Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 17 October 2012
Sec. Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Forethought as an evolutionary doorway to emotions and consciousness View all 27 articles

Brain oscillations and predictive processing

  • Institute of Neurobiology, Bulgarian Academy of Sciences, Sofia, Bulgaria

A commentary on

Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science
by Clark, A. (in press). Behav. Brain Sci.

The hierarchical generative model presented in the target article is based on theoretical and computational grounds. However, providing neurophysiologic evidence for the presence of distinct neural mechanisms mediating top-down predictions and bottom-up transmission of prediction error is critical for model validity.

In human cognitive electrophysiology, several event-related potentials (ERPs) are related to the mechanisms of comparing incoming with expected events. These are the mismatch negativity (MMN), error-related negativity (ERN), and feedback-related negativity (FRN) operating basically in the sensory, action/performance, and cognitive domains, respectively. These ERP components are all correlated with generating and transmitting mismatch information (Falkenstein et al., 1991; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2004; Näätänen et al., 2007) and have been previously targeted as markers of bottom-up prediction error (Garrido et al., 2009; Cavanagh et al., 2010; Friston, 2012). There are, however, several issues that may question the suitability of these components for testing the predictive coding model.

The latencies of mismatch ERP components are relatively long (150–250 ms for MMN, 200–300 ms for FRN, and around 100 ms for ERN) under the assumption that they reflect a feed-forward “error” input from lower processing levels to the cortex. More critically, there is experimental evidence that much earlier ERP components are sensitive to deviance. For example, brainstem and middle-latency components within 10–80 ms (Slabu et al., 2010, 2012), the P1 component within 10–100 ms (Bendixen et al., 2009), and early gamma (>30 Hz) oscillatory responses at around 50 ms of auditory ERPs (Schadow et al., 2009) discriminate deviant from standard stimuli. Further, the sources of auditory MMN have been localized in the bilateral temporal and in the prefrontal cortices (Giard et al., 1990; Rinne et al., 2000), the latter supposed to mediate a switch/re-allocation of attention focus (Escera et al., 1998, 2003). Likewise, the sources of ERN and FRN are localized in the medial frontal and prefrontal brain regions (Ullsperger and von Cramon, 2001; Holroyd et al., 2004; Cohen et al., 2011). These data show that the “error” negativities of human ERPs may not represent the virtual error information transmitted to the cortex from lower processing levels. Rather, they may reflect a follow-up utilization of the mismatch information that would be incorporated to update and optimize top-down representations (via attention, memory, and emotional activations). Thus, the feed-forward “error” signal presumed by the predictive coding theory should be sought in much earlier time windows after stimulus and even before stimulus (Romei et al., 2010). Referring to the classical “error” or “prediction-violation” ERP components needs to be conceptually clarified.

In search for error prediction ERP signatures, it is important to recognize that ERP components have a complex heterogeneous structure composed of frequency-specific oscillations (Kolev et al., 1997; Yordanova et al., 2000). The ERN, in particular, comprises at least two oscillatory sub-components that carry error signals from co-existent processing systems (theta – 4–7 Hz and delta – <4 Hz) operating in parallel at the level of movement and behavioral control (Yordanova et al., 2004). It is remarkable that the oscillatory theta component has been detected not only for ERN (Luu and Tucker, 2001; Yordanova et al., 2004; Cavanagh et al., 2009, 2010), but it has also been established as a primary constituent of MMN (Fuentemilla et al., 2008; Hsiao et al., 2009; Ko et al., 2012) and FRN (Cohen et al., 2007, 2011; Marco-Pallares et al., 2008; Christie and Tata, 2009). Thus, the oscillatory theta activity may be a consistent neurophysiologic marker of the utilization of the mismatch (error) information by the executive-control systems in the cortex.

In contrast, there is evidence that increased fast-frequency (beta/gamma – 15–80 Hz) oscillations may signal match detection. Cohen et al. (2007) and Marco-Pallares et al. (2008) demonstrated that positive feedback (prediction confirmation) was associated with increased beta/gamma activity, in contrast to negative feedback characterized by enhanced theta oscillations. Schadow et al. (2009) showed that early phase-locked gamma-band responses (20–80 Hz) were significantly increased for predictable regular auditory stimuli as compared to deviant stimuli, as found for fast-frequency responses in the brainstem (Slabu et al., 2012). This is in line with a model according to which matching between incoming stimulus and a memory template results in enhanced gamma-band activity (Herrmann et al., 2004). Thus, time-frequency decomposition of brain responses may be critical for identifying error signals in functional systems operating in parallel at different processing levels.

