Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 26 March 2020
Sec. Forensic and Legal Psychology

Living in a Disadvantaged Neighborhood Affects Neural Processing of Facial Trustworthiness

  • Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States

Neighborhood disorder (i.e., physical or social decay) is associated with decreased trust, which reinforces criminal behavior among some individuals in these communities. However, past research largely is descriptive and has not measured processes underlying trust. Using behavioral and neural indices [the late positive potential (LPP), a marker of salience elaboration] in a sample of adults (N = 55), we examined the association between perceived neighborhood disorder and facial trustworthiness perception as well as the potential moderating role of trustworthiness perception on the association between PND and criminal behavior. Individuals with higher perceived neighborhood disorder displayed less LPP differentiation between untrustworthy and trustworthy faces. Moreover, individuals with higher perceived neighborhood disorder and less LPP differentiation were less likely to commit a variety of crimes, whereas those with higher perceptions of neighborhood disorder and high LPP differentiation were more likely to commit a variety of crimes. Combined, these findings suggest that similarly processing trustworthy and untrustworthy faces, as indexed by less LPP differentiation, may reflect an adaptation among those with higher perceived neighborhood disorder that mitigates against deviant behavior and contacts with the law. Understanding the intersection between neighborhood characteristics and individual-level cognitive-affective processing may provide insight into what shapes beliefs and behaviors about important social information.

Introduction

Since the 2000’s, the number of distressed neighborhoods in the United States grew by nearly three-quarters (Kneebone, 2014). These neighborhoods are typically defined as having at least 40% of its residents living in poverty and are associated with an array of challenges that negatively impact its residents – whether they are poor or not. For example, distressed neighborhoods are high in neighborhood disorder, which refers to observed or perceived physical (e.g., graffiti, litter) and social (e.g., prostitution, gang activity) decay. Residents in neighborhoods with high disorder exhibit poorer physical and mental health and are at higher risk for being both the victims and perpetrators of crime (see Freedman and Woods, 2013 for review). Researchers suggest that the elevated rates of social problems (e.g., crime) in neighborhoods with high levels of disorder may relate to weakened trust in one’s neighbor and in individuals outside of the neighborhood (Sampson et al., 1997, 2008; Freedman and Woods, 2013). Notably, residents in neighborhoods marked by high disorder report increased fear of victimization by others (Ross and Mirowsky, 2009), decreased trust in their neighbors and in general (Ross et al., 2001; Marschall and Stolle, 2004; Ross, 2011), and reduced trust in the police (Nix et al., 2014). Despite this research showing that high neighborhood disorder is associated with the erosion of trust (Ross et al., 2001; Ross, 2011; Steenbeek and Hipp, 2011), no research has examined the social cognitive process that underlie the formation and maintenance of trust.

Individuals form stable trustworthiness judgments about another person based solely on their facial features, even in the absence of information about the other person’s behavior or emotional state (Oosterhof and Todorov, 2008). These judgments of trustworthiness are formed within just 100 ms of exposure to the face (Willis and Todorov, 2006). Furthermore, they affect the ways in which individuals decide to engage with others (e.g., investing more money in trustworthy vs. untrustworthy faces; Chang et al., 2010; Ewing et al., 2015; approaching trustworthy faces faster than untrustworthy faces; Slepian et al., 2017). Thus, facial trustworthiness perception is an important social cue that can influence and guide behavior.

Considering how quickly judgments of trustworthiness are formed, the high temporal resolution of event-related potentials (ERPs) makes them particularly well-suited for investigating the neural processing that underlies these rapid judgments. ERPs can reveal information about the specific cognitive processes (e.g., face vs. emotion processing) that relate to trustworthiness judgments. Past research indicates that facial trustworthiness elicits distinct patterns of neural activity. Untrustworthy faces evoke larger brain responses by 300 ms [i.e., referred to as the late positive potential (LPP), a measure of salience elaboration; Foti et al., 2009; Hajcak et al., 2009]. Compared to subjectively (Marzi et al., 2014; Lischke et al., 2017) and pre-classified (Yang et al., 2011) neutral and trustworthy faces, untrustworthy faces evoke larger LPPs. Taken together, these studies suggest that untrustworthy faces are more salient and recruit more neural resources to process than trustworthy and neutral faces in general.

However, the majority of research on trustworthiness perception has been conducted in undergraduate samples (e.g., Engell et al., 2007; Todorov et al., 2008; Oosterhof and Todorov, 2009; Todorov et al., 2009; Dotsch and Todorov, 2012) or in lesion patients (Adolphs et al., 1998; Todorov and Duchaine, 2008; Todorov and Olson, 2008). No studies have examined how a social context, like neighborhood disorder, differentially impacts processes underlying facial trustworthiness perception and connects to criminal behavior. Exploring the links between neighborhood disorder and trustworthiness processing are especially important given the two separate lines of research showing that trust behavior may be affected by the experience of neighborhood disorder and that trustworthiness perception is essential for promoting social engagement.

