Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 17 July 2020
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Understanding Socioemotional And Academic Adjustment During Childhood And Adolescence, Volume I View all 22 articles

Emotional Intelligence, Empathy, Self-Esteem, and Life Satisfaction in Spanish Adolescents: Regression vs. QCA Models

  • 1Faculty of Teaching and Educational Sciences, Catholic University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
  • 2Department of Nursing, Faculty of Nursing and Chiropody, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
  • 3Social Psychology Department, Faculty of Psychology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain

Adolescence is a complex period, in which the individual is subject to profound emotional, physical, and psychological changes. Healthy development during adolescence is crucial for future positive development; self-esteem and life satisfaction are fundamental. The importance of sociodemographic variables (sex and age), empathy, and emotional intelligence (EI) on self-esteem and life satisfaction was studied, comparing complementary methodologies, regression models, and fuzzy-set qualitative comparative analysis (fsQCA) models. This is a cross-sectional design in a convenience sample of 991 adolescents (528 females, 53.3%; aged between 12 and 19 years; M = 14.01, SD = 1.40) from Spanish schools. Data were collected using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES), the Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS), the Basic Empathy Scale (BES), and Trait Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS)24. The results of the regression models suggest that cognitive empathy, emotional clarity, and emotional repair are the main predictor variables for self-esteem. Meanwhile, the results of the fsQCA suggest that being older and low levels of cognitive empathy, emotional clarity, and emotional repair predict higher levels of self-esteem. On the other hand, life satisfaction in regression models is significantly predicted by the emotional clarity and emotional repair dimensions of the TMMS24 and self-esteem. Meanwhile, in the prediction of life satisfaction, the results of the fsQCA suggest that the most important interactions were high emotional clarity, emotional repair, and low self-esteem. As research has already shown, promoting empathy and EI leads to higher levels of self-esteem and life satisfaction.

Introduction

Adolescence is a complex period in which the individual is subject to profound emotional, physical, and psychological changes (Fonagy et al., 2004; Bucchianeri et al., 2013; Normandin et al., 2015; Powers and Casey, 2015). It is, therefore, a sensitive period in the development of mental disorders (Ensink et al., 2015; Sharp and Wall, 2017). Healthy development during adolescence is therefore crucial not only in avoiding problems in the adolescent himself but also for his future development (Huebner et al., 2013). Within positive or healthy development, there appears to be some agreement on the importance of both self-esteem and well-being in general (Alfaro et al., 2015).

In this respect, adequate self-esteem is particularly important since self-esteem influences the way of perceiving and evaluating oneself. It is defined as confidence in oneself, in one’s values or abilities (McCrae and Costa, 2012), and is therefore considered a protective factor in adolescence since it contributes to preserving one’s biological, psychological, and social well-being (Alvarez Aguirre et al., 2010). High self-esteem is considered vitally important for people’s quality of life since it affects the way they value themselves and their relationship with others (John et al., 2010). Healthy self-esteem is considered a protective factor in life and is essential in adolescence, as it influences human motivation and is associated with a wide range of desirable outcomes (Pyszczynski et al., 2004): general psychological adjustment, positive emotion, social confidence, and prosocial behavior among others (Brown and Ryan, 2003; Leary and MacDonald, 2003; Pepping et al., 2013).

Life satisfaction can be considered to be the cognitive component of subjective well-being and is defined as a conscious cognitive judgment of life in which individuals compare their life circumstances with a self-imposed standard (Diener, 2012; Diener et al., 2013). Life satisfaction is closely related to positive personal, psychological, and social outcomes (Özer et al., 2016). Adolescents with high life satisfaction have a better quality of life (Pavot and Diener, 2008), better levels of physical and psychological health (Friedman and Ryff, 2012; Uchino et al., 2016), positive lifestyle habits (Kim et al., 2014), academic success, increased self-confidence, self-efficacy, mental health, and more supportive relationships (Suldo and Huebner, 2006). Self-esteem and satisfaction with life are therefore related concepts (Judge and Bono, 2001; Frederick et al., 2016; Wichstrom and von Soest, 2016) that appear to affect the individual’s physical and mental health (Sowislo and Orth, 2013). Individuals who feel good about who they are likely make positive evaluations about their life in overall terms (Thomaes et al., 2017). Both concepts seem to be quite stable over time (Anusic and Schimmack, 2016), so intervention to improve them could ensure a healthier present and future development among both children and adults. Life satisfaction has also been consistently positively associated with self-esteem (Proctor et al., 2009).

According to the literature, both concepts seem to be influenced by emotional aspects (Proctor et al., 2009). Within the emotional variables, emotional intelligence (EI) and empathy are particularly important and influential in many aspects of the life of individuals (Kong et al., 2014; Özer et al., 2016). EI is defined as the ability to recognize, express, understand, and regulate one’s own emotions and those of others to improve personal growth and quality of life in social relationships (Mayer et al., 2001) or as a measure of a person’s needs, self-awareness capacities, social awareness, and social skills (Goleman et al., 2002). Previous studies have shown how EI relates positively to different measures of psychological well-being and negatively to affective disorders such as anxiety and depression (Zeidner et al., 2012). Likewise, various studies suggest that EI is negatively related to depression and maladaptive coping styles and positively related to social relationships between peer competition and adaptive coping styles (Palmer et al., 2002; Petrides et al., 2006; Mavroveli et al., 2007). Meanwhile, empathy is considered as a multidimensional construction that implies affective and cognitive responses to another person (Jolliffe and Farrington, 2006; Van der Graaff et al., 2018). Affective empathy includes the right emotional response, while cognitive empathy refers to understanding the other person’s state of mind. Empathy in adolescents has been associated with self-esteem and positive social behaviors (Findlay et al., 2006). Despite the importance of these emotional aspects in the self-esteem and well-being of individuals, the literature is scarce, particularly relating to adolescence, and including both components in the same study. In summary, previous research points to a relationship of emotional variables: EI and empathy with self-esteem and life satisfaction in adolescence. Moreover, life satisfaction has also been consistently positively associated with self-esteem. These relationships suggest the following hypotheses: H1: Higher empathy and EI will predict higher levels of self-esteem. H2: Higher empathy, EI, and self-esteem will predict higher levels of life satisfaction.

