Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 05 February 2013
Sec. Decision Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Consumer neuroscience applications towards understanding reward-based decision-making processes View all 4 articles

Reward and punishment: investigating cortico-bulbar excitability to disclose the value of goods

  • 1School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
  • 2Faculty of Motor Science, University of Palermo, Palermo, Italy
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Bologna “Alma Mater Studiorum,” Bologna, Italy
  • 4Centro di Studi e Ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive, Polo Scientifico-Didattico di Cesena, Cesena, Italy
  • 5Istituto Di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico, Fondazione Santa Lucia, Roma, Italy
  • 6School of Psychology, Univerisity of Wales, Bangor, UK

A continuing challenge for neuroscientists is to develop new conceptual tools and methodologies for understanding, predicting, and modeling the influences of rewarding/punishing outcomes on human behavior and decision-making. Reinforcement shapes behaviors from the most primitive (fight/flight, ingest/regurgitate, approach/avoid) to complex (buy/sell). Understanding the neural processes underlying reinforcement is critical for understanding economic and social decision-making. Moreover, comprehension of de-ranged processing and responses to reinforcing stimuli is crucial across a range of psychology fields and society as a whole, including psychiatric and neurological illness, eating disorders, criminality, and sociopathy (Vicario and Crescentini, 2012).

Neuroimaging methods have provided important information on the neural network underlying reward processing. Studies have shown that the reward value (O'Doherty et al., 2000) and the subjective pleasantness (Kringelbach et al., 2003) of food and non-food stimuli are reflected in the activity of orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). This region has been implicated in computing the hedonic value of food and in response to reward predicting signals in humans (Anderson et al., 2003). The OFC is part of a widespread network that includes also several subcortical regions such as hypothalamus, amygdala, dopaminergic midbrain, as well as parts of the basal ganglia including the ventral and dorsal striatum (Kringelbach, 2005). The network includes also cortical regions such as the insula and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) which are reciprocally connected to the OFC and to hypothalamic and brainstem pathways mediating autonomic and visceral control (Freedman et al., 2000).

Dysregulation of this network could be responsible for several typologies of eating disorders such as anorexia nervosa (AN) and hyperphagia. Decreases in striatal D2 receptors have been linked to compulsive food intake in obese rodents (Johnson and Kenny, 2010) and with decreased metabolic activity in OFC and ACC in obese humans (Tremblay and Schultz, 1999). On the other hand, it was suggested that some changes in dopamine D2 receptor binding in obese individuals may be a consequence rather than the cause of their over-eating (Berridge et al., 2010). Dopaminergic dysfunction, particularly in striatal circuits, might also contribute to altered reward and decreased food ingestion in individuals with AN (Kaye et al., 2009). For example, a seminal imaging study reported that the observation of food pictures by underweight subjects with AN led to altered activity in insula, OFC, and ACC (Frank et al., 2005). The role of dopaminergic alteration corresponding to these midbrain circuits was furthermore demonstrated in a recent work reporting that patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) experienced less motivational arousal for appetitive images compared with individuals of the same age without PD (Shore et al., 2011). By contrast, hyperphagic individuals with Prader–Willi syndrome manifested enhanced motivational arousal for appetitive images in the same paradigm (Hinton et al., 2010).

Modulation of reward circuitry was observed when viewing faces contingent on their sexual attractiveness (Kranz and Ishai, 2006) or by monetary compensation (Urban et al., 2012). For instance, in a decision-making task (O'Doherty et al., 2001) participants had to associate arbitrary stimuli with monetary wins or losses. A dissociation in location of fMRI signals was found such that activity in the medial OFC cortex correlated with wins on single trials, and activity of the lateral OFC cortex correlated with losses on single trials. Finally, reward related circuits for appetitive stimuli are also involved in social evaluation. For example, the insula is a key region in processing disgusting odors (Wicker et al., 2003) but is also modulated when processing social and moral emotions such empathy, guilt, and shame (Wicker et al., 2003; Moll et al., 2005; Lamm and Singer, 2010; Azevedo et al., 2012). For example, observing faces of people expressing disgust activates the anterior insula of the observer similarly to what would occur if the observer was disgusted himself (Wicker et al., 2003; Jabbi et al., 2007). Moreover, Sanfey et al. (2003) reported insula activation, in participants who received unfair monetary offers. These findings indicate that sensorial and social disgust might share, at least in part, common neural pathways (see also Schaich Borg et al., 2008). In keeping with this proposal, scholars have suggested that higher mental processes, such as those mediating interpersonal and moral disgust, may have originated from the corresponding sensorial pattern (Haidt et al., 1997; Chapman et al., 2009).