In the context of the generative predictive model, a major focus of exploration needs to be placed on error processing in different brain states, in which top-down executive-control mechanisms are reduced, totally inhibited, or qualitatively altered. Such states are best represented by different stages of sleep, anesthesia, coma, and brain diseases. For example, neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that subcortical regions and extrastriate visual cortices are more active during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep compared with wake and non-REM sleep, whereas executive-control (dorsolateral prefrontal and parietal) and primary visual cortices are suppressed in REM sleep compared with wake; In non-REM sleep, the activity of all brain areas relevant for information processing during wake is mostly suppressed (Maquet et al., 1996; Braun et al., 1997, 1998; Nofzinger et al., 1997). Interestingly, although inconsistent and contradictory, there is some evidence that in REM sleep, an analog of the MMN emerges despite the gross suppression of both executive top-down processing and external input transmission (Atienza et al., 2000; Ibáñez et al., 2009). No MMN has been reliably recorded during non-REM sleep and slow wave sleep (Ibáñez et al., 2009; Sculthorpe et al., 2009). It remains poorly understood if and how brain states differing dramatically in top-down and bottom-up mechanisms, neurochemistry, synaptic connectivity, and neuroelectric signaling such as sleep stages (Hobson et al., 2000; Hobson and Pace-Schott, 2002; Nir and Tononi, 2010) affect deviance detection, if inherent (free of executive-control prediction) error signals can be generated, and the extent to which the proposed dynamic Bayesian predictions are moderated by consciousness. Probing generative prediction theory would certainly benefit from referring to such models.

References

Atienza, M., Cantero, J. L., and Gómez, C. M. (2000). Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness and REM sleep. Psychophysiology 37, 485–493.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bendixen, A., Schröger, E., and Winkler, I. (2009). I heard that coming: event-related potential evidence for stimulus-driven prediction in the auditory system. J. Neurosci. 29, 8447–8451.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesensten, N. J., Gwadry, F., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., et al. (1998). Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid eye movement sleep. Science 279, 91–95.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesenten, N. J., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., et al. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle. An H2(15)O PET study. Brain 120, 1173–1197.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cavanagh, J. F., Cohen, M. X., and Allen, J. J. (2009). Prelude to and resolution of an error: EEG phase synchrony reveals cognitive control dynamics during action monitoring. J. Neurosci. 29, 98–105.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cavanagh, J. F., Frank, M. J., Klein, T. J., and Allen, J. J. (2010). Frontal theta links prediction errors to behavioral adaptation in reinforcement learning. Neuroimage 49, 3198–3209.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Christie, G. J., and Tata, M. S. (2009). Right frontal cortex generates reward-related theta-band oscillatory activity. Neuroimage 48, 415–422.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, M. X., Elger, C. E., and Ranganath, C. (2007). Reward expectation modulates feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra. Neuroimage 35, 968–978.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, M. X., Wilmes, K., and Vijver, I. V. (2011). Cortical electrophysiological network dynamics of feedback learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 15, 558–566.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Escera, C., Alho, K., Winkler, I., and Näätänen, R. (1998). Neural mechanisms of involuntary attention to acoustic novelty and change. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 590–604.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Escera, C., Yago, E., Corral, M. J., Corbera, S., and Nuñez, M. I. (2003). Attention capture by auditory significant stimuli: semantic analysis follows attention switching. Eur. J. Neurosci. 8, 2408–2412.

CrossRef Full Text

Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., and Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 78, 447–455.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Friston, K. (2012). Prediction, perception and agency. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 83, 248–252.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fuentemilla, L., Marco-Pallarés, J., Münte, T. F., and Grau, C. (2008). Theta EEG oscillatory activity and auditory change detection. Brain Res. 1220, 93–101.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Garrido, M. I., Kilner, J. M., Kiebel, S. J., and Friston, K. J. (2009). Dynamic causal modeling of the response to frequency deviants. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 2620–2631.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Giard, M. H., Perrin, F., Pernier, J., and Bouchet, P. (1990). Brain generators implicated in the processing of auditory stimulus deviance: a topographic event-related potential study. Psychophysiology 27, 627–640.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Herrmann, C. S., Munk, M. H., and Engel, A. K. (2004). Cognitive functions of gamma-band activity: memory match and utilization. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 8, 347–355.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hobson, J. A., and Pace-Schott, E. F. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: neuronal systems, consciousness and learning. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 679–693.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., and Stickgold, R. (2000). Dreaming and the brain: toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 793–842; discussion 904–1121.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Holroyd, C. B., Nieuwenhuis, S., Yeung, N., Nystrom, L., Mars, R. B., Coles, M. G., et al. (2004). Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex shows fMRI response to internal and external error signals. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 497–498.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hsiao, F. J., Wu, Z. A., Ho, L. T., and Lin, Y. Y. (2009). Theta oscillation during auditory change detection: an MEG study. Biol. Psychol. 81, 58–66.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ibáñez, A. M., Martín, R. S., Hurtado, E., and López, V. (2009). ERPs studies of cognitive processing during sleep. Int. J. Psychol. 44, 290–304.