The goal of the present study was to investigate the association between perceived neighborhood disorder (PND) and processing of facial trustworthiness. We opted to use a measure of perceived neighborhood disorder, as opposed to census data, because it assessed one’s recognition of visible cues in their neighborhood that indicate lack of order and social control (Ross and Mirowsky, 1999; Matthews et al., 2019). This assessment of self-reported perceptions of these neighborhood cues was theoretically aligned with the concepts related to perception of facial cues that might signal trustworthiness. First, we explored the association between PND and processing (i.e., ratings and LPP) of facial trustworthiness. Second, given that not all individuals in neighborhoods marked by disorder engage in criminal behavior, we explored whether the association between PND and processes underlying trustworthiness perception predicted engagement in criminal behavior.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from New Haven County, Connecticut. New Haven ranks in the 95th percentile for crime (Note: Data accessed from https://www.neighborhoodscout.com/ct/new-haven/crime on 9/15/19). Additionally, the County contains blocks of areas ranked nationally in the 100th percentile of neighborhood disadvantage, as measured by the Area Deprivation Index (data accessed on 9/15/19).1 Adults aged 18–66 (M = 41, SD = 13) were recruited through flyers advertising for individuals who engage in risk-taking behavior (e.g., gambling, alcohol, and drug use). These demographic features, combined with our targeted recruitment of “risk-takers,” resulted in a sample with varied exposure to neighborhood disorder and enriched for criminal behavior (see Table 1 for sample characteristics).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics (n = 55).

A priori power analyses, based on previous studies on related topics (e.g., neural processing of facial trustworthiness perception (Yang et al., 2011; Marzi et al., 2014); were conducted using GPower statistical software (Faul et al., 2007). Power analyses indicated that a sample size of 50 participants would result in sufficient (80%) power to detect a moderate effect size for the omnibus interaction between level of trustworthiness of the face and one continuous predictor. All measures, manipulations, and exclusions in the study are disclosed in the sections that follow.

A prescreen phone interview and initial session including an in-person clinical assessment were used to exclude individuals who were younger than 18 or over 75, had performed below a fourth-grade level of a standardized measure of reading (WRAT-III; Wilkinson, 1993), who scored below 70 on a brief measure of IQ (Shipley; Zachary, 1986), who had diagnoses of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, or psychosis not otherwise specified, or who had a history of medical problems (e.g., uncorrectable auditory or visual deficits, seizures, head injury with loss of consciousness >30 min, color blindness) that may impact their comprehension of materials and performance on the task. Participants also completed a self-report measure of neighborhood disorder during the initial session. During the second session, participants completed the experimental task. All participants provided written informed consent and experimental procedures were approved by the Yale University Human Investigation Committee. Participants were paid $10/h per session.

Measures

Perceived Neighborhood Disorder Scale (PND; Ross and Mirowsky, 1999)

The PND scale is a 15-item self-report measure of physical (i.e., graffiti, abandoned buildings) and social (i.e., police protection, loitering) order in one’s community. Scores are summated into a total PND score (range 15–60) with higher scores indicating higher levels of PND in one’s community (sample M = 32.62, SD = 9.70). The PND scale has been found to be a reliable and valid measure of PND (Ross and Mirowsky, 1999). For this sample, internal consistency (i.e., reliability) was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = 0.75).

Criminal Behavior

All participants were asked if they ever committed a crime, even if they were never officially charged. Participant self-report was corroborated using the State of Connecticut Department of Correction inmate database. Crimes were coded as non-violent or violent based on state legal code. A variable was created that indexed the variety of types of crimes that the participant reported (0 = none, 1 = one type; non-violent or violent, 2 = two types; non-violent and violent). This type of variable was used because a significant portion of the sample endorsed engagement in criminal activity (see Table 1), therefore, accounting for the variety in types of criminal engagement provided a more meaningful measure of variability in criminal behavior.

Experimental Procedure

Apparatus

Presentation of all stimuli and measurement of behavioral responses was controlled using the Pyschtoolbox extension in MATLAB. EEG recording was controlled by a MATLAB script and Neuroscan Synamps amplifiers and acquisition software (Compumedics, North Carolina). All tasks were presented on a Ben-Q 27-inch high performance LED gaming monitor. Participants’ eyes were at a distance of 75 cm from the screen. Participants registered their responses using a button box.