While the research reviewed above highlights the usefulness of neuroimaging techniques in functionally localizing the correlates of reward/punishment outcomes, other methodological approaches are needed to address several key issues that are opaque to functional neuroimaging. It should be taken into account that blood oxygen level dependent fMRI alone cannot determine whether neural activity is excitatory or inhibitory in nature, it has low temporal resolution and its signal mainly represents the local field potentials thus reflecting more the local processing of inputs rather than the output signals (Goense and Logothetis, 2008).

To overcome some of these limitations, we suggest an embodied cognition approach for the investigation of the psychobiology of reinforcement. According to previous suggestion (Vicario and Candidi, 2011; Vicario and Ticini, 2012), we propose the hypothesis that somatosensory intra-oral activity might represent a possible somatic marker linking neural processes and motor output along the reward/punishment continuum. There are a number of arguments in support of the view that reward/punishment stimuli may influence the somatosensory intra-oral activity. First, it is well-established that cortico-bulbar motor neurons in rats respond to four basic tastes (Yamamoto et al., 1982). Second, studies have shown that agonists and antagonists of dopamine, a neurotransmitter critically involved in the regulation of the reward system, spontaneously activate neurons in the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS), dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus (nX), and nucleus nervi hypoglossi (nXI1) (Granata and Woodruff, 1982), cortico-bulbar components actively involved in the regulating feeding behavior (see Simon et al., 2006). More recently, Pilarski et al. (2012) have shown in mice that chronic exposure to nicotine, which affects the activity of the reward system during embryonic development and during the first week of life, also alters neurotransmission and excitability in hypoglossal motoneurons by decreasing the glutamatergic excitatory synaptic input of these neurons. These findings hint at the possible functional link between the neural cortico-bulbar representation of intraoral muscles and basic rewards processing. Notably, previous work using psychophysiological assessment in humans has provided indirect support for such link which may extend to non-food reinforcers. For example, using peripheral electromyography (EMG) Chapman et al. (2009) showed that activity of the levator labii—a muscle involved in the facial expression of disgust—increased not only when drinking unpleasant liquids, but also when watching disgusting pictures of contaminants, and when exposed to unfair treatment in an economic game (i.e., during moral disgust). In a similar vein, Gal (2012) has shown that salivary secretion is stimulated even by material rewards such as money or sport cars, similar to what happens with physiological hunger (refer to the Table 1 for details). However, to date, no studies have directly investigated whether food and non-food reinforcement stimuli, along the rewarding-punishing continuum, modulate the cortical representation of intra-oral muscles, although a recent work has shown that combined odor and tastant stimulation (i.e., flavor of lemon) can increase motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) amplitude from the submental muscles during swallowing (Wahab et al., 2010).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Experimental evidence in support of relation between cortico-bulbar excitability and reward/punishment outcomes.

We propose that reinforcement stimuli along the rewarding-punishing continuum can be mapped in a specific somatotopic pattern of intra-oral muscles. This pattern can be investigated at a cortical level using specific neurophysiological assessment that allows to measure changes in the excitability of tongue representations in the primary motor cortex and cortico-bulbar system. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) represents an extraordinary method to non-invasively stimulate the human motor cortex and assess motor excitability of specific body parts, by recording MEPs from target muscles. Notably, changes in motor excitability as assessed by means of TMS-induced MEPs can occur in the absence of peripheral changes in EMG. Thus, this method is in principle more sensitive than peripheral EMG and can provide more direct information about neural processing at a cortical level. Importantly, this method offers high temporal resolution, can disambiguate between inhibitory and excitatory neural activity by assessing the amplitude of MEPs and has been effectively used to explore sensorimotor functions (Hallett, 2007; Avenanti et al., 2012a,b) as well as social cognition (Schütz-Bosbach et al., 2009; Candidi et al., 2010; Avenanti et al., 2012c).