Along with these aspects, the literature suggests that sociodemographic aspects such as age and sex could influence both self-esteem and satisfaction with life. There are studies that indicate differences in levels of empathy according to the sex variable; girls had higher levels in the affective component while there were no differences in the cognitive component according to sex variable (Lafferty, 2004). In addition, other studies (Goldbeck et al., 2007; Salmela-Aro and Tuominen-Soini, 2010; Moksnes and Espnes, 2013) present gender differences for life satisfaction and self-esteem, indicating that males obtain better results for both life satisfaction and self-esteem than females. Moksnes and Espnes (2013) found that self-esteem plays a positive role in the association with adolescent life satisfaction, and this relationship is equally robust for both genders and across ages (Moksnes and Espnes, 2013). The aforementioned shows gender differences for life satisfaction and self-esteem; this relationship is not affected by age, which suggests the following hypotheses: H3: Males obtain better results for both life satisfaction and self-esteem than females. H4: Self-esteem and life satisfaction are not influenced by age.

Despite the importance of sociodemographic and emotional aspects in the health of individuals, understood as a combination of self-esteem and satisfaction with life, we were unable to find any study that considers both emotional and demographic aspects simultaneously. This study focuses not only on sociodemographic variables but also EI and empathy on self-esteem. In addition, it involves all these variables on life satisfaction and incorporates two types of methodology. Most of the studies on adolescents have focused on linear models (Vecchio et al., 2007; Çivitci and Çivitci, 2009; Moksnes and Espnes, 2013; Huang and Su, 2014; Chen et al., 2017), ignoring the interactions and the way in which different paths could lead to the same result, which could be evaluated using qualitative comparative analysis (QCA) models (Blackman et al., 2011). These two methodologies are complementary (Ragin, 2008; Eng and Woodside, 2012). Linear regression models indicate the individual contribution of each variable, while the QCA model is based on the combination of variables, considering equifinality, i.e., the different paths that lead to the same results (Ragin, 2008).

The objective of this study was, therefore, to identify the importance of sociodemographic variables (sex and age), empathy, and EI on the psychological health of adolescents (self-esteem and satisfaction with life), comparing complementary methodologies, regression models, and fuzzy-set qualitative comparative analysis (fsQCA) models.

Methods

Participants

A convenience sample of 991 adolescents (528 females, 53.3%, and 463 males, 46.7%) aged between 12 and 19 years (M = 14.01, SD = 1.40) was recruited from Spanish schools from the Valencian Community. The majority were in the first (28.4%) or second (27.4%) year of Compulsory Secondary Education, 19.9% were in the third year, and 19.9% in the fourth year; 2.7% were in the first year of high school and 1.5% in the second year of high school. This study respected the fundamental principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013), with particular emphasis on the anonymization of the data collected, and confidentiality and non-discrimination of participants. After the schools in the Valencian Community had been selected, they were contacted and the project was outlined to them, and then sessions were organized with the psychologist who wanted to participate. The assessment was performed after receiving authorization from both parents of the underage students and the older students themselves. In order to ensure anonymity, the schools that participated voluntarily were given the total number of questionnaires requested according to the number of students that voluntarily agreed to participate, and sealed boxes where the questionnaire could be deposited thereby guarantee the participating subjects’ anonymity. The evaluation was carried out by the psychologist in the classrooms, collectively and during school hours. The participants completed the self-reported questionnaires, which took around 40 min.

Measures

– Sociodemographic variables of the adolescents and their age, sex, and school year group were registered.

– Self-esteem was measured using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1989). This contains 10 items, rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree). The items are divided into two factors: positive self-esteem and negative self-esteem. Sample items from this scale are “I feel that I have a number of good qualities” (positive) “At times, I think I am no good at all” (negative). Higher scores indicate higher self-esteem. The scale showed adequate psychometric properties in the original version (α = 0.89) and previous studies [α = 0.87, Huang et al. (2019), and α = 0.89, Schwinn et al. (2019)], which is also observed in this study (α = 0.81).

– Life satisfaction was assessed using the Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 1985). It contains five items and a 7-point Likert scale format, from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). A sample item from this scale is “In most ways, my life is close to my ideal.” Higher values indicate greater satisfaction. It had adequate psychometric properties in the original version (α = 0.87) and previous studies [α = 0.71, Bendayan et al. (2013), and α = 0.80, Miller et al. (2019)], which is also observed in this study (α = 0.78).

Empathy was assessed using the Basic Empathy Scale (BES; Jolliffe and Farrington, 2006), which was translated and adapted into Spanish for adolescents (Villadangos et al., 2016). It is composed of 20 items and uses a 5-point Likert scale answer format, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The items are divided into two subcategories: Cognitive Empathy, or the ability to rationally understand the emotions of others, and Emotional Empathy, or the ability to have an emotional response similar to that of others. Sample items from this scale are “I get caught up in other people’s feelings easily” (affective) and “I can often understand how people are feeling even before they tell me” (cognitive). A high score indicates a high level of empathy. The instrument has shown adequate internal consistency in the original version (α = 0.79 cognitive empathy; α = 0.85 affective empathy) and previous studies (α = 0.96 cognitive empathy; α = 0.92 affective empathy) (Villadangos et al., 2016). In the present study, internal consistency was a Cronbach alpha of 0.68 for both dimensions of the scale.

