' pro-environmental behaviors. Sustainability. (2020) 12:187313. doi: 10.3390/su12187313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Žukauskiene R, Truskauskaite-Kunevičiene I, Kaniušonyte G. “My words matter”: the role of adolescents in changing pro-environmental habits in the family. Environm Behav. (2020) 53:1140–62. doi: 10.1177/0013916520953150

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Abraham J, Pane M, Chairiyani R. An investigation on cynicism and environmental self-efficacy as predictors of pro-environmental behavior. Psychology. (2015) 6:234–42. doi: 10.4236/psych.2015.63023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Davignon L.-M, Poulin F, Denault A-S. Organized activities in adolescence and pro-environmental behaviors in adulthood: the mediating role of pro-environmental attitudes. J Adolescence. (2022) 95:284–95. doi: 10.1002/jad.12113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Balunde A, Perlaviciute G, Truskauskaite-Kuneviciene I. Sustainability in youth: environmental considerations in adolescence and their relationship to pro-environmental behavior. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:582920. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.582920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Barszcz SJ, Oleszkowicz A, Bak O, Slowinska A. The role of types of motivation, life goals, and beliefs in pro-environmental behavior: the Self-Determination Theory perspective. Curr Psychol. (2022) 42:1–16. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-02995-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Sulemana I. Are happier people more willing to make income sacrifices to protect the environment? Soc Indic Res. (2016) 127:447–67. doi: 10.1007/s11205-015-0960-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Diener E, Oishi S, Tay L. Advances in subjective well-being research. Nat Hum Behav. (2018) 2:253–60. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Duroy QMH. Testing the affluence hypothesis: a cross-cultural analysis of the determinants of environmental action. Soc Sci J. (2008) 45:419–39. doi: 10.1016/j.soscij.2008.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Kushlev K, Radosic N, Diener E. Subjective wellbeing and prosociality around the globe: happy people give more of their time and money to others. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. (2021) 13:849–61. doi: 10.1177/19485506211043379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Ouyang X, Qi W, Song D, Zhou J. Does subjective well-being promote pro-environmental behaviors? Evidence from rural residents in China. Int J Environm Res Public Health. (2022) 19:5992. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19105992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Wang E, Kang N. Does life satisfaction matter for pro-environmental behavior? Empirical evidence from China. General Soc Sur Qual Quant. (2019) 53:449–69. doi: 10.1007/s11135-018-0763-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Gowdy J. Toward a new welfare economics for sustainability. Ecol Econ. (2005) 53:211–22. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2004.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Frey BS, Stutzer A. What can economists learn from happiness research? J Econ Lit. (2002) 40:402–35. doi: 10.1257/jel.40.2.402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Kushlev K, Drummond DM, Heintzelman S, Diener E. Do happy people care about society's problems? J Posit Psychol. (2020) 15:467–77. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2019.1639797

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. WHO. Available online at: https://www.who.int/health-topics/adolescent-health#tab=tab_1 (accessed September 26, 2023).

Google Scholar

50. Krettenauer T. Pro-environmental behavior and adolescent moral development. J Res Adolesc. (2017) 27:581–93. doi: 10.1111/jora.12300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Solano-Pinto N, Garrido D, Barrlo FG, Cezar R. Is knowledge of circular economy, pro-environmental behavior, satisfaction with life, and beliefs a predictor of connectedness to nature in rural children and adolescents? A pilot study. Sustainability. (2020) 12:239951. doi: 10.3390/su12239951

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Diener E, Oishi S, Lucas RE. National accounts of subjective well-being. Am Psychol. (2015) 70:234–42. doi: 10.1037/a0038899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Diener E. Subjective well-being. In: The Science of Well-being. (2009) p. 11–58.

Google Scholar

54. Diener E. Subjective well-being. Psychol Bull. (1984) 95:542–75. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Watson D, Clark LA. Tellegen development A, and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1988) 54:1063–70. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Diener E, Emmons RA. The independence of positive and negative affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1984) 47:1105–17. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Stern PC. Psychological dimensions of global environmental change. Annu Rev Psychol. (1992) 43:269–302. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.43.020192.001413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Swim JK, Clayton S, Howard GS. Human behavioral contributions to climate change: psychological and contextual drivers. Am Psychol. (2011) 66:251. doi: 10.1037/a0023472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Wynes S, Nicholas KA. The climate mitigation gap: education and government recommendations miss the most effective individual actions. Environm Res Lett. (2017) 12:074024. doi: 10.1088/1748-9326/aa7541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Kollmuss A, Agyeman J. Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environm Educ Res. (2002) 8:239–60. doi: 10.1080/13504620220145401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Steg L, Vlek C. Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: An integrative review and research agenda. J Environm Psychol. (2009) 29:309–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Molinario E, Lorenzi C, Bartoccioni F, Perucchini P, Bobeth S, Colleny A. From childhood nature experiences to adult pro-environmental behaviors: an explanatory model of sustainable food consumption. Environm Educ Res. (2020) 26:1137–63. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2020.1784851

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. UN. GoGreen Project: University Studies Environmental Psychology in Young People. Available oonline at: https://www.un.org/en/academic-impact/gogreen-project-university-studies-environmental-psychology-young-people (accessed September 26, 2023).