Relevant to the present proposal, TMS studies have already shown that perception of emotional stimuli modulates the excitability of upper-limb motor representations. For example, watching emotionally arousing pictures (i.e., sexual scenes or images depicting aggressions or disgusting situations) brings about a general increase of motor excitability (Hajcak et al., 2007; van Loon et al., 2010; Borgomaneri et al., 2012), in keeping with the notion that emotion prime the body for action (Frijda, 2009). On the other hand, different types of salient stimuli may also induce a fast reduction of motor excitability. For example, visual flashes (Cantello et al., 2000) or unexpected loud auditory stimuli (Furubayashi et al., 2000) as well as suddenly approaching objects (Makin et al., 2009) or sounds (Serino et al., 2009; Avenanti et al., 2012a) occurring near the body (i.e., within the peripersonal space; Serino et al., 2011) are typically associated to short-latency motor inhibition. Moreover, reduced motor excitability is found during administration of noxious stimuli to a specific body part (Farina et al., 2001; Urban et al., 2004) or when observing pain stimuli on the same body of others (Minio-Paluello et al., 2006; Avenanti et al., 2009). All these findings have been interpreted as reflecting specific approaching/avoidance and freezing upper-limb motor responses to emotionally salient stimuli. The fine-grained topographic features of such motor modulations hint at the possibility that reinforcement stimuli, and in particular food, may specifically modulate neural activity within tongue motor cortical representations.

When applied to the primary orofacial motor cortex, TMS can be used to monitor changes in the excitability of the cortico-bulbar representations of tongue or lip muscles (Muellbacher et al., 1994) with a relatively high temporal resolution. This can be done by measuring tongue MEPs elicited by TMS from the cortico-bulbar pathway under various experimental conditions. Previous TMS studies show that the amplitude of tongue MEPs is modulated by motor training (e.g., repeated tongue protrusion), pain, and language-related processing (i.e., speech listening or viewing) (Fadiga et al., 2002; Watkins et al., 2003; Boudreau et al., 2007; Sato et al., 2010). The excitability of tongue motor representations may be modulated directly or indirectly (i.e., via the premotor cortex) by the outputs originating from the reward system network and possibly conveyed through the axonal pathways linking the tongue with the primary gustatory cortex. We speculate that food, and possibly evolutionarily advanced reinforcers such as monetary compensation or social disgust may impinge on motor representations of intraoral muscles in the cortex. Future studies are needed to directly test the influence of rewards on tongue and lips cortico-bulbar representation.