EI was evaluated from the perspective of emotional processing using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS)24. It is a reduced version of the TMMS48 (Fernandez-Berrocal et al., 2004) and assesses each person’s understanding of his or her emotional states (Inglés et al., 2017) using 24 items with a 5-point Likert scale with anchors of 1 (do not agree at all) and 5 (agree completely). They are organized into three factors of eight items each: (i) Attention, or the ability to realize one’s own feelings and those of others; (ii) Clarity, or the ability to perceive emotions as well as the cause of emotions; and (iii) Repair, or the ability to regulate one’s own and others’ emotions. Examples of these items are “I pay a lot of attention to my feelings” (attention), “I am usually very clear about my feelings” (clarity), and “Although I am sometimes sad, I have a mostly optimistic outlook” (repair). A high score indicates a high level of EI. All three scales have shown adequate psychometric properties in the original version (all Cronbach alphas were above 0.85) and previous studies. Specifically, emotional attention presented a Cronbach’s α ≥ 0.91. Emotional clarity showed a Cronbach’s α ≥ 0.84. Finally, for emotional repair, Cronbach’s α ≥ 0.86 was observed (Gomez-Baya et al., 2016). Another study showed satisfactory internal consistency α = 0.85 for emotional attention, α = 0.81 for emotional clarity, and α = 0.82 for emotional repair (Inglés et al., 2017). In this study, reliability was α = 0.85 for attention, α = 0.83 for emotional clarity, and α = 0.78 for emotional repair.

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analysis was carried out using hierarchical regression models and fsQCA models. In the case of hierarchical regression models, three steps were considered for the prediction of self-esteem: sociodemographic variables (sex and age) (step 1), the two dimensions of the BES questionnaire (step 2), and the three dimensions of the TMMS24 (step 3). For the SWLS prediction, and as suggested by the literature, one more step was added to the overall self-esteem using the RSES (step 4). For the calculation of fsQCA prior to the calculation of the necessary and sufficiency analyses, the missing data were eliminated, and the variables were recalibrated with values between 0 (without having the characteristic, completely out of the set, low values) and 1 (having the characteristic, completely in the set, high values). For sex, 0 = man and 1 = woman were assigned.

QCA models identify the percentage of variance explained, or cases where the model is applicable, coverage, as well as indicators of goodness of fit and consistency (Ragin, 2008; Eng and Woodside, 2012). A condition is considered necessary in QCA models when it must always be present for a given result, and its consistency is ≥0.90 (Ragin, 2008). A condition is considered sufficient when the consistency is around or above 0.75 (Eng and Woodside, 2012); it does not always have to be present for a given result and implies a combination of conditions that can lead to a particular result. The values of each variable were subsequently recalibrated with the fsQCA 2.5 software (Claude and Christopher, 2014). To perform this calibration, the three thresholds suggested by the literature (Woodside, 2013) are used: the first (0) considers that observation with this value is totally outside the set (under agreement/value, low level) of the 10% percentile; the second (0.5) considers a midpoint, neither inside nor outside the set (an intermediate level of agreement/value, intermediate level) 50%; and the last value (1) considers that the observation is totally inside the set (high level of agreement/value, high level) 90% percentile (Ragin, 2008; Giménez-Espert and Prado-Gascó, 2018). Necessary and sufficient condition tests were used to evaluate the effect of sociodemographic variables (sex and age), empathy, and EI on the psychological health of individuals (self-esteem and satisfaction with life). The identification of sufficient fsQCA conditions is done through an algorithm that calculates the truth table and then generates three possible solutions: complex, parsimonious, and intermediate (Eng and Woodside, 2012). The intermediate solution is the one recommended in the literature (Ragin, 2008), and it is shown here.

QCA models have advantages. They analyze the logical relationships between associate conditions and an outcome, providing more detailed results (Ragin, 2008; Eng and Woodside, 2012). They do not establish linear relationships. They allow to identify combinations of multiple causes. This means that the conditions that lead adolescents to have a high tolerance for diversity will not necessarily be the same (in the opposite direction) as those that lead to a low tolerance for diversity.

The IBM SPSS Statistics version 24.0 software package (IBM Corp, 2016) was used to calculate the hierarchical regression model (HRM) and calibration values, and the fsQCA 2.5 software package (Claude and Christopher, 2014) was used to calculate fsQCA models.

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Calibration Values

The main descriptors and calibration values for the variables studied in fsQCA are shown in Table 1. The different calibration values were calculated by multiplying the items in each dimension of the instruments in order to maximize the variance (Ragin, 2008).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Main descriptions and calibration values.

Hierarchical Regression Models

A predictive analysis of the effect of both sociodemographic variables, empathy, and EI on self-esteem and sociodemographic variables, empathy, EI, and self-esteem on the life satisfaction of adolescents was then carried out using hierarchical regression according to the objectives of the study. On the effect of sociodemographic variables, empathy, and EI on self-esteem, the criterion variable was the self-esteem, and the predictor variables were sociodemographic variables (sex and age) and the dimensions of the BES and TMMS24. Three differential steps were established: the sociodemographic variables (sex and age) were included in the first step, the two dimensions of the BES questionnaire were included in the second step, and the three dimensions of TMMS24 were included in the last step.

