Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 06 February 2017
Sec. Cognition

Intelligence and Creativity in problem solving: The importance of test features in cognition research

  • Center for Cognitive Science, Cognitive and Developmental Psychology Unit, University of Kaiserslautern, Kaiserslautern, Germany

This paper discusses the importance of three features of psychometric tests for cognition research: construct definition, problem space, and knowledge domain. Definition of constructs, e.g., intelligence or creativity, forms the theoretical basis for test construction. Problem space, being well or ill-defined, is determined by the cognitive abilities considered to belong to the constructs, e.g., convergent thinking to intelligence, divergent thinking to creativity. Knowledge domain and the possibilities it offers cognition are reflected in test results. We argue that (a) comparing results of tests with different problem spaces is more informative when cognition operates in both tests on an identical knowledge domain, and (b) intertwining of abilities related to both constructs can only be expected in tests developed to instigate such a process. Test features should guarantee that abilities can contribute to self-generated and goal-directed processes bringing forth solutions that are both new and applicable. We propose and discuss a test example that was developed to address these issues.

Much of cognition research is based on psychometric tests, such as tests assessing the constructs of intelligence or creativity. We argue that three test features must be explicitly considered, however, in order to reliably infer from individual’s test performance the underlying cognitive processes. These three test features are: definition of the construct, problem space, and knowledge domain.

The definition of the construct a test is to measure is most important in test construction and application, because cognitive processes reflect the possibilities a task offers. For instance, a test constructed to assess intelligence will operationalize the definition of this construct, being, in short, finding the correct answer. Also, the definition of a construct becomes important when selecting tests for the confirmation of a specific hypothesis. One can only find confirmation for a hypothesis if the chosen task instigates the necessary cognitive operations. For instance, in trying to confirm the assumed intertwining of certain cognitive abilities (e.g., convergent thinking and divergent thinking), tasks should be applied that have shown to yield the necessary cognitive process.

The second test feature, problem space, determines the degrees of freedom cognition has to its disposal in solving a problem. For instance, cognition will go through a wider search path when problem constraints are less well defined and, consequently, data will differ accordingly.

The third test feature, knowledge domain, is important when comparing results from two different tests. When tests differ in problem space, it is not advisable they should differ in knowledge domain. For instance, when studying the differences in cognitive abilities between tests constructed to asses convergent thinking (mostly defined problem space) and divergent thinking (mostly ill-defined problem space), in general test practice, both tests also differ in knowledge domain. Hence, data will reflect cognition operating not only in different problem spaces, but also operating on different knowledge domains, which makes the interpretation of results ambiguous.

The proposed approach for test development and test application holds the promise of, firstly, studying cognitive abilities in different problem spaces while operating on an identical knowledge domain. Although cognitions’ operations have been studied extensively and superbly in both contexts separately, they have rarely been studied in test situations where one or the other test feature is controlled for. The proposed approach also presents a unique method for studying thinking processes in which cognitive abilities intertwine. On the basis of defined abilities, tasks can be developed that have a higher probability of yielding the hypothesized results.

The construct of intelligence is defined as the ability to produce the single best (or correct) answer to a clearly defined question, such as a proof to a theorem (Simon, 1973). It may also be seen as a domain-general ability (g-factor; Spearman, 1904; Cattell, 1967) that has much in common with meta cognitive functions, such as metacognitive knowledge, metacognitive monitoring, and metacognitive control (Saraç et al., 2014).

The construct of creativity, in contrast, is defined as the ability to innovate and move beyond what is already known (Wertheimer, 1945/1968; Ghiselin, 1952/1985; Vernon, 1970). In other words, it emphasizes the aspect of innovation. This involves the ability to consider things from an uncommon perspective, transcend the old order (Ghiselin, 1952/1985; Chi, 1997; Ward, 2007), and explore loosely associated ideas (Guilford, 1950; Mednick, 1962; Koestler, 1964; Gentner, 1983; Boden, 1990; Christensen, 2007). Creativity could also be defined as the ability to generate a solution to problems with ill-defined problem spaces (Wertheimer, 1945/1968; Getzels and Csikszentmihalyi, 1976). In this sense it involves the ability to identify problematic aspects of a given situation (Ghiselin, 1952/1985) and, in a wider sense, the ability to define completely new problems (Getzels, 1975, 1987).

Guilford (1956) introduced the constructs of convergent thinking and divergent thinking abilities. Both thinking abilities are important because they allow us insights in human problem solving. On the basis of their definitions convergent and divergent thinking help us to structurally study human cognitive operations in different situations and over different developmental stages. Convergent thinking is defined as the ability to apply conventional and logical search, recognition, and decision-making strategies to stored information in order to produce an already known answer (Cropley, 2006). Divergent thinking, by contrast, is defined as the ability to produce new approaches and original ideas by forming unexpected combinations from available information and by applying such abilities as semantic flexibility, and fluency of association, ideation, and transformation (Guilford, 1959, as cited in Cropley, 2006, p. 1). Divergent thinking brings forth answers that may never have existed before and are often novel, unusual, or surprising (Cropley, 2006).

Guilford (1967) introduced convergent and divergent thinking as part of a set of five operations that apply in his Structure of Intellect model (SOI model) on six products and four kinds of content, to produce 120 different factors of cognitive abilities. With the SOI model Guilford wanted to give the construct of intelligence a comprehensive model. He wanted the model to include all aspects of intelligence, many of which had been seriously neglected in traditional intelligence testing because of a persistent adherence to the belief in Spearman’s g (Guilford, 1967, p. vii). Hence, Guilford envisaged cognition to embrace, among other abilities, both convergent and divergent thinking abilities. After these new constructs were introduced and defined, tests for convergent and divergent thinking emerged. Despite the fact that Guilford reported significant loadings of tests for divergent production on tests constructed to measure convergent production (Guilford, 1967, p. 155), over the years, both modes of thinking were considered as separate identities where convergent thinking tests associated with intelligence and divergent thinking tests with creativity (Cropley, 2006; Shye and Yuhas, 2004). Even intelligence tests that assess aspects of intelligence that supposedly reflect creative abilities do not actually measure creativity (Kaufman, 2015).

The idea that both convergent and divergent thinking are important for solving problems, and that intelligence helps in the creative process, is not really new. In literature we find models of the creative process that define certain stages to convergent and divergent thinking; the stages of purposeful preparation at the start and those of critical verification at the end of the process, respectively (Wallas, 1926; Webb Young, 1939/2003). In this view, divergent thinking enables the generation of new ideas whereas the exploratory activities of convergent thinking enable the conversion of ideas into something new and appropriate (Cropley and Cropley, 2008).

We argue that studying the abilities of divergent and convergent thinking in isolation does not suffice to give us complete insight of all possible aspects of human problem solving, its constituent abilities and the structure of its processes. Processes that in a sequence of thoughts and actions lead to novel and adaptive productions (Lubart, 2001) are more demanding of cognition for understanding the situation at hand and planning a path to a possible solution, than abilities involved in less complex situations (Jaušovec, 1999). Processes that yield self-generated and goal-directed thought are the most complex cognitive processes that can be studied (Beaty et al., 2016). Creative cognition literature is moving toward the view that especially in those processes that yield original and appropriate solutions within a specific context, convergent and divergent abilities intertwine (Cropley, 2006; Ward, 2007; Gabora, 2010).