CrossRef Full Text

Ko, D., Kwon, S., Lee, G. T., Im, C. H., Kim, K. H., and Jung, K. Y. (2012). Theta oscillation related to the auditory discrimination process in mismatch negativity: oddball versus control paradigm. J. Clin. Neurol. 8, 35–42.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kolev, V., Demiralp, T., Yordanova, J., Ademoglu, A., and Isoglu-Alkaç, U. (1997). Time-frequency analysis reveals multiple functional components during oddball P300. Neuroreport 8, 2061–2065.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Luu, P., and Tucker, D. M. (2001). Regulating action: alternating activation of midline frontal and motor cortical networks. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 1295–1306.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maquet, P., Péters, J., Aerts, J., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., et al. (1996). Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming. Nature 383, 163–166.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marco-Pallares, J., Cucurell, D., Cunillera, T., García, R., Andrés-Pueyo, A., Münte, T. F., et al. (2008). Human oscillatory activity associated to reward processing in a gambling task. Neuropsychologia 46, 241–248.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Näätänen, R., Paavilainen, P., Rinne, T., and Alho, K. (2007). The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: a review. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2544–2590.

CrossRef Full Text

Nieuwenhuis, S., Holroyd, C. B., Mol, N., and Coles, M. G. (2004). Reinforcement-related brain potentials from medial frontal cortex: origins and functional significance. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 441–448.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nir, Y., and Tononi, G. (2010). Dreaming and the brain: from phenomenology to neurophysiology. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 14, 88–100.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nofzinger, E. A., Mintun, M. A., Wiseman, M., Kupfer, D. J., and Moore, R. Y. (1997). Forebrain activation in REM sleep: an FDG PET study. Brain Res. 770, 192–201.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rinne, T., Alho, K., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Virtanen, J., and Näätänen, R. (2000). Separate time behaviors of the temporal and frontal mismatch negativity sources. Neuroimage 12, 14–19.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Romei, V., Gross, J., and Thut, G. (2010). On the role of prestimulus alpha rhythms over occipito-parietal areas in visual input regulation: correlation or causation? J. Neurosci. 30, 8692–8697.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schadow, J., Lenz, D., Dettler, N., Fründ, I., and Herrmann, C. S. (2009). Early gamma-band responses reflect anticipatory top-down modulation in the auditory cortex. Neuroimage 47, 651–658.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sculthorpe, L. D., Ouellet, D. R., and Campbell, K. B. (2009). MMN elicitation during natural sleep to violations of an auditory pattern. Brain Res. 1290, 52–62.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Slabu, L., Escera, C., Grimm, S., and Costa-Faidella, J. (2010). Early change detection in humans as revealed by auditory brainstem and middle-latency evoked potentials. Eur. J. Neurosci. 32, 859–865.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Slabu, L., Grimm, S., and Escera, C. (2012). Novelty detection in the human auditory brainstem. J. Neurosci. 32, 1447–1452.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ullsperger, M., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). Subprocesses of performance monitoring: a dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs. Neuroimage 14, 1387–1401.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yordanova, J., Devrim, M., Kolev, V., Ademoglu, A., and Demiralp, T. (2000). Multiple time-frequency components account for the complex functional reactivity of P300. Neuroreport 11, 1097–1103.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yordanova, J., Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., and Kolev, V. (2004). Parallel systems of error processing in the brain. Neuroimage 22, 590–602.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Yordanova J, Kolev V and Kirov R (2012) Brain oscillations and predictive processing. Front. Psychology 3:416. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00416

Received: 01 September 2012; Accepted: 30 September 2012;
Published online: 17 October 2012.

Edited by:

Shimon Edelman, Cornell University, USA

Reviewed by:

Axel Cleeremans, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium

Copyright: © 2012 Yordanova, Kolev and Kirov. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: jyord@bio.bas.bg; ru@bio.bas.bg

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.