When evaluating the effect of sociodemographic variables, empathy, EI, and self-esteem on the life satisfaction of adolescents, the criterion variable was the life satisfaction of adolescents and the predictor variables were sociodemographic variables (sex and age), dimensions of the BES, TMMS24, and global self-esteem according to RSES. Four differential steps were established. Sociodemographic variables (sex and age) were included in the first step, the two dimensions of the BES questionnaire were included in the second step, the three dimensions of TMMS24 were included in the third step, and global self-esteem was included in the last step.

When evaluating the effect of sociodemographic variables (sex and age), empathy and EI accounted for 25% of self-esteem (R2adjusted = 0.25, p ≤ 0.001), and the effect of sociodemographic variables (sex and age), empathy, EI, and self-esteem explained 36% of life satisfaction (R2adjusted = 0.36, p ≤ 0.001) (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Hierarchical regressions for the dimensions of BES, TMMS24, RSES, and SWLS.

According to the self-esteem that can be found in Table 2, in the first step, the sociodemographic variables significantly increased the variance by 1% (ΔR2 = 0.01, p ≤ 0.01) including the dimensions of the BES scale, accounting for 3% of the variance (ΔR2 = 0.03, p ≤ 0.001), and accounting for 21% of the variance (ΔR2 = 0.21, p ≤ 0.001) with the addition of the TMMS24 variables. In this last step, sex (β = 0.01, p ≤ 0.05) and emotional attention (β = 0.07, p ≤ 0.05) showed statistically significant and positive beta coefficients for self-esteem. On the other hand, cognitive empathy (β = −0.09, p ≤ 0.01), emotional clarity (β = −0.25, p ≤ 0.001), and emotional repair (β = −0.34, p ≤ 0.001) showed statistically significant and negative beta coefficients for self-esteem.

For life satisfaction shown in Table 2, in the first step, sociodemographic variables did not increase variance, including the dimensions of the BES scale, and accounted for 4% of the variance (ΔR2 = 0.04, p ≤ 0.001), including the TMMS24 variables, which explained 19% of the variance (ΔR2 = 0.19 p ≤ 0.001) and explaining 14% of the variance (ΔR2 = 0.14, p ≤ 0.001) with the addition of self-esteem. In this last step, emotional clarity (β = 0.24, p ≤ 0.001) and emotional repair (β = 0.31, p ≤ 0.001) showed statistically significant and positive beta coefficients for life satisfaction. On the other hand, self-esteem (β = -0.43, p ≤ 0.001) showed statistically significant and negative beta coefficients for life satisfaction among adolescents.

Fuzzy-Set Qualitative Comparative Analysis

Necessary Conditions

Table 3 shows the test of necessary conditions, none of the associate conditions can be considered necessary for high or low levels of self-esteem and life satisfaction since the consistency is below 0.90 (Ragin, 2008).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Necessity analysis for RSES and SWLS.

Sufficient Conditions

Table 4 shows the models resulting from the sufficiency analyses, with consistency around or above 0.75 being a sufficient condition (Eng and Woodside, 2012).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Summary of the three main sufficient conditions for the intermediate solution for RSES and SWLS.

In the prediction of self-esteem of the adolescents, nine paths were observed that explained 41% of the cases with high levels of self-esteem (Overall Consistency = 0.73; Overall Coverage = 0.41). The most relevant path or combination for predicting high levels of self-esteem (explaining 21% of cases) was the result of the interaction of being older, with high attention to emotions, low cognitive empathy, and low emotional clarity and emotional repair (Raw Coverage = 0.27; Consistency = 0.81). However, in the prediction of low levels of self-esteem in the adolescents, 17 paths were observed that explained 68% of the cases (Overall Consistency = 0.83; Overall Coverage = 0.68). Meanwhile, the most relevant path or combination for predicting low levels of self-esteem (explaining 29% of cases) was the result of the interaction of high emotional clarity, being younger, and low emotional attention (Raw Coverage = 0.29; Consistency = 0.89).

Furthermore, in the prediction of high levels of life satisfaction for the adolescents, 16 paths were examined that explained 70% of the cases with high levels of life satisfaction (Overall Consistency = 0.77; Overall Coverage = 0.70). The most relevant path or combination for predicting high levels of life satisfaction (accounting for 48% of cases) was the result of the interaction of high emotional clarity and emotional repair and low self-esteem (Raw Coverage = 0.48; Consistency = 0.84). Finally, in the prediction of low levels of life satisfaction in the adolescents, 11 paths were observed that explained 66% of the cases (Overall Consistency = 0.86; Overall Coverage = 0.66). Meanwhile, the most relevant path or combination for predicting low levels of life satisfaction (explaining 52% of cases) was the result of the interaction of high emotional clarity and self-esteem (Raw Coverage = 0.52; Consistency = 0.89).

Discussion

This study extends the research on self-esteem and life satisfaction in adolescence by exploring the importance of sociodemographic variables, empathy, and EI on self-esteem, as well as sociodemographic variables, empathy, EI, and self-esteem on life satisfaction. It compares complementary methodologies, regression models, and fsQCA models.

In general, based on the obtained results, hypothesis one (higher empathy and EI will predict higher levels of self- esteem) can be accepted in the regression model and partially in the fsQCA model. The regression models suggest that sex, cognitive empathy on the BES, and the dimensions of the TMMS24 (emotional attention, emotional clarity, and emotional repair) are significant predictors of self-esteem, as reported in previous studies supporting the influence of emotional skills (empathy and EI), where it was found to be significantly associated with prosocial behaviors, and more positive beliefs about the self (Oberle et al., 2010). The most predictive variable was EI, accounting for 21% of variance explained. In line with the literature, EI has been associated with positive self-esteem and academic, social, and psychological outcomes in adolescence (Di Fabio and Saklofske, 2014; Perera and DiGiacomo, 2015). The most important interactions in predicting self-esteem in sufficiency analyses in the fsQCA, explaining 21% of the cases, were being older, high emotional attention, low cognitive empathy, and low emotional clarity and emotional repair. These results may show that in later adolescence, the cognitive component of empathy, as well as emotional clarity and regulation, plays a less relevant role. These findings can be considered the first step because our study and others show the weak correlations that are usually reported between demographic variables (sex and age), self-esteem, and subjective well-being (Proctor et al., 2009). It highlights the need for future theories and research.