References

Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Stappen, I., Panitz, D., Ghahremani, D. G., Glover, G., et al. (2003). Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in human olfaction. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 196–202.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Avenanti, A., Annela, L., and Serino, A. (2012a). Suppression of premotor cortex disrupts motor coding of peripersonal space. Neuroimage 63, 281–288.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Avenanti, A., Annella, L., Candidi, M., Urgesi, C., and Aglioti, S. M. (2012b). Compensatory plasticity in the action observation network: virtual lesions of STS enhance anticipatory simulation of seen actions. Cereb. Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs040. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Avenanti, A., Coccia, M., Ladavas, E., Provinciali, L., and Ceravolo, M. G. (2012c). Low-frequency rTMS promotes use-dependent motor plasticity in chronic stroke: a randomized trial. Neurology 78, 256–264.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Avenanti, A., Minio-Paluello, I., Sforza, A., and Aglioti, S. M. (2009). Freezing or escaping? Opposite modulations of empathic reactivity to the pain of others. Cortex 45, 1072–1077.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Azevedo, R. T., Macaluso, E., Avenanti, A., Santangelo, V., Cazzato, V., and Aglioti, S. M. (2012). Their pain is not our pain: brain and autonomic correlates of empathic resonance with the pain of same and different race individuals. Hum. Brain Mapp. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22133. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berridge, K. C., Ho, C. Y., Richard, J. M., and DiFeliceantonio, A. G. (2010). The tempted brain eats: pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating disorders. Brain Res. 1350, 43–64.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Borgomaneri, S., Gazzola, V., and Avenanti, A. (2012). Motor mapping of implied actions during perception of emotional body language. Brain Stimul. 5, 70–76.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boudreau, S., Romaniello, A., Wang, K., Svensson, P., Sessle, B. J., and Arendt-Nielsen, L. (2007). The effects of intra-oral pain on motor cortex neuroplasticity associated with short-term novel tongue-protrusion training in humans. Pain 132, 169–178.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Candidi, M., Vicario, C. M., Abreu, A. M., and Aglioti, S. M. (2010). Competing mechanisms for mapping action-related categorical knowledge and observed actions. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2832–2841.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cantello, R., Civardi, C., Cavalli, A., Varrasi, C., and Vicentini, R. (2000). Effects of a photic input on the human cortico-motoneuron connection. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 1981–1989.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Chapman, H. A., Kim, D. A., Susskind, J. M., and Anderson, A. K. (2009). In bad taste: evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science 323, 1222–1226.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Buccino, G., and Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 399–402.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Farina, S., Valeriani, M., Rosso, T., Aglioti, S., Tamburin, S., Fiaschi, A., et al. (2001). Transient inhibition of the human motor cortex by capsaicin-induced pain. A study with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurosci. Lett. 314, 97–101.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frank, G. K., Bailer, U. F., Henry, S. E., Drevets, W., Meltzer, C. C., Price, J. C., et al. (2005). Increased dopamine D2/D3 receptor binding after recovery from anorexia nervosa measured by positron emission tomography and [11c] raclopride. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 908–912.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Freedman, L. J., Insel, T. R., and Smith, Y. (2000). Subcortical projections of area 25 (subgenual cortex) of the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 421, 172–188.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frijda, N. H. (2009). Emotion experience and its varieties. Emot. Rev. 1, 264–271.

Furubayashi, T., Ugawa, Y., Terao, Y., Hanajima, R., Sakai, K., Machii, K., et al. (2000). The human hand motor area is transiently suppressed by an unexpected auditory stimulus. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 178–183.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gal, D. (2012). A mouth-watering prospect: salivation to material reward. J. Consum. Res. 38, 1022–1029.

Goense, J. B., and Logothetis, N. K. (2008). Neurophysiology of the BOLD fMRI signal in awake monkeys. Curr. Biol. 18, 631–640.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Granata, A. R., and Woodruff, G. N. (1982). Dopaminergic mechanisms in the nucleus tractus solitarius and effects on blood pressure. Brain Res. Bull. 8, 483–488.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Haidt, J., Rozin, P., McCauley, C., and Imada, S. (1997). Body, psyche, and culture: the relationship of disgust to morality. Psychol. Dev. Soc. 9, 107–131.