Stimuli

Stimuli were obtained from a set of computer-generated faces manipulated on a dimension of trustworthiness developed by Todorov and Oosterhof (2011) and Oosterhof and Todorov (2008). This dimension of trustworthiness was derived by Oosterhof and Todorov (2008) using a model-based approach, which determined the four facial features most strongly driving judgments of trustworthiness. The four facial features that constitute this dimension of trustworthiness are the brow ridge (i.e., inner eyebrows), cheekbones, chins, and nose sellion. For example, more trustworthy faces are characterized by high inner eyebrows, pronounced cheekbones, wide chins, and shallow nose sellions, whereas more untrustworthy faces are characterized by the opposite directionality of these facial features. Manipulation of this dimension (i.e., four facial features) has been used to generate faces varying on levels of trustworthiness. The faces were generated by the Todorov group using FaceGen 3.1 (Singular Inversions, 2005) and were all male, bald, Caucasian, and front facing with direct gaze. The set contained 100 unique identities varied on three levels of trustworthiness, for a total of 300 faces. The three levels of trustworthiness represented less trustworthy (−3 SD), neutral (0 SD), and more trustworthy (+3 SD) versions of each facial identity. This stimulus set has been used in previous EEG studies investigating evaluation of facial trustworthiness (Yang et al., 2011; Marzi et al., 2014). Furthermore, use of this dataset allowed the ratings of trustworthiness made by our participants to be compared against consensus ratings (Oosterhof and Todorov, 2008).

Trustworthiness Rating Task

Participants were instructed to rate the trustworthiness of the face that appeared on the screen. It was emphasized that participants should go with their “gut reaction” when rating the face, as faces would be presented briefly. When prompted by an instruction screen, participants rated the presented face by pressing either “trustworthy,” “neutral,” or “untrustworthy” labeled buttons on a button box with the index finger of the dominant hand.

In total, participants completed 180 trials comprised of 60 randomly selected identities that varied on 3 levels of trustworthiness. The trustworthiness ratings task consisted of 6 blocks of 30 trials. To control for repetition effects, the order of the stimuli was randomized such that the same facial identity was not presented more than once in a block. Each trial consisted of a white fixation cross presented on a black background for 500 ms, followed by presentation of a target face for 750 ms, and finally an instruction screen: “Rate the face on trustworthiness.” Participants had up to 2,250 ms to respond with a button press. If the participant did not respond within that time, the task automatically advanced to the next trial. The intertrial interval was variable between 1,000 and 2,000 ms.

Behavioral Data Reduction

To examine agreement in rating of facial trustworthiness with respect to level of trustworthiness as determined by Todorov et al.’s original ratings (Oosterhof and Todorov, 2008), a percentage of “congruent” responses was calculated by dividing the number of rated faces that matched Todorov et al.’s original ratings (Oosterhof and Todorov, 2008) by the total number of faces presented for each level of trustworthiness (i.e., 60). Thus, a percentage of congruent ratings was calculated for untrustworthy, trustworthy, and neutral faces.

Psychophysiological Recording and Data Reduction

EEG was continuously collected at a 2,500 Hz sampling rate from 8 AG/AgCl electrodes based on the 10–20 system (Fz, Cz, Pz, CP3, CP4, Oz, O1, O2) and referenced to the left mastoid electrode. Vertical electro-oculographic (VEOG) activity was recorded in line with the pupil above and below the left eye and utilized to correct for ocular artifact. An upper forehead electrode was used for ground. Electrode impedances for all channels were kept below 10 KΩ by lightly abrading the scalp and applying gel in the sensor cups at the start of the experimental session.

Data was processed offline using the PhysBox plugin (Curtin, 2011) within the EEGLab toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) in MATLAB. Offline processing included low-pass and high-pass filtering (2nd-order, 30 Hz low-pass Butterworth filter; 0.5 Hz high pass filter), epoching (−200 to 750 ms epochs), baseline correction (200 ms), and artifact rejection (rejection of trials with voltages exceeding ±100 V).

Waveforms were averaged based on subjective trustworthiness ratings (trustworthy, neutral, or untrustworthy). Participants were excluded from analyses if they rated fewer than 10 faces for any of the three face types (e.g., only rated five faces as trustworthy), as this resulted in insufficient trials to generate a robust ERP. Thirteen participants were eliminated for this criterion. Participants were also excluded if they had fewer than 10 valid trials for any of the three face types [e.g., controlling for electrical noise and excluding trials with significant artifact (±100 V)]. This criterion eliminated four participants. Finally, three outliers on the omnibus interaction and EEG main effects were identified using Studentized residuals (with Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.04) and were excluded. The final sample was 55,2 and data collection did not continue after data analysis began.