The approach of intertwining cognitive abilities is also developed within cognitive neuroscience by focusing on the intertwining of brain networks (Beaty et al., 2016). In this approach divergent thinking relates to the default brain network. This network operates in defocused or associative mode of thought yielding spontaneous and self-generated cognition (Beaty et al., 2015). Convergent thinking relates to the executive control network operating in focused or analytic modes of thought, yielding updating, shifting, and inhibition (Benedek et al., 2014). Defocused attention theory (Mendelssohn, 1976) states that less creative individuals operate with a more focused attention than do creative individuals. This theory argues that e.g., attending to two things at the same time, might result in one analogy, while attending to four things might yield six analogies (Martindale, 1999).

In the process of shifting back and forth along the spectrum between associative and analytic modes of thinking, the fruits of associative thought become ingredients for analytic thought processes, and vice versa (Gabora, 2010). In this process, mental imagery is involved as one sensory aspect of the human ability to gather and process information (Jung and Haier, 2013). Mental imagery is fed by scenes in the environment that provide crucial visual clues for creative problem solving and actuates the need for sketching (Verstijnen et al., 2001).

Creative problem solving processes often involve an interactive relationship between imagining, sketching, and evaluating the result of the sketch (van Leeuwen et al., 1999). This interactive process evolves within a type of imagery called “visual reasoning” where forms and shapes are manipulated in order to specify the configurations and properties of the design entities (Goldschmidt, 2013). The originality of inventions is predicted by the application of visualization, whereas their practicality is predicted by the vividness of imagery (Palmiero et al., 2015). Imaginative thought processes emerge from our conceptual knowledge of the world that is represented in our semantic memory system. In constrained divergent thinking, the neural correlates of this semantic memory system partially overlap with those of the creative cognition system (Abraham and Bubic, 2015).

Studies of convergent and divergent thinking abilities have yielded innumerable valuable insights on the cognitive and neurological aspects involved, e.g., reaction times, strategies, brain areas involved, mental representations, and short and long time memory components. Studies on the relationship between both constructs suggest that it is unlikely that individuals employ similar cognitive strategies when solving more convergent than more divergent thinking tasks (Jaušovec, 2000). However, to arrive at a quality formulation the creative process cannot do without the application of both, convergent and divergent thinking abilities (e.g., Kaufmann, 2003; Runco, 2003; Sternberg, 2005; Dietrich, 2007; Cropley and Cropley, 2008; Silvia et al., 2013; Jung, 2014).

When it is our aim to study the networks addressed by the intertwining of convergent and divergent thinking processes that are considered to operate when new, original, and yet appropriate solutions are generated, then traditional thinking tests like intelligence tests and creativity tests are not appropriate; they yield processes related to the definition of one or the other type of construct.

Creative Reasoning Task

According to the new insights gained in cognition research, we need tasks that are developed with the aim to instigate precisely the kind of thinking processes we are looking for. Tasks should also provide a method of scoring independently the contribution of convergent and divergent thinking. As one possible solution for such tasks we present the Creative Reasoning Task (CRT; Jaarsveld, 2007; Jaarsveld et al., 2010, 2012, 2013).

The CRT presents participants with an empty 3 × 3 matrix and asks them to fill it out, as original and complex as possible, by creating components and the relationships that connect them. The created matrix can, in principle, be solved by another person. The creation of components is entirely free, as is the generation of the relationships that connects them into a completed pattern. Created matrices are scored with two sub scores; Relations, which scores the logical complexity of a matrix and is, therefore, considered a measure for convergent thinking, and Components and Specifications, which scores the originality, fluency, and flexibility and, therefore, is considered an indication for divergent thinking (for a more detailed description of the score method, see Appendix 1 in Supplementary Material).

Psychometric studies with the CRT showed, firstly, that convergent and divergent thinking abilities apply within this task and can be assessed independently. The CRT sub score Relations correlated with the Standard Progressive Matrices test (SPM) and the CRT sub score Components and Specifications correlated with a standard creativity test (TCT–DP, Test of Creative Thinking–Drawing Production; Urban and Jellen, 1995; Jaarsveld et al., 2010, 2012, 2013). Studies further showed that, although a correlation was observed for the intelligence and creativity test scores, no correlation was observed between the CRT sub scores relating to intelligent and creative performances (Jaarsveld et al., 2012, 2013; for further details about the CRT’s objectivity, validity, and reliability, see Appendix 2 in Supplementary Material).

Reasoning in creative thinking can be defined as the involvement of executive/convergent abilities in the inhibition of ideas and the updating of information (Benedek et al., 2014). Jung (2014) describes a dichotomy for cognitive abilities with at one end the dedicated system that relies on explicit and conscious knowledge and at the other end the improvisational system that relies more upon implicit or unconscious knowledge systems. The link between explicit and implicit systems can actually be traced back to Kris’ psychoanalytic approach to creativity dating from the 1950s. The implicit system refers to Kris’ primary process of adaptive regression, where unmodulated thoughts intrude into consciousness; the explicit system refers to the secondary process, where the reworking and transformation of primary process material takes place through reality-oriented and ego-controlled thinking (Sternberg and Lubart, 1999). The interaction between explicit and implicit systems can be seen to form the basis of creative reasoning, i.e., the cognitive ability to solve problems in an effective and adaptive way. This interaction evolved as a cognitive mechanism when human survival depended on finding effective solutions to both common and novel problem situations (Gabora and Kaufman, 2010). Creative reasoning solves that minority of problems that are unforeseen and yet of high adaptability (Jung, 2014).

Hence, common tests are insufficient when it comes to solving problems that are unforeseen and yet of high adaptability, because they present problems that are either unforeseen and measure certain abilities contained in the construct of creativity or they address adaptability and measure certain abilities contained in the construct of intelligence. The CRT presents participants with a problem that they could not have foreseen; the form is blank and offers no stimuli. All tests, even creativity tests, present participants with some kind of stimuli. The CRT addresses adaptability; to invent from scratch a coherent structure that can be solved by another person, like creating a crossword puzzle. Problems, that are unforeseen and of high adaptability, are solved by the application of abilities from both constructs.

Neuroscience of Creative Cognition

Studies in neuroscience showed that cognition operating in ill-defined problem space not only applies divergent thinking but also benefits from additional convergent operations (Gabora, 2010; Jung, 2014). Understanding creative cognition may be advanced when we study the flow of information among brain areas (Jung et al., 2010).

In a cognitive neuroscience study with the CRT we focused on the cognitive process evolving within this task. Participants performed the CRT while EEG alpha activity was registered. EEG alpha synchronization in frontal areas is understood as an indication of top-down control (Cooper et al., 2003). When observed in frontal areas, for divergent and convergent thinking tasks, it may not reflect a brain state that is specific for creative cognition but could be attributed to the high processing demands typically involved in creative thinking (Benedek et al., 2011). Top-down control, relates to volitionally focusing attention to task demands (Buschman and Miller, 2007). That this control plays a role in tasks with an ill-defined problem space showed when electroencephalography (EEG) alpha synchronization was stronger for individuals engaged in creative ideation tasks compared to an intelligence related tasks (Fink et al., 2007, 2009; Fink and Benedek, 2014). This activation was also found for the CRT; task related alpha synchronization showed that convergent thinking was integrated in the divergent thinking processes. Analyzes of the stages in the CRT process showed that this alpha synchronization was especially visible at the start of the creative process at prefrontal and frontal sites when information processing was most demanding, i.e., due to multiplicity of ideas, and it was visible at the end of the process, due to narrowing down of alternatives (Jaarsveld et al., 2015).