Regarding hypothesis two, higher empathy, EI, and self- esteem will predict higher levels of life satisfaction and can be accepted in the regression model and partially in the fsQCA model. Life satisfaction in the regression model is significantly predicted by the emotional clarity and emotional repair dimensions of the TMMS24 and self-esteem, as shown in the literature, and various environmental variables, sociodemographic variables, and intrapersonal variables such as self-esteem have been recognized as being associated with adolescents’ life satisfaction (Proctor et al., 2009). The most predictable variable again is EI, which explained 19% of the variance. EI influences adaptation to deal with social problems and interpersonal conflicts. Reducing negative emotions, increasing positive ones, and improving emotional regulation promote positive relationships with others, providing positive self-esteem and satisfaction with life (Zeidner and Olnick-Shemesh, 2010; Friedman and Kern, 2014). Meanwhile, in the prediction of life satisfaction, the most important interactions were high emotional clarity and emotional repair and low self-esteem in the fsQCA. These results show the importance of EI, the ability to understand and regulate emotions, and its impact on life satisfaction (Perera and DiGiacomo, 2015; Di Fabio and Kenny, 2016).

No conditions necessary for predicting self-esteem and life satisfaction were observed in the fsQCA. With regard to the third and fourth hypotheses, sex and age on self-esteem and life satisfaction, our results are less conclusive. In regression models, sociodemographic variables increase the variance 1% or nothing. In fsQCA models, age is a condition in predicting high levels of self-esteem. As in other studies, sociodemographic variables are weak predictors (Huebner et al., 2004, 2005), environmental variables are moderate predictors, and intrapersonal variables such as self-esteem are strong predictors (Boden et al., 2008; Deng et al., 2015, 2016; Chen et al., 2017). In general, the results of both methodologies (regression and fsQCA models) indicate emotional variables, empathy, and EI are strong predictors on self-esteem, and all these variables are strong predictors on life satisfaction. When comparing both methodologies, fsQCA models seem to have a higher predictive value than the HRM, and there are variables such as age that appear in the fsQCA and not in the HRM. This is important from the perspective of intervention since some conditions are susceptible to intervention, such as the improvement of emotional skills, self-esteem, and life satisfaction. Adequate self-esteem is associated with life satisfaction (Brown and Ryan, 2003; Leary and MacDonald, 2003; Pepping et al., 2013). EI and empathy encourage prosocial behaviors (Inglés et al., 2014) and are associated with better social relationships (Batanova and Loukas, 2011; Gázquez et al., 2015; Van der Graaff et al., 2018), better mental health (Gomez-Baya et al., 2016), and life satisfaction (Zeidner et al., 2012). In short, promoting empathy and EI, especially focusing on emotional clarity and emotional regulation, in adolescents leads to higher levels of self-esteem and life satisfaction.

In conclusion, this study was an attempt to explore the relationships between empathy, EI, self-esteem, and life satisfaction in a sample of Valencian adolescents. One of the strengths of this study was the comparison of two types of complementary methodologies in predicting self-esteem and life satisfaction of adolescents; fsQCA models involve a greater number of factors than regression models. However, it is not exempt from limitations that will be considered in future research. The type of sampling, not probabilistic, and the inclusion of subjects from only one geographical area make it difficult to generalize the results. Another limitation of this study is the use of self-report for data collection as it may introduce social desirability biases (Dalton and Ortegren, 2011). Despite the limitations of the study, our results show how EI in general and especially emotional clarity and emotional repair and cognitive empathy predict self-esteem in adolescents. In other words, the ability to perceive emotions as well as the cause of emotions and the ability to regulate one’s own and others’ emotions, as well as the ability to understand the other person’s state of mind, predict adolescent self-esteem. In addition, self-esteem and EI, especially emotional clarity and emotional repair, are closely related to positive personal, psychological, and social outcomes and life satisfaction.

This study has implications for theoretical and empirical research of self-esteem related to life satisfaction by identifying conditions, variables, or combinations, such as emotional skills, that appear to have a positive impact on them. The high levels of self-esteem and life satisfaction are psychological resources that promote healthy adaptation in adolescence. Therefore, these results constitute the first step for intervention programs in adolescents’ emotional skills, which in turn will have a direct and positive impact on their self-esteem and life satisfaction, as well as on positive functioning in adolescence.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comité Ético de Investigación en Humanos Universitat de València. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

MG, DN-M, MG-E, and VP-G made substantial contribution to the concept or design of the work or acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data; drafted the article or revised it critically for important intellectual content; and participated sufficiently in the work to take public responsibility for appropriate portions of the content. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all adolescents who participated in this study.

References

Alfaro, J., Casas, F., and López, V. (2015). Bienestar en la infancia y adolescencia. [Well- being in childhood and adolescence]. Psicoperspectivas 14, 1–5.