Based on the grand average event-related potential (ERP) waveform, previous literature examining the neural correlates of trustworthiness appraisal (Marzi et al., 2014), and EEG literature indicating that the LPP is maximal over central-parietal sites (Hajcak et al., 2010), the LPP was measured as the average amplitude over CP3 and CP4 between 450 and 750 ms. In order to confirm our electrode selection and guide further analyses, a General Linear Model (GLM) with LPP amplitude as the continuous, dependent measure and scalp site (O1, O2, Oz, Cz, Fz, Pz, CP3, CP4, and Cz) as a between-subjects factor was conducted. Results indicated a main effect of scalp site F(1, 54) = 45.61, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.46, and a within-subject interaction of scalp site F(7, 378) = 9.31, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.15. Post-hoc interaction contrasts revealed that there were no significant differences between CP3 and CP4, thus further analyses were conducted by averaging amplitudes for CP3 and CP4 electrodes.

Results

Ratings

To examine the effect of PND on ratings of facial trustworthiness, we entered the percentage of congruent ratings in a GLM with face type (neutral, trustworthy, or untrustworthy) as a within-subjects factor and PND total score as a continuous covariate. Planned Helmert contrasts of neutral vs. trustworthy and untrustworthy faces and trustworthy vs. untrustworthy faces were conducted. To protect against violations of the assumption of sphericity, Huynh-Feldt corrected p values were reported for all following analyses. Results showed no significant interaction between PND score and face type F(1.41, 74.68) = 0.23, p = 0.718, ηp2 = 0.004 on the percentage of congruent ratings of trustworthiness. Additionally, there was no main effect of PND on face type, F(1, 53) = 0.18, p = 0.671, ηp2 = 0.003. This suggests that PND does not affect the ability to rate perceptions of facial trustworthiness in a manner congruent with established norms.

Neural Reactivity to Faces

To examine the effect of PND on LPP, mean LPP amplitude was entered in a GLM with face type (neutral, trustworthy, or untrustworthy) as a within-subjects factor and PND total score as a continuous variable. Planned Helmert contrasts of neutral vs. trustworthy and untrustworthy faces and trustworthy vs. untrustworthy faces were conducted. Results showed a significant interaction between face type and PND score F(2, 106) = 3.23, p = 0.043, ηp2 = 0.06 on LPP.3 Planned Helmert contrasts showed no interaction of PND with face type when comparing LPP to neutral vs. trustworthy/untrustworthy faces, F(1, 53) = 1.34, p = 0.252, ηp2 = 0.03, but there was a significant interaction of PND on LPP when comparing trustworthy and untrustworthy faces F(1, 53) = 5.60, p = 0.022, ηp2 = 0.10 (see Figures 1A,B). In order to determine whether the effect on LPP is specific to PND, rather than poverty more generally, we ran the same analysis controlling for household income. Including household income as a covariate in the above model did not affect the statistical significance of primary results, F(2, 104) = 3.10, p = 0.049, ηp2 = 0.06. Thus, the interaction between PND and trustworthy vs. untrustworthy faces revealed that higher PND, independent of household income, is related to decreased differentiation in motivated neural processing of trustworthy and untrustworthy faces.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Average ERP waveform at electrode CP4, where LPP mean amplitude was maximal for individuals low and high on PND total score. Primary analyses were conducted using continuous PND scores, and “low PND” (individuals who scored below the mean on the PND variable) and “high PND” (individuals who scored above the mean on the PND variable) groups were created solely for ease of visual representation of results. Dashed lines represent the low PND group, and the solid lines represent the high PND group. The black lines represent trials where the face was rated as trustworthy, and the gray lines represent trials where the face was rated as untrustworthy. The gray box indicates the timeframe (450–740 ms) used to derive the LPP mean amplitude measure. (B) Regression lines represent LPP amplitude to trustworthy and untrustworthy faces as a function of perceived neighborhood disorder (PND total; lines represent scores at ±1 SD from the mean). Error bands indicate 1 SE.

Neighborhood Disorder, Neural Processing of Faces, and Engagement in Criminal Behavior

To investigate whether difference in neural processing of trustworthy and untrustworthy faces moderates the relationship between PND and variety of criminal engagement, a regression model was constructed as follows: X = PND total (z-scored), Y = variety in types of committed crimes (0–2), M = LPP to untrustworthy faces minus LPP to trustworthy faces (z-scored). The overall model was significant R2 = 0.18, F(3, 51) = 3.62, p = 0.019, as was the interaction term between PND and difference in LPP to untrustworthy and trustworthy faces b = 0.33, t(51) = 2.89, p = 0.006, which accounted for a significant proportion of variance ΔR2 = 0.14, F(1, 51) = 8.35, p = 0.006. Moreover, to examine the conditional effect of PND on engagement in crime at each level of difference in LPP to untrustworthy and trustworthy faces, this interaction was decomposed using the Johnson-Neyman procedure. At low LPP untrustworthy and trustworthy differentiation (specifically, below −0.23), PND was significantly negatively correlated with variety in types of committed crimes (p-value range: 0.002–0.050); however, as the difference in LPP to untrustworthy and trustworthy faces increased (i.e., >2.02), PND was significantly positively correlated with variety in types of crimes committed (p-value range: 0.051–0.040) (see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Variety of types of crime committed as a function of difference in LPP amplitude to untrustworthy and trustworthy faces (z-scored; lines represent scores at ±1 SD from the mean) and perceived neighborhood disorder (PND total z-scored). Error bands indicate 1 SE.