A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study (Beaty et al., 2015) with a creativity task in which cognition had to meet specific constraints, showed the networks involved. The default mode network which drives toward abstraction and metaphorical thinking and the executive control network driving toward certainty (Jung, 2014). Control involves not only maintenance of patterns of activity that represent goals and the means to achieve those (Miller and Cohen, 2001), but also their voluntary suppression when no longer needed, as well as the flexible shift between different goals and mental sets (Abraham and Windmann, 2007). Attention can be focused volitionally by top-down signals derived from task demands and automatically by bottom-up signals from salient stimuli (Buschman and Miller, 2007). Intertwining between top-down and bottom-up attention processes in creative cognition ensures a broadening of attention in free associative thinking (Abraham and Windmann, 2007).

These studies support and enhance the findings of creative cognition research in showing that the generation of original and applicable ideas involves an intertwining between different abilities, networks, and attention processes.

Problem Space

A problem space is an abstract representation, in the mind of the problem solver, of the encountered problem and of the asked for solution (Simon and Newell, 1971; Simon, 1973; Hayes and Flowers, 1986; Kulkarni and Simon, 1988; Runco, 2007). The space that comes with a certain problem can, according to the constraints that are formulated for the solution, be labeled well-defined or ill-defined (Simon and Newell, 1971). Consequently, the original problems are labeled closed and open problems, respectively (Jaušovec, 2000).

A problem space contains all possible states that are accessible to the problem solver from the initial state, through iterative application of transformation rules, to the goal state (Newell and Simon, 1972; Anderson, 1983). The initial state presents the problem solver with a task description that defines which requirements a solution has to answer. The goal state represents the solution. The proposed solution is a product of the application of transformation rules (algorithms and heuristics) on a series of successive intermediate solutions. The proposed solution is also a product of the iterative evaluations of preceding solutions and decisions based upon these evaluations (Boden, 1990; Gabora, 2002; Jaarsveld and van Leeuwen, 2005; Goldschmidt, 2014). Whether all possible states need to be passed through depends on the problem space being well or ill-defined and this, in turn, depends on the character of the task descriptions.

When task descriptions clearly state which requirements a solution has to answer then the inferences made will show little idiosyncratic aspects and will adhere to the task constraints. As a result, fewer options for alternative paths are open to the problem solver and search for a solution evolves in a well-defined space. Vice versa, when task or problem descriptions are fuzzy and under specified, the problem solver’s inferences are more idiosyncratic; the resulting process will evolve within an ill-defined space and will contain more generative-evaluative cycles in which new goals are set, and the cycle is repeated (Dennett, 1978, as cited in Gabora, 2002, p. 126).

Tasks that evolve in defined problem space are, e.g., traditional intelligence tests (e.g., Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, WAIS; and SPM, Raven, 1938/1998). The above tests consist of different types of questions, each testing a different component of intelligence. They are used in test practice to assess reasoning abilities in diverse domains, such as, abstract, logical, spatial, verbal, numerical, and mathematical domains. These tests have clearly stated task descriptions and each item has one and only one correct solution that has to be generated from memory or chosen from a set of alternatives, like in multiple choice formats. Tests can be constructed to assess crystallized or fluid intelligence. Crystallized intelligence represents abilities acquired through learning, practice, and exposure to education, while fluid intelligence represents a more basic capacity that is valuable to reasoning and problem solving in contexts not necessarily related to school education (Carroll, 1982).

Tasks that evolve in ill-defined problem space are, e.g., standard creativity tests. These types of test ask for a multitude of ideas to be generated in association with a given item or situation (e.g., “think of as many titles for this story”). Therefore, they are also labeled as divergent thinking test. Although they assess originality, fluency, flexibility of responses, and elaboration, they are not constructed, however, to score appropriateness or applicability. Divergent thinking tests assess one limited aspect of what makes an individual creative. Creativity depends also on variables like affect and intuition; therefore, divergent thinking can only be considered an indication of an individual’s creative potential (Runco, 2008). More precisely, divergent thinking explains just under half of the variance in adult creative potential, which is more than three times that of the contribution of intelligence (Plucker, 1999, p. 103). Creative achievement, by contrast, is commonly assessed by means of self-reports such as biographical questionnaires in which participants indicate their achievement across various domains (e.g., literature, music, or theater).

Studies with the CRT showed that problem space differently affects processing of and comprehension of relationships between components. Problem space did not affect the ability to process complex information. This ability showed equal performance in well and ill-defined problem spaces (Jaarsveld et al., 2012, 2013). However, problem space did affect the comprehension of relationships, which showed in the different frequencies of relationships solved and created (Jaarsveld et al., 2010, 2012). Problem space also affected the neurological activity as displayed when individuals solve open or closed problems (Jaušovec, 2000).

Problem space further affected trends over grade levels of primary school children for relationships solved in well-defined and applied in ill-defined problem space. Only one of the 12 relationships defined in the CRT, namely Combination, showed an increase with grade for both types of problem spaces (Jaarsveld et al., 2013). In the same study, cognitive development in the CRT showed in the shifts of preference for a certain relationship. These shifts seem to correspond to Piaget’s developmental stages (Piaget et al., 1977; Siegler, 1998) which are in evidence in the CRT, but not in the SPM (Jaarsveld et al., 2013).

Design Problems

A sub category of problems with an ill-defined problem space are represented by design problems. In contrast to divergent thinking tasks that ask for the generation of a multitude of ideas, in design tasks interim ideas are nurtured and incrementally developed until they are appropriate for the task. Ideas are rarely discarded and replaced with new ideas (Goel and Pirolli, 1992). The CRT could be considered a design problem because it yields (a) one possible solution and (b) an iterative thinking process that involves the realization of a vague initial idea. In the CRT a created matrix, which is a closed problem, is created within an ill-defined problem space. Design problems can be found, e.g., in engineering, industrial design, advertising, software design, and architecture (Sakar and Chakrabarti, 2013), however, they can also be found in the arts, e.g., poetry, sculpting, and dance geography.

These complex problems are partly determined by unalterable needs, requirements and intentions but the major part of the design problem is undetermined (Dorst, 2004). This author points out that besides containing an original and a functional value, these types of problems contain an aesthetic value. He further states that the interpretation of the design problem and the creation and selection of possible suitable solutions can only be decided during the design process on the basis of proposals made by the designer.

In design problems the generation stage may be considered a divergent thinking process. However, not in the sense that it moves in multiple directions or generates multiple possibilities as in a divergent thinking tests, but in the sense that it unrolls by considering an initially vague idea from different perspectives until it comes into focus and requires further processing to become viable. These processes can be characterized by a set of invariant features (Goel and Pirolli, 1992), e.g., structuring. iteration, and coherence.

Structuring of the initial situation is required in design processes before solving can commence. The problem contains little structured and clear information about its initial state and about the requirements of its solution. Therefore, design problems allow or even require re-interpretation of transformation rules; for instance, rearranging the location of furniture in a room according to a set of desirable outcomes. Here one uncovers implicit requirements that introduce a set of new transformations and/or eliminate existing ones (Barsalou, 1992; Goel and Pirolli, 1992) or, when conflicting requirements arise, one creates alternatives and/or introduces new trade-offs between the conflicting constraints (Yamamoto et al., 2000; Dorst, 2011).

A second aspect of design processes is their iterative character. After structuring and planning a vague idea emerges, which is the result of the merging of memory items. A vague idea is a cognitive structure that, halfway the creative process is still ill defined and, therefore, can be said to exist in a state of potentiality (Gabora and Saab, 2011). Design processes unroll in an iterative way by the inspection and adjustment of the generated ideas (Goldschmidt, 2014). New meanings are created and realized while the creative mind imposes its own order and meaning on the sensory data and through creative production furthers its own understanding of the world (Arnheim, 1962/1974, as cited in Grube and Davis, 1988, pp. 263–264).