Google Scholar

Alvarez Aguirre, A., Alonso Castillo, M. M., and Guidorizzi Zanetti, A. C. (2010). Consumo de alcohol y autoestima en adolescentes [Alcohol use and self-esteem in adolescents]. Rev. Latino-Am. Enfermagem 18, 634–640. doi: 10.1590/S0104-11692010000700021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anusic, I., and Schimmack, U. (2016). Stability and change of personality traits, self-esteem, and well-being: introducing the meta-analytic stability and change model of retest correlations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 110, 766–781. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batanova, M. D., and Loukas, A. (2011). Social anxiety and aggression in early adolescents: examining the moderating roles of empathic concern and perspective taking. J. Youth Adolesc. 40, 1534–1543. doi: 10.1007/s10964-011-9634-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bendayan, R., Blanca, M. J., Fernandez-Baena, F. J., Escobar, M., and Trianes, M. V. (2013). New empirical evidence on the validity of the satisfaction with life scale in early adolescents. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 29, 36–43. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a00011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blackman, T., Wistow, J., and Byrne, D. (2011). A qualitative comparative analysis of factors associated with trends in narrowing health inequalities in England. Soc. Sci. Med. 72, 1965–1974. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boden, J. M., Fergusson, D. M., and Horwood, L. J. (2008). Does adolescent self-esteem predict later life outcomes? A test of the causal role of self-esteem. Dev. Psychopathol. 20, 319–339. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological wellbeing. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bucchianeri, M. M., Arikian, A. J., Hannan, P. J., Eisenberg, M. E., and Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2013). Body dissatisfaction from adolescence to young adulthood: findings from a 10-year longitudinal study. Body Image 10, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, W., Zhang, D., Pan, Y., Hu, T., Liu, G., and Luo, S. (2017). Perceived social support and self-esteem as mediators of the relationship between parental attachment and life satisfaction among Chinese adolescents. Pers. Individ. Differ. 108, 98–102. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.12.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Çivitci, N., and Çivitci, A. (2009). Self-esteem as mediator and moderator of the relationship between loneliness and life satisfaction in adolescents. Pers. Individ. Differ. 47, 954–958. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Claude, R., and Christopher, R. (2014). acq [Computer Programme], Version 2.1.12. Houston, TX: University of Houston-Downtown.

Google Scholar

Dalton, D., and Ortegren, M. (2011). Gender differences in ethics research: the importance of controlling for the social desirability response bias. J. Bus. Ethics 103, 73–93. doi: 10.1007/s10551-011-0843-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, L., Ma, B., and Fang, X. (2016). Peer attachment and life satisfaction of junior middle school students: the mediating effect of collective self-esteem. Stud. Psychol. Behav. 14, 145–151.

Google Scholar

Deng, L., Ma, B., and Wu, Y. (2015). Attachment and subjective well-being of junior middle school students: the mediating role of self esteem. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 31, 230–238.

Google Scholar

Di Fabio, A., and Kenny, M. E. (2016). Promoting well-being: the contribution of emotional intelligence. Front. Psychol. 7:1182. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Fabio, A., and Saklofske, D. H. (2014). Promoting individual resources: the challenge of trait emotional intelligence. Pers. Individ. Differ. 65, 19–23. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E. (2012). New findings and future directions for subjective well-being research. Am. Psychol. 67, 590–597. doi: 10.1037/a0029541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Emmons, R., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Inglehart, R., and Tay, L. (2013). Theory and validity of life satisfaction scales. Soc. Indic. Res. 112, 497–527. doi: 10.1007/s11205-012-0076-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eng, S., and Woodside, A. G. (2012). Configural analysis of the drinkingman: fuzzy-set qualitative comparative analyses. Addict. Behav. 37, 541–543. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.11.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ensink, K., Biberdzic, M., Normandin, L., and Clarkin, J. (2015). A developmental psychopathology and neurobiological model of borderline personality disorder in adolescence. J. Infant. Child. Adolesc. Psychother. 14, 46–69, doi: 10.1080/15289168.2015.1007715

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez-Berrocal, P., Extremera, N., and Ramos, N. (2004). Validity and reliability of the Spanish modified version of the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. Psychol. Rep. 94, 751–755. doi: 10.2466/pr0.94.3.751-755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Findlay, L., Girardi, A., and Coplan, R. J. (2006). Links between empathy, social behavior, and social understanding in early childhood. Early Child. Res. Q. 21, 347–359. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiss, P. C. (2011). Building better causal theories: a fuzzy set approach to typologies in organization research. Acad. Manag. Ann. 54, 393–420. doi: 10.5465/amj.2011.60263120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., and Jurist, E. L. (eds) (2004). Affect Regulation, Mentalization and the Development of the Self. London: Karnac Books.

Google Scholar

Frederick, D. A., Sandhu, G., Morse, P. J., and Swami, V. (2016). Correlates of appearance and weight satisfaction in a U.S. national sample: personality, attachment style, television viewing, self-esteem, and life satisfaction. Body Image 17, 191–203. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, E. M., and Ryff, C. D. (2012). Living well with medical comorbidities: a biopsychosocial perspective. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 67, 535–544. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, H. S., and Kern, M. L. (2014). Personality, well-being, and health. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 719–742. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gázquez, J., Sainz, J., Peìrez-Fuentes, M., Molero, M., and Soler, F. (2015). Interpersonal value profiles and analysis of adolescent academic performance and social thinking. Front. Psychol. 6:575. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giménez-Espert, M. C., and Prado-Gascó, V. J. (2018). The role of empathy and emotional intelligence in nurses’ communication attitudes using regression models and fuzzy-set qualitative comparative analysis models (fsQCA). J. Clin. Nurs. 27, 2661–2672. doi: 10.1111/jocn.1432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldbeck, L., Schmitz, T. G., Besier, T., Herschbach, P., and Henrich, G. (2007). Life satisfaction decreases during adolescence. Qual. Life Res. 16, 969–979. doi: 10.1007/s11136-007-9205-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goleman, D., Boyatzis, R. E., and McKee, A. (2002). Leadership and Emotional Intelligence. Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.