Discussion

The impact of trust on the association between neighborhood disorder and social problems is of considerable interest and has generated mounting empirical support (Sampson, 2008; Sampson and Graif, 2009; Freedman and Woods, 2013). The present study adds to prior work by showing that individuals with higher perceptions of neighborhood disorder differ in their neural processing of facial trustworthiness. Specifically, individuals higher on perceptions of neighborhood disorder displayed less LPP differentiation between untrustworthy and trustworthy faces. Perceptions of neighborhood disorder had no effect on rating facial trustworthiness.

People are able to form trustworthiness judgments quickly from someone’s appearance with high reliability across individuals (Engell et al., 2007; Todorov et al., 2013; FeldmanHall et al., 2018). In the present study, despite variability in perceptions of neighborhood disorder, there was no difference in the ability to label a face as trustworthy or untrustworthy compared to the original ratings based on the Todorov and colleagues computer-generated faces (Oosterhof and Todorov, 2008). Even patients with prosopagnosia show normal trustworthiness judgments (Todorov and Duchaine, 2008). In fact, the only evidence, to our knowledge, that supports disruptions in the ability to provide discernable ratings for trustworthy- and untrustworthy-looking faces comes from individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions (Adolphs et al., 1998). However, while the ability to form trustworthiness judgments may be intact for various individuals, this level of assessment can be dissociated from the neural processes that subserve the formation of those judgments.

In the present study, higher perceptions of neighborhood disorder related to less differentiation in neural processing of untrustworthy and trustworthy faces, as indexed by the mean amplitude of the LPP, a neural marker of elaboration on salient stimuli. This suggests that individuals with higher perceptions of neighborhood disorder are neurally processing trustworthy and untrustworthy faces as equally salient, when previous research shows that untrustworthy faces elicit the greatest LPP response compared to trustworthy and neutral faces (Yang et al., 2011; Marzi et al., 2014; Lischke et al., 2017). One interpretation of this perceived neighborhood disorder-less LPP differentiation pattern is that it reflects increased caution engaging with salient social cues, since both positively-valenced (trustworthy faces) and negatively-valenced (untrustworthy faces) stimuli are processed similarly (Williams et al., 2006). While cautiousness is potentially adaptive in unpredictable environments (e.g., protective against potential danger, Olsson et al., 2018), an overgeneralization of that cautiousness could curtail social engagement and increase risk for some social problems in the long run (e.g., social isolation, Cacioppo and Hawkley, 2009). An alternative interpretation of this perceived neighborhood disorder-less LPP differentiation pattern is that it reflects a consequence of having less experience observing and discriminating among types of social stimuli. This interpretation stems from research that highlights increased perceptions of neighborhood disorder are associated with having fewer neighborhood social ties (Ross and Jang, 2000; Oh, 2003; Kim, 2010) and greater feelings of loneliness (Matthews et al., 2019). A separate line of research also demonstrates that decreased exposure to social experiences (e.g., institutionalized children compared to family-reared children; Moulson et al., 2009; Slopen et al., 2012) and self-reported loneliness (Cacioppo and Hawkley, 2009) are associated with abnormal patterns of neural activity to social stimuli (i.e., faces). Thus, for some individuals who perceive higher levels of neighborhood disorder, reduced differentiation in neural processing of trustworthy and untrustworthy faces could indicate experiences of social isolation or reduced exposure to social stimuli, eventually hindering opportunities to learn appropriate decoding of such stimuli.

Neighborhoods marked by disorder are spatially segregated and over-policed (Morenoff et al., 2001; Kane, 2002; Rengifo and Fowler, 2016; Logan and Oakley, 2017). This composition not only promotes a lack of trust in neighbors but also formal institutions (Steenbeek and Hipp, 2011; Nix et al., 2014; Intravia et al., 2016). Relevantly, those with higher perceived neighborhood disorder and less LPP differentiation were less likely to commit a variety of crimes, whereas those with higher perceptions of neighborhood disorder and high LPP differentiation were more likely to commit a variety of crimes. This finding further supports the interpretation that processing trustworthy and untrustworthy faces similarly may reflect a “healthy” adaptation that mitigates against deviant behavior and contacts with the law. Conversely, greater differentiation in processing of untrustworthy vs. trustworthy faces may reflect an “alertness” adaptation where ambient threat is frequently detected and results in engagement (e.g., fights) with threatening stimuli. It is important to note that while these results, taken together, support our interpretation that less differentiation in LPP amplitude to trustworthy and untrustworthy faces potentially reflects a “healthy” adaptive response to one’s environment, we are not able to determine any causality from our current study design.