Google Scholar

64. Neugarten BL, Havighurst RJ, Tobin SS. The measurement of life satisfaction. J Gerontol. (1961) 16:134–43. doi: 10.1093/geronj/16.2.134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Pavot W, Diener E. The satisfaction with life scale and the emerging construct of life satisfaction. J Posit Psychol. (2008) 3:137–52. doi: 10.1080/17439760701756946

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Kaida N, Kaida K. Positive associations of optimism–pessimism orientation with pro-environmental behavior and subjective well-being: a longitudinal study on quality of life and everyday behavior. Qual Life Res. (2019) 28:3323–32. doi: 10.1007/s11136-019-02273-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Carter DM. Recognizing the role of positive emotions in fostering environmentally responsible behaviors. Ecopsychology. (2011) 3:65–9. doi: 10.1089/eco.2010.0071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Yuan L. The happier one is, the more creative one becomes: an investigation on inspirational positive emotions from both subjective well-being and satisfaction at work. Psychology. (2015) 06:201–9. doi: 10.4236/psych.2015.63019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Briones E, Tabernero C, Arenas A. Variables psicosociales relacionadas con el proceso de integración social de los estudiantes inmigrantes. J Cultura y Educación. (2005) 17:337–47. doi: 10.1174/113564005775133766

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Kaida N, Kaida K. Pro-environmental behavior correlates with present and future subjective well-being. Environm Dev Sustainab. (2016) 18:111–27. doi: 10.1007/s10668-015-9629-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Kopp CB. Regulation of distress and negative emotions: a developmental view. Dev Psychol. (1989) 25:343–54. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.343

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Romine CB, Reynolds CR. A model of the development of frontal lobe functioning: findings from a meta-analysis. Appl Neuropsychol. (2005) 12:190–201. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1204_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Fuster JM. Frontal lobe and cognitive development. J Neurocytol. (2002) 31:373–85. doi: 10.1023/A:1024190429920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Erikson EH. Identity and the Life Cycle. New York, NY: WW Norton & Company. (1994).

Google Scholar

75. Sheldon KM, Kasser T. Psychological threat and extrinsic goal striving. Motiv Emot. (2008) 32:37–45. doi: 10.1007/s11031-008-9081-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Sheldon KM, McGregor HA. Extrinsic value orientation and “the tragedy of the commons”. J Pers. (2000) 68:383–411. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Deci EL, Ryan RM. The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol Inq. (2000) 11:227–68. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Kim Y, Kasser T, Lee H. Self-concept aspirations, and weil-being in south korea and the United States. J Soc Psychol. (2003) 143:277–90. doi: 10.1080/00224540309598445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Inglehart R. Public support for environmental protection: objective problems and subjective values in 43 societies. Polit Sci Politi. (1995) 28:57–72. doi: 10.2307/420583

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Easterlin RA. Does economic growth improve the human lot? some empirical evidence. In:David PA, Reder MW, , editors. Nations and Households in Economic Growth1974. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, p. 89–125.

Google Scholar

81. Tacoli C. Rural-urban interactions: a guide to the literature. Environm Urbanizat. (1998) 10:147–66. doi: 10.1177/095624789801000105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Pateman T. Rural and urban areas: comparing lives using rural/urban classifications. Regional Trends. (2011) 43:11–86. doi: 10.1057/rt.2011.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Hair F, Babin BJ, Anderson R, Black W. Multivariate Data Analysis, 8th edn. Andover: Cengage Learning, EMEA. (2018).

Google Scholar

85. Carmi N, Arnon S, Orion N. Transforming environmental knowledge into behavior: the mediating role of environmental emotions. J Environ Educ. (2015) 46:183–201. doi: 10.1080/00958964.2015.1028517

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Latif SA, Omar M, Bidin Y, Awang Z. Role of environmental knowledge in creating pro-environmental residents. Procedia-Social Behavioral Sciences. (2013) 105:866–74. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.11.088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Liu P, Teng M, Han C. How does environmental knowledge translate into pro-environmental behaviors? The mediating role of environmental attitudes and behavioral intentions. J Sci Total Environm. (2020) 728:138126. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.138126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Meyer A. Does education increase pro-environmental behavior? Evidence from Europe. Ecol Econ. (2015) 116:108–21. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2015.04.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Ahmad N, Ullah Z, Arsad MZ, Kamran H, Scholz M, Han H. Relationship between corporate social responsibility at the micro-level and environmental performance: the mediating role of employee pro-environmental behavior and the moderating role of gender. Sustain Product Consumpt. (2021) 27:1138–48. doi: 10.1016/j.spc.2021.02.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Wut TM, Ng P, Ki H, Kan M, Chiu, Fong S. Does gender matter? Attitude towards waste charging policy and pro-environmental behaviours. Social Responsib J. (2021) 17:1100–15. doi: 10.1108/SRJ-03-2020-0102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Li Y, Wang B. Saechang is female a more pro-environmental gender? Evidence from China. Int J Environm Res Public Health. (2022) 19:138002. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19138002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Vicente-Molina MA, Fernández-Sainz A, Izagirre-Olaizola J. Does gender make a difference in pro-environmental behavior? The case of the Basque Country University students. J Cleaner Prod. (2018) 176:89–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2017.12.079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: subjective wellbeing, pro-environmental consumption behaviors, adolescent, life satisfaction, positive emotions, negative emotions

Citation: Zhang M, Zhang W and Shi Y (2023) Can adolescents' subjective wellbeing facilitate their pro-environmental consumption behaviors? Empirical study based on 15-year-old students. Front. Public Health 11:1184605. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1184605

Received: 12 March 2023; Accepted: 19 September 2023;
Published: 05 October 2023.

Edited by:

Iman Permana, Muhammadiyah University of Yogyakarta, Indonesia

Reviewed by:

Simon Grima, University of Malta, Malta
Samuel Honório, Polytechnic Institute of Castelo Branco, Portugal

Copyright © 2023 Zhang, Zhang and Shi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yong Shi, shiyong@zuel.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.