A third aspect of design processes is coherence. Coherence theories characterize coherence in, for instance, philosophical problems and psychological processes, in terms of maximal satisfaction of multiple constraints and compute coherence by using, a.o., connectionist algorithms (Thagard and Verbeurgt, 1998). Another measure of coherence is characterized as continuity in design processes. This measure was developed for a design task (Jaarsveld and van Leeuwen, 2005) and calculated by the occurrence of a given pair of objects in a sketch, expressed as a percentage of all the sketches of a series. In a series of sketches participants designed a logo for a new soft drink. Design series strong in coherence also received a high score for their final design, as assessed by professionals in various domains. Indicating that participants with a high score for the creative quality of their final sketch seemed better in assessing their design activity in relation to the continuity in the process and, thereby, seemed better in navigating the ill-defined space of a design problem (Jaarsveld and van Leeuwen, 2005). In design problems the quality of cognitive production depends, in part, on the abilities to reflect on one’s own creative behavior (Boden, 1996) and to monitor how far along in the process one is in solving it (Gabora, 2002). Hence, design problems are especially suited to study more complex problem solving processes.

Knowledge Domain

Knowledge domain represents disciplines or fields of study organized by general principles, e.g., domains of various arts and sciences. It contains accumulated knowledge that can be divided in diverse content domains, and the relevant algorithms and heuristics. We also speak of knowledge domains when referring to, e.g., visuo-spatial and verbal domains. This latter differentiation may refer to the method by which performance in a certain knowledge domain is assessed, e.g., a visuo-spatial physics task that assesses the content domain of the workings of mass and weights of objects.

In comparing tests results, we should keep in mind that apart from reflecting cognitive processes evolving in different problem spaces, the results also arise from cognition operating on different knowledge domains. We argue that, the still contradictory and inconclusive discussion about the relationship between intelligence and creativity (Silvia, 2008), should involve the issue of knowledge domain.

Intelligence tests contain items that pertain to, e.g., verbal, abstract, mechanical and spatial reasoning abilities, while their content mostly operates on knowledge domains that are related to contents contained in school curricula. Items of creativity tests, by contrast, pertain to more idiosyncratic knowledge domains, their contents relating to associations between stored personal experiences (Karmiloff-Smith, 1992). The influence of knowledge domain on the relationships between different test scores was already mentioned by Guilford (1956, p. 169). This author expected a higher correlation between scores from a typical intelligence test and a divergent thinking test than between scores from two divergent thinking tests because the former pair operated on identical information and the latter pair on different information.

Studies with the CRT showed that when knowledge domain is controlled for, the development of intelligence operating in ill-defined problem space does not compare to that of traditional intelligence but develops more similarly to the development of creativity (Welter et al., in press).

Relationship Intelligence and Creativity

The Threshold theory (Guilford, 1967) predicts a relationship between intelligence and creativity up to approximately an intelligence quotient (IQ) level of 120 but not beyond (Lubart, 2003; Runco, 2007). Threshold theory was corroborated when creative potential was found to be related to intelligence up to certain IQ levels; however, the theory was refuted, when focusing on achievement in creative domains; it showed that creative achievement benefited from higher intelligence even at fairly high levels of intellectual ability (Jauk et al., 2013).

Distinguishing between subtypes of general intelligence known as fluent and crystallized intelligence (Cattell, 1967), Sligh et al. (2005) observed an inverse threshold effect with fluid IQ: a correlation with creativity test scores in the high IQ group but not in the average IQ group. Also creative achievement showed to be affected by fluid intelligence (Beaty et al., 2014). Intelligence, defined as fluid IQ, verbal fluency, and strategic abilities, showed a higher correlation with creativity scores (Silvia, 2008) than when defined as crystallized intelligence. Creativity tests, which involved convergent thinking (e.g., Remote Association Test; Mednick, 1962) showed higher correlations with intelligence than ones that involved only divergent thinking (e.g., the Alternate Uses Test; Guilford et al., 1978).

That the Remote Association test also involves convergent thinking follows from the instructions; one is asked, when presented with a stimulus word (e.g., table) to produce the first word one thinks of (e.g., chair). The word pair table–chair is a common association, more remote is the pair table–plate, and quite remote is table–shark. According to Mednick’s theory (a) all cognitive work is done essentially by combining or associating ideas and (b) individuals with more commonplace associations have an advantage in well-defined problem spaces, because the class of relevant associations is already implicit in the statement of the problem (Eysenck, 2003).

To circumvent the problem of tests differing in knowledge domain, one can develop out of one task a more divergent and a more convergent thinking task by asking, on the one hand, for the generation of original responses, and by asking, on the other hand, for more common responses (Jauk et al., 2012). By changing the instruction of a task, from convergent to divergent, one changes the constraints the solution has to answer and, thereby, one changes for cognition its freedom of operation (Razumnikova et al., 2009; Limb, 2010; Jauk et al., 2012). However, asking for more common responses is still a divergent thinking task because it instigates a generative and ideational process.

Indeed, studying the relationship between intelligence and creativity with knowledge domain controlled for yielded different results as defined in the Threshold theory. A study in which knowledge domain was controlled for showed, firstly, that intelligence is no predictor for the development of creativity (Welter et al., 2016). Secondly, that the relationship between scores of intelligence and creativity tests as defined under the Threshold theory was only observed in a small subset of primary school children, namely, female children in Grade 4 (Welter et al., 2016). We state that relating results of operations yielded by cognitive abilities performing in defined and in ill-defined problem spaces can only be informative when it is ensured that cognitive processes also operate on an identical knowledge domain.

Intertwining of Cognitive Abilities

Eysenck (2003) observed that there is little justification for considering the constructs of divergent and convergent thinking in categorical terms in which one construct excludes the other. In processes that yield original and appropriate solutions convergent and divergent thinking both operate on the same large knowledge base and the underlying cognitive processes are not entirely dissimilar (Eysenck, 2003, p. 110–111).

Divergent thinking is especially effective when it is coupled with convergent thinking (Runco, 2003; Gabora and Ranjan, 2013). A design problem study (Jaarsveld and van Leeuwen, 2005) showed that divergent production was active throughout the design, as new meanings are continuously added to the evolving structure (Akin, 1986), and that convergent production was increasingly important toward the end of the process, as earlier productions are wrapped up and integrated in the final design. These findings are in line with the assumptions of Wertheimer (1945/1968) who stated that thinking within ill-defined problem space is characterized by two points of focus; one is to work on the parts, the other to make the central idea clearer.

Parallel to the discussion about the intertwining of convergent and divergent thinking abilities in processes that evolve in ill-defined problem space we find the discussion about how intelligence may facilitate creative thought. This showed when top-down cognitive control advanced divergent processing in the generation of original ideas and a certain measure of cognitive inhibition advanced the fluency of idea generation (Nusbaum and Silvia, 2011). Fluid intelligence and broad retrieval considered as intelligence factors in a structural equation study contributed both to the production of creative ideas in a metaphor generation task (Beaty and Silvia, 2013). The notion that creative thought involves top-down, executive processes showed in a latent variable analysis where inhibition primarily promoted the fluency of ideas, and intelligence promoted their originality (Benedek et al., 2012).

Definitions of the Constructs Intelligence and Creativity

The various definitions of the constructs of intelligence and creativity show a problematic overlap. This overlap stems from the enormous endeavor to unanimously agree on valid descriptions for each construct. Spearman (1927), after having attended many symposia that aimed at defining intelligence, stated that “in truth, ‘intelligence’ has become a mere vocal sound, a word with so many meanings that finally it has none” (p. 14).