Google Scholar

Gomez-Baya, D., Mendoza, R., and Paino, S. (2016). Perceived emotional intelligence as a predictor of depressive symptoms after a one year follow-up during adolescence. Int. J. Emot. Educ. 8, 35–47. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, C. Y., Roberts, Y. H., Costeines, J., and Kaufman, J. S. (2019). Longitudinal trajectories of parenting stress among ethnic minority adolescent mothers. J. Child Fam. Stud. 28, 1368–1378. doi: 10.1007/s10826-019-01356-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, H., and Su, Y. (2014). Peer acceptance among Chinese adolescents: the role of emotional empathy, cognitive empathy and gender. Int. J. Psychol. 49, 420–424. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huebner, E. S., Hills, K. J., and Jiang, X. (2013). “Assessment and promotion of life satisfaction in youth,” in Research, Applications, and Interventions for Children and Adolescents, eds C. Proctor and P. Linley (Dordrecht: Springer), 23–42. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-6398-2_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huebner, E. S., Suldo, S., Valois, R. F., Drane, J. W., and Zullig, K. (2004). Brief multidimensional students’ life satisfaction scale: sex, race, and grade effects for a high school sample. Psychol. Rep. 94, 351–356. doi: 10.2466/pr0.94.1.351-356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huebner, E. S., Valois, R. F., Paxton, R. J., and Drane, J. W. (2005). Middle school students’ perceptions of quality of life. J. Happiness Stud. 6, 15–24. doi: 10.1007/s10902-004-1170-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corp (2016). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24.0. Released Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Google Scholar

Inglés, C. J., Martínez-Monteagudo, M. C., Pérez Fuentes, M. C., García-Fernández, J. M., Mar Molero, M. D., Suriá-Martinez, R., et al. (2017). Emotional intelligence profiles and learning strategies in secondary school students. Educ. Psychol. 37, 237–248. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2016.1156057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inglés, C. J., Torregrosa, M. S., García-Fernández, J. M., Martínez-Monteagudo, M. C., Estévez, E., Delgado, B., et al. (2014). Conducta agresiva e inteligencia emocional en la adolescencia [Aggressive behavior and emotional intelligence in adolescence]. Eur. J. Educ. Psychol. 7, 29–41.

Google Scholar

John, O. P., Robins, R. W., and Pervin, L. A. (eds) (2010). Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Jolliffe, D., and Farrington, D. P. (2006). Development and validation of the Basic Empathy Scale. J. Adolesc. 29, 589–611. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Judge, T. A., and Bono, J. E. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluations traits – Selfesteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability – With job satisfaction and job performance: a meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 86, 80–92. doi: 10.1037//0021-9010.86.6.1270

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, E. S., Smith, J., and Kubzansky, L. D. (2014). Prospective study of the association between dispositional optimism and incident heart failure. Circulation 7, 394–400. doi: 10.1161/CIRCHEARTFAILURE.113.000644

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, F., Wang, X., and Zhao, J. (2014). Dispositional Mindfulness and Life Satisfaction: the Role of Core Self-Evaluations. Pers. Individ. Differ. 56, 165–169. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lafferty, J. (2004). The relationships between gender, empathy, and aggressive behaviours among early adolescents. Dissert. Abst. Int. 64:6377B.

Google Scholar

Leary, M. R., and MacDonald, G. (2003). “Individual differences in self-esteem: a review and theoretical integration,” in Handbook of Self and Identity, eds M. R. Leary and J. P. Tangney (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 401–418.

Google Scholar

Mavroveli, S., Petrides, K. V., Rieffe, C., and Bakker, F. (2007). Trait emotional intelligence, psychological well-being and peer-rated social competence in adolescence. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 25, 263–275. doi: 10.1348/026151006X118577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D. R., and Sitarenios, G. (2001). Emotional intelligence as a standard intelligence. Emotion 1, 232–242. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.1.3.232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R., and Costa, P. (2012). Personality in Adulthood, Second Edition: A Five-Factor Theory Perspective. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Miller, B. K., Zivnuska, S., and Kacmar, K. M. (2019). Self-perception and life satisfaction. Pers. Individ. Differ. 139, 321–325. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moksnes, U. K., and Espnes, G. A. (2013). Self-esteem and life satisfaction in adolescents—gender and age as potential moderators. Qua Life Res. 22, 2921–2928. doi: 10.1007/s11136-013-0427-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Normandin, L., Ensink, K., and Kernberg, O. F. (2015). Transference-focused psychotherapy for borderline adolescents: a neurobiologically informed psychodynamic psychotherapy. J. Infant. Child Adolesc. Psychother. 14, 98–110. doi: 10.1080/15289168.2015.1006008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oberle, E., Schonert-Reichl, K. A., and Thomson, K. C. (2010). Understanding the link between social and emotional well-being and peer relations in early adolescence: Gender-specific predictors of peer acceptance. J. Youth Adolesc. 39, 1330–1342. doi: 10.1007/s10964-009-9486-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Özer, E., Hamarta, E., and Deniz, M. E. (2016). Emotional intelligence, core-self evaluation, and life satisfaction. Psychology 7, 145–153. doi: 10.4236/psych.2016.72017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmer, B., Donaldson, C., and Stough, C. (2002). Emotional intelligence and life satisfaction. Pers. Individ. Differ. 33, 1091–1100. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00215-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavot, W., and Diener, E. (2008). The satisfaction with life scale and the emerging construct of life satisfaction. J. Posit. Psychol. 3, 137–152. doi: 10.1080/17439760701756946