Before concluding, the limitations of the present study should be noted. First, while the Todorov stimulus set is the only highly controlled and empirically validated set that manipulates the features most strongly driving trustworthiness perception, it currently contains only White faces. The majority of the present sample was Black. While race had no effect on the congruence between the present sample ratings and the computer-generated face ratings (see footnote 2), previous research suggests that race can affect social judgments (Blair et al., 2004; Stanley et al., 2011; Strachan et al., 2017) and implicit race biases can affect the extent to which an individual trusts others from different racial backgrounds (Stanley et al., 2011). Importantly, these effects related to engagement with or judgment about the behaviors of others that are perceived as untrustworthy, does not mean that there is a race-based difference in one’s ability to know what faces are signaling trustworthiness, which is what was measured in the ratings aspect of the present study. Second, though sufficient based on previous research, the sample size is small. Future research should replicate and extend this work, potentially examining variability in neural and behavioral responses based on specific social relationships or specific contexts. Third, we designed our study to investigate the LPP ERP component, given that it has been previously studied in relation to judgments of facial trustworthiness (Yang et al., 2011; Marzi et al., 2014; Lischke et al., 2017). However, we were not able to investigate how perceptions of neighborhood disorder related to early ERP components and ratings of facial trustworthiness. Future research could investigate how neural activity that reflects early attentional processes (i.e., P1) or encoding process (i.e., N170) of faces might affect trustworthiness processing and relate to individual differences in perceptions of neighborhood disorder. Further, our study design was not longitudinal, thus we were unable to establish causality amongst our variables of interest. Future research should investigate changes in ERP components to facial trustworthiness and perceptions of neighborhood disorder over time, in order to understand causal relationships between brain activity and social stimuli, and differences in social environments.

Overall, the present study provides evidence that perceptions of neighborhood disorder differentially relate to the way facial trustworthiness is processed. Moreover, these differences may be protective against social problems (i.e., criminal behavior). Exploring the association between neighborhood characteristics and variability in individual-level processing is important for refining our understanding of what shapes our beliefs and behaviors in navigating social landscapes.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Yale University Human Investigation Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

S-AC and AB-S designed the experiment. S-AC collected and analyzed the data under the supervision of AB-S. S-AC wrote the article with AB-S providing critical revisions.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.neighborhoodatlas.medicine.wisc.edu/
  2. ^ Separate binary logistic regressions were run with inclusion or exclusion due to quality of EEG data as the dependent variable and age, education level, race, IQ, household income, and PND scores as the independent variables. Participants excluded due to quality of EEG data did not differ significantly from the included participants in age, education level, race, IQ, or PND scores (p > 0.05). Participants that were excluded due to quality of EEG data differed significantly from the included participants on household income, such that included participants were more likely to come from households with lower household incomes Odds Ratio = 0.82, B = −0.20, p = 0.025.
  3. ^ There was no effect of race on mean LPP amplitude in a GLM with face type (neutral, trustworthy, or untrustworthy) as a within-subjects factor and race as a between-subjects factor, F(2, 106) = 1.09, p = 0.340. Moreover, race did not moderate the association between PND and face type LPP, F(2, 102) = 0.44, p = 0.645.

References

Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., and Damasio, A. R. (1998). The human amygdala in social judgment. Nature 393:470. doi: 10.1038/30982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, I., Chapleau, K. M., and Judd, C. M. (2004). The use of Afrocentric features as cues for judgment in the presence of diagnostic information. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 35, 59–68. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., and Hawkley, L. C. (2009). Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends Cognit. Sci. 13, 447–454. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, L. J., Doll, B. B., van’t Wout, M., Frank, M. J., and Sanfey, A. G. (2010). Seeing is believing: trustworthiness as a dynamic belief. Cognit. Psychol. 61, 87–105. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtin, J. J. (2011). PhysBox: The Psychophysiology Toolbox. An open Source Toolbox for Psychophysiological Data Reduction Within EEGLab. Available online at: http://dionysus.psych.wisc.edu/PhysBox.htm (accessed August 19, 2019).

Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dotsch, R., and Todorov, A. (2012). Reverse correlating social face perception. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 3, 562–571. doi: 10.1177/1948550611430272

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V., and Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness decisions: automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 19, 1508–1519. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ewing, L., Caulfield, F., Read, A., and Rhodes, G. (2015). Perceived trustworthiness of faces drives trust behaviour in children. Dev. Sci. 18, 327–334. doi: 10.1111/desc.12218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

FeldmanHall, O., Dunsmoor, J. E., Tompary, A., Hunter, L. E., Todorov, A., and Phelps, E. A. (2018). Stimulus generalization as a mechanism for learning to trust. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115:E1690. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1715227115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foti, D., Hajcak, G., and Dien, J. (2009). Differentiating neural responses to emotional pictures: evidence from temporal-spatial PCA. Psychophysiology 46, 521–530. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00796.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freedman, D., and Woods, G. W. (2013). Neighborhood effects, mental illness and criminal behavior: a review. J. Polit. Law 6, 1–16. doi: 10.5539/jpl.v6n3p1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hajcak, G., Dunning, J. P., and Foti, D. (2009). Motivated and controlled attention to emotion: time-course of the late positive potential. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 505–510. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.11.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hajcak, G., MacNamara, A., and Olvet, D. M. (2010). Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotion regulation: an integrative review. Dev. Neuropsychol. 35, 129–155. doi: 10.1080/87565640903526504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Intravia, J., Stewart, E. A., Warren, P. Y., and Wolff, K. T. (2016). Neighborhood disorder and generalized trust: a multilevel mediation examination of social mechanisms. J. Crim. Just. 46, 148–158. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kane, R. J. (2002). The social ecology of police misconduct. Criminology 40, 867–896. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2002.tb00976.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. (2010). Neighborhood disadvantage and mental health: the role of neighborhood disorder and social relationships. Soc. Sci. Res. 39, 260–271. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2009.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kneebone, E. (2014). The Growth and Spread of Concentrated Poverty, 2000 to 2008–2012. Available online at: https://www.brookings.edu/interactives/the-growth-and-spread-of-concentrated-poverty-2000-to-2008-2012/ (accessed September 28, 2019).

Google Scholar

Lischke, A., Junge, M., Hamm, A., and Weymar, M. (2017). Enhanced processing of untrustworthiness in natural faces with neutral expressions. Emotion 18, 181–189. doi: 10.1037/emo0000318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, J. R., and Oakley, D. (2017). Black lives and policing: the larger context of ghettoization. J. Urban Affairs 39, 1031–1046. doi: 10.1080/07352166.2017.1328977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marschall, M. J., and Stolle, D. (2004). Race and the city: neighborhood context and the development of generalized trust. Political Behav. 26, 125–153. doi: 10.1023/B:POBE.0000035960.73204.64

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marzi, T., Righi, S., Ottonello, S., Cincotta, M., and Viggiano, M. P. (2014). Trust at first sight: evidence from ERPs. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 63–72. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, T., Odgers, C. L., Danese, A., Fisher, H. L., Newbury, J. B., Caspi, A., et al. (2019). Loneliness and neighborhood characteristics: a multi-informant, nationally representative study of young adults. Psychol. Sci. 30, 765–775. doi: 10.1177/0956797619836102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morenoff, J. D., Sampson, R. J., and Raudenbush, S. W. (2001). Neighborhood inequality, collective efficacy, and the spatial dynamics of urban violence. Criminology 39, 517–558. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2001.tb00932.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moulson, M. C., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., and Nelson, C. A. (2009). Early adverse experiences and the neurobiology of facial emotion processing. Dev. Psychol. 45:17. doi: 10.1037/a0014035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nix, J., Wolfe, S. E., Rojek, J., and Kaminski, R. J. (2014). Trust in the police: the influence of procedural justice and perceived collective efficacy. Crime Delinquency 61, 610–640. doi: 10.1177/0011128714530548

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oh, J. H. (2003). Assessing the social bonds of elderly neighbors: the roles of length of residence, crime victimization, and perceived disorder. Sociol. Inq. 73, 490–510. doi: 10.1111/1475-682x.00068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olsson, A., FeldmanHall, O., Haaker, J., and Hensler, T. (2018). Social regulation of survival circuits through learning. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 24, 161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oosterhof, N. N., and Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 11087–11092. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805664105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oosterhof, N. N., and Todorov, A. (2009). Shared perceptual basis of emotional expressions and trustworthiness impressions from faces. Emotion 9, 128. doi: 10.1037/a0014520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rengifo, A. F., and Fowler, K. (2016). Stop, question, and complain: citizen grievances against the NYPD and the opacity of police stops across New York City precincts, 2007-2013. J. Urban Health 93(Suppl. 1), 32–41. doi: 10.1007/s11524-015-0010-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, C. (2011). Collective threat, trust, and the sense of personal control. J. Health Soc. Behav. 52, 287–296. doi: 10.1177/0022146511404558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, C., and Jang, S. J. (2000). Neighborhood disorder, fear, and mistrust: the buffering role of social ties with neighbors. Am. J. Commun. Psychol. 28, 401–420. doi: 10.1023/a:1005137713332