Intelligence is expressed in terms of adaptive, goal-directed behavior; and the subset of such behavior that is labeled “intelligent” seems to be determined in large part by cultural or societal norms (Sternberg and Salter, 1982). The development of the IQ measure is discussed by Carroll (1982): “Binet (around 1905) realized that intelligent behavior or mental ability can be ranged along a scale. Not much later, Stern (around 1912) noticed that, as chronological age increased, variation in mental age changes proportionally. He developed the IQ ratio, whose standard deviation would be approximately constant over chronological age if mental age was divided by chronological age. With the development of multiple-factor-analyses (Thurstone, around 1935) it could be shown that intelligence is not a simple unitary trait because at least seven somewhat independent factors of mental ability were identified.”

Creativity is defined as a combined manifestation of novelty and usefulness (Jung et al., 2010). Although it is identified with divergent thinking, and performance on divergent thinking tasks predicts, e.g., quantity of creative achievements (Torrance, 1988, as cited in Beaty et al., 2014) and quality of creative performance (Beaty et al., 2013), it cannot be identified uniquely with divergent thinking.

Divergent thinking often leads to highly original ideas that are honed to appropriate ideas by evaluative processes of critical thinking, and valuative and appreciative considerations (Runco, 2008). Divergent thinking tests should be more considered as estimates of creative problem solving potential rather than of actual creativity (Runco, 1991). Divergent thinking is not specific enough to help us understand what, exactly, are the mental processes—or the cognitive abilities—that yield creative thoughts (Dietrich, 2007).

Although current definitions of intelligence and creativity try to determine for each separate construct a unique set of cognitive abilities, analyses show that definitions vary in the degree to which each includes abilities that are generally considered to belong to the other construct (Runco, 2003; Jaarsveld et al., 2012). Abilities considered belonging to the construct of intelligence such as hypothesis testing, inhibition of alternative responses, and creating mental images of new actions or plans are also considered to be involved in creative thinking (Fuster, 1997, as cited in Colom et al., 2009, p. 215). The ability, for instance, to evaluate, which is considered to belong to the construct of intelligence and assesses the match between a proposed solution and task constraints, has long been considered to play a role in creative processes that goes beyond the mere generation of a series of ideas as in creativity tasks (Wallas, 1926, as cited in Gabora, 2002, p. 1; Boden, 1990).

The Geneplore model (Finke et al., 1992) explicitly models this idea; after stages in which objects are merely generated, follow phases in which an object’s utility is explored and estimated. The generation phase brings forth pre inventive objects, imaginary objects that are generated without any constraints in mind. In exploration, these objects are evaluated for their possible functionalities. In anticipating the functional characteristics of generated ideas, convergent thinking is needed to apprehend the situation, make evaluations (Kozbelt, 2008), and consider the consequences of a chosen solution (Goel and Pirolli, 1992). Convergent reasoning in creativity tasks invokes criteria of functionality and appropriateness (Halpern, 2003; Kaufmann, 2003), goal directedness and adaptive behavior (Sternberg, 1982), as well as the abilities of planning and attention. Convergent thinking stages may even require divergent thinking sub processes to identify restrictions on proposed new ideas and suggest requisite revision strategies (Mumford et al., 2007). Hence, evaluation, which is considered to belong to the construct of intelligence, is also functional in creative processes.

In contrast, the ability of flexibility, which is considered to belong to the construct of creativity and denotes an openness of mind that ensures the generation of ideas from different domains, showed, as a factor component for latent divergent thinking, a relationship with intelligence (Silvia, 2008). Flexibility was also found to play an important role in intelligent behavior where it enables us to do novel things smartly in new situations (Colunga and Smith, 2008). These authors studied children’s generalizations of novel nouns and concluded that if we are to understand human intelligence, we must understand the processes that make inventiveness. They propose to include the construct of flexibility within that of intelligence. Therefore, definitions of the constructs we are to measure affect test construction and the resulting data. However, an overlap between definitions, as discussed, yields a test diversity that makes it impossible to interpret the different findings across studies with any confidence (Arden et al., 2010). Also Kim (2005) concluded that because of differences in tests and administration methods, the observed correlation between intelligence and creativity was negligible. As the various definitions of the constructs of intelligence and creativity show problematic overlap, we propose to circumvent the discussion about which cognitive abilities are assessed by which construct, and to consider both constructs as being involved in one design process. This approach allows us to study the contribution to this process of the various defined abilities, without one construct excluding the other.

Reasoning Abilities

The CRT is a psychometrical tool constructed on the basis of an alternative construct of human cognitive functioning that considers creative reasoning as a thinking process understood as the cooperation between cognitive abilities related to intelligent and creative thinking.

In generating relationships for a matrix, reasoning and more specifically the ability of rule invention is applied. The ability of rule invention could be considered as an extension of the sequence of abilities of rule learning, rule inference, and rule application, implying that creativity is an extension of intelligence (Shye and Goldzweig, 1999). According to this model, we could expect different results between a task assessing abilities of rule learning and rule inference, and a task assessing abilities of rule application. In two studies rule learning and rule inference was assessed with the RPM and rule application was assessed with the CRT. Results showed that from Grades 1 to 4, the frequencies of relationships applied did not correlate with those solved (Jaarsveld et al., 2010, 2012). Results showed that performance in the CRT allows an insight of cognitive abilities operating on relationships among components that differs from the insight based on performance within the same knowledge domain in a matrix solving task. Hence, reasoning abilities lead to different performances when applied in solving closed as to open problems.

We assume that reasoning abilities are more clearly reflected when one formulates a matrix from scratch; in the process of thinking and drawing one has, so to speak, to solve one’s own matrix. In doing so one explains to oneself the relationship(s) realized so far and what one would like to attain. Drawing is thinking aloud a problem and aids the designer’s thinking processes in providing some “talk-back” (Cross and Clayburn Cross, 1996). Explanatory activity enhances learning through increased depth of processing (Siegler, 2005). Analyzing explanations of examples given with physics problems showed that they clarify and specify the conditions and consequences of actions, and that they explicate tacit knowledge; thereby enhancing and completing an individual’s understanding of principles relevant to the task (Chi and VanLehn, 1991). Constraint of the CRT is that the matrix, in principle, can be solved by another person. Therefore, in a kind of inner explanatory discussion, the designer makes observations of progress, and uses evaluations and decisions to answer this constraint. Because of this, open problems where certain constraints have to be met, constitute a powerful mechanism for promoting understanding and conceptual advancement (Chi and VanLehn, 1991; Mestre, 2002; Siegler, 2005).

Conclusion

Convergent and divergent thinking processes have been studied with a variety of intelligence and creativity tests, respectively. Relationships between performances on these tests have been demonstrated and a large number of research questions have been addressed. However, the fact that intelligence and creativity tests vary in the definition of their construct, in their problem space, and in their knowledge domain, poses methodological problems regarding the validity of comparisons of test results. When we want to focus on one cognitive process, e.g., intelligent thinking, and on its different performances in well or ill-defined problem situations, we need pairs of tasks that are constructed along identical definitions of the construct to be assessed, that differ, however, in the description of their constraints but are identical regarding their knowledge domain.

One such possible pair, the Progressive Matrices Test and the CRT was suggested here. The CRT was developed on the basis of creative reasoning, a construct that assumes the intertwining of intelligent and creativity related abilities when looking for original and applicable solutions. Matched with the Matrices test, results indicated that, besides similarities, intelligent thinking also yielded considerable differences for both problem spaces. Hence, with knowledge domain controlled, and only differences in problem space remaining, comparison of data yielded new results on intelligence’s operations. Data gathered from intelligence and creativity tests, whether they are performance scores or physiological measurements on the basis of, e.g., EEG, and fMRI methods, are reflections of cognitive processes performing on a certain test that was constructed on the basis of a certain definition of the construct it was meant to measure. Data are also reflections of the processes evolving within a certain problem space and of cognitive abilities operating on a certain knowledge domain.