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pepping, C. A., O’Donovan, A., and Davis, P. J. (2013). The positive effects of mindfulness on self-esteem. J. Posit. Psychol. 8, 376–386. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2013.807353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perera, H. N., and DiGiacomo, M. (2015). The role of trait emotional intelligence inacademic performance during the university transition: an integrative model ofmediation via social support, coping, and adjustment. Personal. Individ. Differ. 83, 208–213. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V., Sangareau, Y., Furnham, A., and Frederickson, N. (2006). Trait emotional intelligence and children’s peer relations at school. Soc. Dev. 15, 537–547. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00355.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, A., and Casey, B. J. (2015). The adolescent brain and the emergence and peak of psychopathology. J. Infant. Child Adolesc. Psychother. 14, 3–15. doi: 10.1080/15289168.2015.1004889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proctor, C. L., Linley, P. A., and Maltby, J. (2009). Youth life satisfaction: a review of the literature. J. Happiness Stud. 10, 583–630. doi: 10.1007/s10902-008-9110-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Arndt, J., and Schimel, J. (2004). Why do people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical review. Psychol. Bull. 130, 435–468. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ragin, C. C. (2008). Redesigning Social Inquiry: Fuzzy Sets and Beyond. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Online Library.

Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. (1989). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Salmela-Aro, K., and Tuominen-Soini, H. (2010). Adolescents’ life satisfaction during the transition to post-comprehensive education: antecedents and consequences. J. Happiness Stud. 11, 683–701. doi: 10.1007/s10902-009-9156-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwinn, T. M., Schinke, S. P., Keller, B., and Hopkins, J. (2019). Two-and three-year follow-up from a gender-specific, web-based drug abuse prevention program for adolescent girls. Addict. Behav. 93, 86–92. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharp, C., and Wall, K. (2017). Personality pathology grows up: adolescence as a sensitive period. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 21, 111–116. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowislo, J. F., and Orth, U. (2013). Does low self-esteem predict depression and anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol. Bull. 139, 213–240. doi: 10.1037/a0028931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suldo, S., and Huebner, S. (2006). Is Extremely High Life Satisfaction During Adolescence Advantageous? Soc. Indic. Res. 78, 179–203. doi: 10.1007/s11205-005-8208-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomaes, S., Sedikides, C., van den Bos, N., Hutteman, R., and Reijntjes, A. (2017). Happy to be “me?” Authenticity, psychological need satisfaction, and subjective well-being in adolescence. Child Dev. 88, 1045–1056. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uchino, B. N., Bowen, K., Kent De Gray, R. G., Smith, T. W., Baucom, B. R., Light, K. A., et al. (2016). Loving-kindness meditation improves relationship negativity and psychological well-being: a pilot study. Psychology 7, 6–11. doi: 10.4236/psych.2016.71002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Graaff, J., Carlo, G., Crocetti, E., Koot, H. M., and Branje, S. (2018). Prosocial behavior in adolescence: gender differences in development and links with empathy. J. Youth Adolesc. 47, 1086–1099. doi: 10.1007/s10964-017-0786-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecchio, G. M., Gerbino, M., Pastorelli, C., Del Bove, G., and Caprara, G. V. (2007). Multi-faceted self-efficacy beliefs as predictors of life satisfaction in late adolescence. Pers. Individ. Differ. 43, 1807–1818. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.05.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villadangos, M., Errasti, J., Amigo, I., Jolliffe, D., and Garcìa-Cueto, E. (2016). Characteristics of empthy in young people measured by the Spanish validation of the Basic Empathy Scale. Psicothema 28, 323–329. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2016.6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wichstrom, L., and von Soest, T. (2016). Reciprocal relations between body satisfaction and self-esteem: a large 13-year prospective study of adolescents. J. Adolesc. 47, 16–27. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodside, A. G. (2013). Moving beyond multiple regression analysis to algorithms: calling for adoption of a paradigm shift from symmetric to asymmetric thinking in data analysis and crafting theory. J. Bus. Res. 66, 463–472. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2012.12.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Medical Association (2013). Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. 64th WMA General Assembly October 2013, (Fortaleza: Brazil).

Google Scholar

Zeidner, M., Matthews, G., and Roberts, R. D. (2012). The emotional intelligence, health, and well-being nexus: what have we learned and what have we missed? Appl. Psychol. 4, 1–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2011.01062.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeidner, M., and Olnick-Shemesh, D. (2010). Emotional intelligence and subjective well-being revisited. Pers. Individ. Differ. 48, 431–435. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adolescent, emotional intelligence, empathy, life satisfaction, qualitative comparative analysis, regression models, self-esteem

Citation: Guasp Coll M, Navarro-Mateu D, Giménez-Espert MDC and Prado-Gascó VJ (2020) Emotional Intelligence, Empathy, Self-Esteem, and Life Satisfaction in Spanish Adolescents: Regression vs. QCA Models. Front. Psychol. 11:1629. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01629

Received: 13 May 2020; Accepted: 16 June 2020;
Published: 17 July 2020.

Edited by:

Ricardo Sanmartín, University of Alicante, Spain

Reviewed by:

Sonia Brito- Costa, Instituto Politécnico de Coimbra, Portugal
Diego Gomez-Baya, University of Huelva, Spain

Copyright © 2020 Guasp Coll, Navarro-Mateu, Giménez-Espert and Prado-Gascó. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: María Del Carmen Giménez-Espert, maria.c.gimenez@uv.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.