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, C., and Mirowsky, J. (1999). Disorder and decay: the concept and measurement of perceived neighborhood disorder. Urban Affairs Rev. 34, 412–432. doi: 10.1177/10780879922184004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, C., and Mirowsky, J. (2009). Neighborhood disorder, subjective alienation, and distress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 50, 49–64. doi: 10.1177/002214650905000104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, C., Mirowsky, J., and Pribesh, S. (2001). Powerlessness and the amplification of threat: neighborhood disadvantage, disorder, and mistrust. Am. Sociol. Rev. 66, 568–591. doi: 10.2307/3088923

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampson, R. J. (2008). “Collective efficacy theory: lessons learned and directions for future inquiry,” in Taking stock: The Status of Criminological Theory, Vol. 15, eds F. T. Cullen, J. P. Wright, and K. R. Blevins (New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers), 149–167. doi: 10.4324/9781315130620-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampson, R. J., and Graif, C. (2009). Neighborhood social capital as differential social organization: resident and leadership dimensions. Am. Behav. Sci. 52, 1579–1605. doi: 10.1177/0002764209331527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., and Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: a multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science 277, 918–924. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampson, R. J., Sharkey, P., and Raudenbush, S. W. (2008). Durable effects of concentrated disadvantage on verbal ability among African-American children. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 845–852. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710189104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singular Inversions. (2005). Facegen Main Software Development Kit (Version 3.1). Vancouver, BC: Singular Inversions.

Google Scholar

Slepian, M. L., Young, S. G., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2017). An approach-avoidance motivational model of trustworthiness judgments. Motiv. Sci. 3, 91–97. doi: 10.1037/mot0000046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slopen, N., McLaughlin, K. A., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., and Nelson, C. A. (2012). Alterations in neural processing, and psychopathology in children raised in institutions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 1022–1030. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanley, D. A., Sokol-Hessner, P., Banaji, M. R., and Phelps, E. A. (2011). Implicit race attitudes predict trustworthiness judgments and economic trust decisions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 7710. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014345108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenbeek, W., and Hipp, J. R. (2011). A longitudinal test of social disorganization theory: feedback effects among cohesion, social control and disorder. Criminology 49, 833–871. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2011.00241.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strachan, J. W. A., Kirkham, A. J., Manssuer, L. R., Over, H., and Tipper, S. P. (2017). Incidental learning of trust from eye-gaze: effects of race and facial trustworthiness. Vis. Cognit. 25, 802–814. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2017.1338321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, A., Baron, S. G., and Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Evaluating face trustworthiness: a model based approach. Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 119–127. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, A., Dotsch, R., Porter, J. M., Oosterhof, N. N., and Falvello, V. B. (2013). Validation of data-driven computational models of social perception of faces. Emotion 13, 724–738. doi: 10.1037/a0032335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, A., and Duchaine, B. (2008). Reading trustworthiness in faces without recognizing faces. Cognit. Neuropsychol. 25, 395–410. doi: 10.1080/02643290802044996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, A., and Olson, I. R. (2008). Robust learning of affective trait associations with faces when the hippocampus is damaged, but not when the amygdala and temporal pole are damaged. Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 195–203. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, A., and Oosterhof, N. N. (2011). Modeling social perception of faces [social sciences]. IEEE Signal Process. Mag. 28, 117–122. doi: 10.1109/MSP.2010.940006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, A., Pakrashi, M., and Oosterhof, N. N. (2009). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness after minimal time exposure. Soc. Cognit. 27, 813–833. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.813

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). The Wide Range Achievement Test: Manual, 3rd Edn. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range Inc.

Google Scholar

Williams, L. M., Palmer, D., Liddell, B. J., Song, L., and Gordon, E. (2006). The ‘when’and ‘where’of perceiving signals of threat versus non-threat. Neuroimage 31, 458–467. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willis, J., and Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure. Psychol. Sci. 17, 592–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, D., Qi, S., Ding, C., and Song, Y. (2011). An ERP study on the time course of facial trustworthiness appraisal. Neurosci. Lett. 496, 147–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.03.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zachary, R. A. (1986). Shipley Institute of Living Scale: Revised Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Keywords: neighborhood disorder, facial trustworthiness, late positive potential, crime, trust

Citation: Chang SA and Baskin-Sommers A (2020) Living in a Disadvantaged Neighborhood Affects Neural Processing of Facial Trustworthiness. Front. Psychol. 11:409. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00409

Received: 17 December 2019; Accepted: 24 February 2020;
Published: 26 March 2020.

Edited by:

Pekka Santtila, New York University Shanghai, China

Reviewed by:

Kathryn Sharratt, University of Huddersfield, United Kingdom
Chris N. H. Street, University of Huddersfield, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2020 Chang and Baskin-Sommers. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shou-An A. Chang, ariel.chang@yale.edu; Arielle Baskin-Sommers, arielle.baskin-sommers@yale.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.