Data can unhide brain networks that are involved in the performance of certain tasks, e.g., traditional intelligence and creativity tests, but data will always be related to the characteristics of the task. The characteristics of the task, such as problem space and knowledge domain originated at the construction of the task, and the construction, on its turn, is affected by the definition of the construct the task is meant to measure.

Here we present the CRT as one possible solution for the described problems in cognition research. However, for research on relationships among test scores other pairs of tests are imaginable, e.g., pairs of tasks operating on the same domain where one task has a defined problem space and the other one an ill-defined space. It is conceivable that pairs of test could operate, besides on the domain of mathematics, on content of e.g., visuo-spatial, verbal, and musical domains. Pairs of test have been constructed by changing the instruction of a task; instructions instigated a more convergent or a more a divergent mode of response (Razumnikova et al., 2009; Limb, 2010; Jauk et al., 2012; Beaty et al., 2013).

The CRT involves the creation of components and their relationships for a 3 × 3 matrix. Hence, matrices created in the CRT are original in the sense that they all bear individual markers and they are applicable in the sense, that they can, in principle, be solved by another person. We showed that the CRT instigates a real design process; creators’ cognitive abilities are wrapped up in a process that should produce a closed problem within an ill-defined problem space.

For research on the relationship among convergent and divergent thinking, we need pairs of test that differ in the problem spaces related to each test but are identical in the knowledge domain on which cognition operates. The test pair of RPM and CRT provides such a pair. For research on the intertwining of convergent and divergent thinking, we need tasks that measure more than tests assessing each construct alone. We need tasks that are developed on the definition of intertwining cognitive abilities; the CRT is one such test.

Hence, we hope to have sufficiently discussed and demonstrated the importance of the three test features, construct definition, problem space, and knowledge domain, for research questions in creative cognition research.

Author Contributions

All authors listed, have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00134/full#supplementary-material

References

Abraham, A., and Bubic, A. (2015). Semantic memory as the root of imagination. Front. Psychol. 6:325. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abraham, A., and Windmann, S. (2007). Creative cognition: the diverse operations and the prospect of applying a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Methods 42, 38–48. doi: 10.1016/j.ymeth.2006.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akin, O. (1986). Psychology of Architectural Design London: Pion.

Google Scholar

Anderson, J. R. (1983). The Architecture of Cognition Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Arden, R., Chavez, R. S., Grazioplene, R., and Jung, R. E. (2010). Neuroimaging creativity: a psychometric view. Behav. Brain Res. 214, 143–156. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnheim, R. (1962/1974). Picasso’s Guernica Berkeley: University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (1992). Cognitive Psychology: An Overview for Cognitive Scientists Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.

Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., and Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Kaufman, S. B., Benedek, M., Jung, R. E., Kenett, Y. N., Jauk, E., et al. (2015). Personality and complex brain networks: the role of openness to experience in default network efficiency. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 773–777. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Nusbaum, E. C., and Silvia, P. J. (2014). Does insight problem solving predict real-world creativity? Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 8, 287–292. doi: 10.1037/a0035727

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., and Silvia, R. E. (2013). Metaphorically speaking: cognitive abilities and the production of figurative language. Mem. Cognit. 41, 255–267. doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0258-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Smeekens, B. A., Silvia, P. J., Hodges, D. A., and Kane, M. J. (2013). A first look at the role of domain-general cognitive and creative abilities in jazz improvisation. Psychomusicology 23, 262–268. doi: 10.1037/a0034968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M., Bergner, S., Konen, T., Fink, A., and Neubauer, A. C. (2011). EEG alpha synchronization is related to top-down processing in convergent and divergent thinking. Neuropsychologia 49, 3505–3511. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., and Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Pers. Individ. Dif. 53, 480–485. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Sommer, M., Arendasy, M., and Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: the common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence 46, 73–83. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boden, M. A. (1990). The Creative Mind: Myths and Mechanisms London: Abacus.

Google Scholar

Boden, M. A. (1996). Artificial Intelligence New York, NY: Academic.

Google Scholar

Buschman, T. J., and Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315, 1860–1862. doi: 10.1126/science.1138071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carroll, J. B. (1982). “The measurement of Intelligence,” in Handbook of Human Intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 29–120.

Google Scholar

Cattell, R. B. (1967). The theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligence checked at the 5-6 year-old level. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 37, 209–224. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1967.tb01930.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chi, M. T. H. (1997). “Creativity: Shifting across ontological categories flexibly,” in Creative Thought: An Investigation of Conceptual Structures and Processes, eds T. Ward, S. Smith, and J. Vaid (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 209–234.

Google Scholar

Chi, M. T. H., and VanLehn, K. A. (1991). The content of physics self-explanations. J. Learn. Sci. 1, 69–105. doi: 10.1207/s15327809jls0101_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christensen, B. T. (2007). The relationship of analogical distance to analogical function and preinventive structure: the case of engineering design. Mem. Cogn. 35, 29–38. doi: 10.3758/BF03195939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colom, R., Haier, R. J., Head, K., Álvarez-Linera, J., Quiroga, M. A., Shih, P. C., et al. (2009). Gray matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence: testing the P-FIT model. Intelligence 37, 124–135. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colunga, E., and Smith, L. B. (2008). Flexibility and variability: essential to human cognition and the study of human cognition. New Ideas Psychol. 26, 158–192. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, N. R., Croft, R. J., Dominey, S. J. J., Burgess, A. P., and Gruzelier, J. H. (2003). Paradox lost? Exploring the role of alpha oscillations during externally vs. internally directed attention and the implications for idling and inhibition hypotheses. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 47, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(02)00107-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creat. Res. J. 18, 391–404. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1803_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cropley, A., and Cropley, D. (2008). Resolving the paradoxes of creativity: an extended phase model. Camb. J. Educ. 38, 355–373. doi: 10.1080/03057640802286871

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, N., and Clayburn Cross, A. (1996). Winning by design: the methods of Gordon Murray, racing car designer. Des. Stud. 17, 91–107. doi: 10.1016/0142-694X(95)00027-O

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dennett, D. (1978). Brainstorms: Philosophical Essays on Mind and Psychology Montgomery, VT: Bradford Books.

Google Scholar

Dietrich, A. (2007). Who’s afraid of a cognitive neuroscience of creativity? Methods 42, 22–27. doi: 10.1016/j.ymeth.2006.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorst, K. (2004). The problem of design problems: Problem solving and design expertise. J. Design Res. 4. doi: 10.1504/JDR.2004.009841

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorst, K. (2011). The core of ‘design thinking’ and its application. Des. Stud. 32, 521–532. doi: 10.1016/j.destud.2011.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J. (2003). “Creativity, personality and the convergent-divergent continuum,” in Critical Creative Processes, ed. M. A. Runco (Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press), 95–114.

Google Scholar

Fink, A., and Benedek, M. (2014). EEG alpha power and creative ideation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44, 111–123. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, A., Benedek, M., Grabner, R. H., Staudt, B., and Neubauer, A. C. (2007). Creativity meets neuroscience: experimental tasks for the neuroscientific study of creative thinking. Methods 42, 68–76. doi: 10.1016/j.ymeth.2006.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Benedek, M., Reishofer, G., Hauswirth, V., Fally, M., et al. (2009). The creative brain: investigation of brain activity during creative problem solving by means of EEG and FMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 734–748. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., and Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative Cognition: Theory, Research, and Applications Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M. (1997). Network memory. Trends Neurosci. 20, 451–459. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01128-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabora, L. (2002). “Cognitive mechanisms underlying the creative process,” in Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Creativity and Cognition, eds T. Hewett and T. Kavanagh (Loughborough: Loughborough University), 126–133.

Google Scholar

Gabora, L. (2010). Revenge of the ‘neurds’: Characterizing creative thought in terms of the structure and dynamics of human memory. Creat. Res. J. 22, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/10400410903579494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabora, L., and Kaufman, S. B. (2010). “Evolutionary approaches to creativity,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity, eds J. S. Kaufman and R. J. Sternberg (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 279–300.

Google Scholar

Gabora, L., and Ranjan, A. (2013). “How insight emerges in a distributed, content-addressable memory,” in The Neuroscience of Creativity, eds A. Bristol, O. Vartanian, and J. Kaufman (Cambridge: MIT Press), 19–43.

Google Scholar

Gabora, L., and Saab, A. (2011). “Creative inference and states of potentiality in analogy problem solving,” in Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Boston, MA, 3506–3511.

Gentner, D. (1983). Structure mapping: a theoretical framework for analogy. Cogn. Sci. 7, 155–170. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0702_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Getzels, J. W. (1975). Problem finding and the inventiveness of solutions. J. Creat. Behav. 9, 12–18. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.1975.tb00552.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Getzels, J. W. (1987). “Creativity, intelligence, and problem finding: retrospect and prospect,” in Frontiers of Creativity Research: Beyond the Basics, ed. S. G. Isaksen (Buffalo, NY: Bearly Limited), 88–102.

Google Scholar

Getzels, J. W., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The Creative Vision: A Longitudinal Study of Problem Finding in Art New York, NY: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Ghiselin, B. (ed.). (1952/1985). The Creative Process Los Angeles: University of California.

Google Scholar

Goel, V., and Pirolli, P. (1992). The structure of design problem spaces. Cogn. Sci. 16, 395–429. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1603_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldschmidt, G. (2013). “A micro view of design reasoning: two-way shifts between embodiment and rationale,” in Creativity and Rationale: Enhancing Human Experience by Design, Human-Computer Interaction Series, ed. J. M. Carroll (London: Springer Verlag). doi: 10.1007/978-1-4471-2_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldschmidt, G. (2014). Linkography: Unfolding the Design Process Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Grube, H. E., and Davis, S. N. (1988). “Inching our way up mount Olympus: The evolving-systems approach to creative thinking,” in The Nature of Creativity, ed. R. J. Sternberg (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 243–270.

Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. Am. Psychol. 5, 444–454. doi: 10.1037/h0063487

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P. (1956). The structure of intellect model. Psychol. Bull. 53, 267–293. doi: 10.1037/h0040755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P. (1959). “Traits of creativity,” in Creativity and its Cultivation, ed. H. H. Anderson (New York: Harper), 142–161.

Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P. (1967). The Nature of Human Intelligence New York, NY: McGraw-Hill, Inc.

Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P., Christensen, P. R., Merrifield, P. R., and Wilson, R. C. (1978). Alternate Uses: Manual of Instructions and Interpretation Orange, CA: Sheridan Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Halpern, D. F. (2003). “Thinking critically about creative thinking,” in Critical Creative Processes, ed. M. A. Runco (Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press), 189–208.

Google Scholar

Hayes, J. R., and Flowers, L. S. (1986). Writing research and the writer. Am. Psychol. 41, 1106–1113. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.41.10.1106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaarsveld, S. (2007). Creative Cognition: New Perspectives on Creative Thinking Kaiserslautern: University of Kaiserslautern Press.

Google Scholar

Jaarsveld, S., Fink, A., Rinner, M., Schwab, D., Benedek, M., and Lachmann, T. (2015). Intelligence in creative processes; an EEG study. Intelligence 49, 171–178. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaarsveld, S., Lachmann, T., Hamel, R., and van Leeuwen, C. (2010). Solving and creating Raven Progressive Matrices: reasoning in well and ill defined problem spaces. Creat. Res. J. 22, 304–319. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2010.503541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaarsveld, S., Lachmann, T., and van Leeuwen, C. (2012). Creative reasoning across developmental levels: convergence and divergence in problem creation. Intelligence 40, 172–188. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaarsveld, S., Lachmann, T., and van Leeuwen, C. (2013). “The impact of problem space on reasoning: Solving versus creating matrices,” in Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, eds M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, and I. Wachsmuth (Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society), 2632–2638.

Google Scholar

Jaarsveld, S., and van Leeuwen, C. (2005). Sketches from a design process: creative cognition inferred from intermediate products. Cogn. Sci. 29, 79–101. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2901_4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., and Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship between intelligence and creativity: new support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence 41, 212–221. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jauk, E., Benedek, M., and Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Tackling creativity at its roots: evidence for different patterns of EEG alpha activity related to convergent and divergent modes of task processing. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 84, 219–225. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaušovec, N. (1999). “Brain biology and brain functioning,” in Encyclopedia of Creativity, eds M. A. Runco and S. R. Pritzker (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 203–212.

Google Scholar

Jaušovec, N. (2000). Differences in cognitive processes between gifted, intelligent, creative, and average individuals while solving complex problems: an EEG Study. Intelligence 28, 213–237. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(00)00037-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, R. E. (2014). Evolution, creativity, intelligence, and madness: “here be dragons”. Front. Psychol 5:784. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, R. E., and Haier, R. J. (2013). “Creativity and intelligence,” in Neuroscience of Creativity, eds O. Vartanian, A. S. Bristol, and J. C. Kaufman (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 233–254.

Google Scholar

Jung, R. E., Segall, J. M., Bockholt, H. J., Flores, R. A., Smith, S. M., Chavez, R. S., et al. (2010). Neuroanatomy of creativity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 398–409. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20874

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Kaufman, J. C. (2015). Why creativity isn’t in IQ tests, why it matters, and why it won’t change anytime soon probably. Intelligence 3, 59–72. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence303005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufmann, G. (2003). What to measure? A new look at the concept of creativity. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 47, 235–251. doi: 10.1080/00313830308604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, K. H. (2005). Can only intelligent people be creative? J. Second. Gift. Educ. 16, 57–66.

Google Scholar

Koestler, A. (1964). The Act of Creation London: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Kozbelt, A. (2008). Hierarchical linear modeling of creative artists’ problem solving behaviors. J. Creat. Behav. 42, 181–200. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.2008.tb01294.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kulkarni, D., and Simon, H. A. (1988). The processes of scientific discovery: the strategy of experimentation. Cogn. Sci. 12, 139–175. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2009.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Limb, C. J. (2010). Your Brain on Improve Available at: http://www.ted.com/talks/charles_limb_your_brain_on_improv

Lubart, T. I. (2001). Models of the creative process: past, present and future. Creat. Res. J. 13, 295–308. doi: 10.1207/S15326934CRJ1334_07

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubart, T. I. (2003). Psychologie de la Créativité. Cursus. Psychologie Paris: Armand Colin.

Google Scholar

Martindale, C. (1999). “Biological basis of creativity,” in Handbook of Creativity, ed. R. J. Sternberg (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 137–152.

Google Scholar

Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychol. Rev. 69, 220–232. doi: 10.1037/h0048850

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendelssohn, G. A. (1976). Associational and attentional processes in creative performance. J. Pers. 44, 341–369. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1976.tb00127.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mestre, J. P. (2002). Probing adults’ conceptual understanding and transfer of learning via problem posing. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 23, 9–50. doi: 10.1016/S0193-3973(01)00101-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mumford, M. D., Hunter, S. T., Eubanks, D. L., Bedell, K. E., and Murphy, S. T. (2007). Developing leaders for creative efforts: a domain-based approach to leadership development. Hum. Res. Manag. Rev. 17, 402–417. doi: 10.1016/j.hrmr.2007.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newell, A., and Simon, H. A. (1972). “The theory of human problem solving,” in Human Problem Solving, eds A. Newell and H. Simon (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall), 787–868.

Google Scholar

Nusbaum, E. C., and Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Intelligence 39, 36–40. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2010.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmiero, M., Nori, R., Aloisi, V., Ferrara, M., and Piccardi, L. (2015). Domain-specificity of creativity: a study on the relationship between visual creativity and visual mental imagery. Front. Psychol. 6:1870. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piaget, J., Montangero, J., and Billeter, J. (1977). “La formation des correlats,” in Recherches sur L’abstraction Reflechissante I, ed. J. Piaget (Paris: Presse Universitaires de France), 115–129.

Google Scholar

Plucker, J. (1999). Is the proof in the pudding? Reanalyses of torrance’s (1958 to present) longitudinal study data. Creat. Res. J. 12, 103–114. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1202_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raven, J. C. (1938/1998). Standard Progressive Matrices, Sets A, B, C, D & E Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.

Razumnikova, O. M., Volf, N. V., and Tarasova, I. V. (2009). Strategy and results: sex differences in electrographic correlates of verbal and figural creativity. Hum. Physiol. 35, 285–294. doi: 10.1134/S0362119709030049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runco, M. A. (1991). The evaluative, valuative, and divergent thinking of children. J. Creat. Behav. 25, 311–319. doi: 10.1177/1073858414568317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runco, M. A. (2003). “Idea evaluation, divergent thinking, and creativity,” in Critical Creative Processes, ed. M. A. Runco (Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press), 69–94.

Google Scholar

Runco, M. A. (2007). Creativity, Theories and Themes: Research, Development, and Practice New York, NY: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Runco, M. A. (2008). Commentary: divergent thinking is not synonymous with creativity. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2, 93–96. doi: 10.1037/1931-3896.2.2.93

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakar, P., and Chakrabarti, A. (2013). Support for protocol analyses in design research. Des. Issues 29, 70–81. doi: 10.1162/DESI_a_00231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saraç, S., Önder, A., and Karakelle, S. (2014). The relations among general intelligence, metacognition and text learning performance. Educ. Sci. 39, 40–53.

Google Scholar

Shye, S., and Goldzweig, G. (1999). Creativity as an extension of intelligence: Faceted definition and structural hypotheses. Megamot 40, 31–53.

Google Scholar

Shye, S., and Yuhas, I. (2004). Creativity in problem solving. Tech. Rep. doi: 10.13140/2.1.1940.0643

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R. S. (1998). Children’s Thinking, 3rd Edn. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall, 28–50.

Google Scholar

Siegler, R. S. (2005). Children’s learning. Am. Psychol. 60, 769–778. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.8.769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvia, P. J. (2008). Creativity and intelligence revisited: a reanalysis of Wallach and Kogan (1965). Creat. Res. J. 20, 34–39. doi: 10.1080/10400410701841807

CrossRef Full Text

Silvia, P. J., Beaty, R. E., and Nussbaum, E. C. (2013). Verbal fluency and creativity: general and specific contributions of broad retrieval ability (Gr) factors to divergent thinking. Intelligence 41, 328–340. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, H. A. (1973). The structure of ill structured problems. Artif. Intell. 4, 1012–1021. doi: 10.1016/0004-3702(73)90011-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, H. A., and Newell, A. (1971). Human problem solving: state of theory in 1970. Am. Psychol. 26, 145–159. doi: 10.1037/h0030806

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sligh, A. C., Conners, F. A., and Roskos-Ewoldsen, B. (2005). Relation of creativity to fluid and crystallized intelligence. J. Creat. Behav. 39, 123–136. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.2005.tb01254.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spearman, C. (1904). ‘General intelligence,’ objectively determined and measured. Am. J. Psychol. 15, 201–293. doi: 10.2307/1412107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spearman, C. (1927). The Abilities of Man London: Macmillan.

Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J. (1982). “Conceptions of intelligence,” in Handbook of Human Intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 3–28.

Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J. (2005). “The WICS model of giftedness,” in Conceptions of Giftedness, 2nd Edn, eds R. J. Sternberg and J. E. Davidson (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 237–243.

Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J., and Lubart, T. I. (1999). “The concept of creativity: Prospects and paradigms,” in Handbook of Creativity, ed. R. J. Sternberg (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 3–15.

Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J., and Salter, W. (1982). “The nature of intelligence and its measurements,” in Handbook of Human Intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 3–24.

Thagard, P., and Verbeurgt, K. (1998). Coherence as constraint satisfaction. Cogn. Sci. 22, l–24. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2201_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrance, E. P. (1988). “The nature of creativity as manifest in its testing,” in The Nature of Creativity: Contemporary Psychological Perspectives, ed. R. J. Sternberg (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 43–75.

Google Scholar

Urban, K. K., and Jellen, H. G. (1995). Test of Creative Thinking – Drawing Production Frankfurt: Swets Test Services.

Google Scholar

van Leeuwen, C., Verstijnen, I. M., and Hekkert, P. (1999). “Common unconscious dynamics underlie uncommon conscious effect: a case study in the iterative nature of perception and creation,” in Modeling Consciousness Across the Disciplines, ed. J. S. Jordan (Lanham, MD: University Press of America), 179–218.

Vernon, P. E. (ed.). (1970). Creativity London: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Verstijnen, I. M., Heylighen, A., Wagemans, J., and Neuckermans, H. (2001). “Sketching, analogies, and creativity,” in Visual and Spatial Reasoning in Design, II. Key Centre of Design Computing and Cognition, eds J. S. Gero, B. Tversky, and T. Purcell (Sydney, NSW: University of Sydney).

Google Scholar

Wallas, G. (1926). The Art of Thought New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace & World.

Google Scholar

Ward, T. B. (2007). Creative cognition as a window on creativity. Methods 42, 28–37. doi: 10.1016/j.ymeth.2006.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb Young, J. (1939/2003). A Technique for Producing Ideas New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Welter, M. M., Jaarsveld, S., and Lachmann, T. (in press). Problem space matters: development of creativity and intelligence in primary school children. Creat. Res. J.

Welter, M. M., Jaarsveld, S., van Leeuwen, C., and Lachmann, T. (2016). Intelligence and creativity; over the threshold together? Creat. Res. J. 28, 212–218. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2016.1162564

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wertheimer, M. (1945/1968). Productive Thinking (Enlarged Edition) London: Tavistock.

Google Scholar

Yamamoto, Y., Nakakoji, K., and Takada, S. (2000). Hand on representations in two dimensional spaces for early stages of design. Knowl. Based Syst. 13, 357–384. doi: 10.1016/S0950-7051(00)00078-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: creative cognition, cognitive neuroscience, creative reasoning, problem space, knowledge domain

Citation: Jaarsveld S and Lachmann T (2017) Intelligence and Creativity in Problem Solving: The Importance of Test Features in Cognition Research. Front. Psychol. 8:134. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00134

Received: 05 July 2016; Accepted: 19 January 2017;
Published: 06 February 2017.

Edited by:

Anatoliy V. Kharkhurin, American University of Sharjah, UAE

Reviewed by:

Tanja Gabriele Baudson, Technical University of Dortmund, Germany
Massimiliano Palmiero, University of L’Aquila, Italy

Copyright © 2017 Jaarsveld and Lachmann. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Saskia Jaarsveld, jaarsvel@rhrk.uni-kl.de Thomas Lachmann, lachmann@sowi.uni-kl.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.