Hajcak, G., Molnar, C., George, M. S., Bolger, K., Koola, J., and Nahas, Z. (2007). Emotion facilitates action: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study of motor cortex excitability during picture viewing. Psychophysiology 44, 91–97.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hajiha, M., DuBord, M. A., Liu, H., and Horner, R. L. (2009). Opioid receptor mechanisms at the hypoglossal motor pool and effects on tongue muscle activity in vivo. J. Physiol. 587(Pt 11), 2677–2692.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hallett, M. (2007). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: a primer. Neuron 55, 187–199.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hinton, E. C., Isles, A. R., Williams, N. M., and Parkinson, J. A. (2010). Excessive appetitive arousal in Prader-Willi syndrome. Appetite 54, 225–228.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jabbi, M., Swart, M., and Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. Neuroimage 34, 1744–1753.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Johnson, P. M., and Kenny, P. J. (2010). Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 635–641.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kaye, W. H., Fudge, J. L., and Paulus, M. (2009). New insights into symptoms and neurocircuit function of anorexia nervosa. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 573–584.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kranz, F., and Ishai, A. (2006). Face perception is modulated by sexual preference. Curr. Biol. 16, 63–68.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 691–702.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kringelbach, M. L., O'Doherty, J., Rolls, E. T., and Andrews, C. (2003). Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1064–1071.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lamm, C., and Singer, T. (2010). The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotions. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 579–591.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Makin, T. R., Holmes, N. P., Brozzoli, C., Rossetti, Y., and Farnè, A. (2009). Coding of visual space during motor preparation: approaching objects rapidly modulate corticospinal excitability in hand-centered coordinates. J. Neurosci. 29, 11841–11851.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Minio-Paluello, I., Avenanti, A., and Aglioti, S. M. (2006). Left hemisphere dominance in reading the sensory qualities of others' pain? Soc. Neurosci. 1, 320–333.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moll, J., Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Krueger, F., and Grafman, J. (2005). Opinion: the neural basis of human moral cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 799–809.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muellbacher, W., Mathis, J., and Hess, C. W. (1994). Electrophysiological assessment of central and peripheral motor routes to the lingual muscles. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 57, 309–315.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

O'Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., and Andrews, C. (2001). Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 95–102.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Doherty, J., Rolls, E. T., Francis, S., Bowtell, R., McGlone, F., Kobal, G., et al. (2000). Sensory-specific satiety-related olfactory activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroreport 11, 893–897.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Pilarski, J. Q., Wakefield, H. E., Fuglevand, A. J., Levine, R. B., and Fregosi, R. F. (2012). Increased nicotinic receptor desensitization in hypoglossal motor neurons following chronic developmental nicotine exposure. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 257–264.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., and Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science 300, 1755–1758.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sato, M., Buccino, G., Gentilucci, M., and Cattaneo, L. (2010). On the tip of the tongue: modulation of the primary motor cortex during audiovisual speech perception. Speech Commun. 5, 2533–2541.

Schaich Borg, J., Lieberman, D., and Kiehl, K. A. (2008). Infection, incest, and iniquity: investigating the neural correlates of disgust and morality. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 1529–1546.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schütz-Bosbach, S., Avenanti, A., Aglioti, S. M., and Haggard, P. (2009). Don't do it! Cortical inhibition and self-attribution during action observation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1215–1227.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Serino, A., Annella, L., and Avenanti, A. (2009). Motor properties of peripersonal space in humans. PLoS ONE 4:e6582. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006582

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Serino, A., Canzoneri, E., and Avenanti, A. (2011). Fronto-parietal areas necessary for a multisensory representation of peripersonal space in humans: an rTMS study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2956–2967.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shore, D. M., Rafal, R., and Parkinson, J. A. (2011). Appetitive motivational deficits in individuals with Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 26, 1887–1892.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simon, S. A., de Araujo, I. E., Gutierrez, R., and Nicolelis, M. A. (2006). The neural mechanisms of gustation: a distributed processing code. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 890–901.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tremblay, L., and Schultz, W. (1999). Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal cortex. Nature 398, 704–708.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Urban, N. B., Slifstein, M., Meda, S., Xu, X., Ayoub, R., Medina, O., et al. (2012). Imaging human reward processing with positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 221, 67–77.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Urban, P. P., Solinski, M., Best, C., Rolke, R., Hopf, H. C., and Dieterich, M. (2004). Different short-term modulation of cortical motor output to distal and proximal upper-limb muscles during painful sensory nerve stimulation. Muscle Nerve 29, 663–669.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Loon, A. M., van den Wildenberg, W. P., van Stegeren, A. H., Hajcak, G., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2010). Emotional stimuli modulate readiness for action: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cogn. Affect Behav. Neurosci. 10, 174–181.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vicario, C. M., and Candidi, M. (2011). Somatosensory intra-oral activity reveals functional abnormalities in the insula of anorexia nervosa suffers. Med. Hypotheses 77, 698–699.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vicario, C. M., and Crescentini, C. (2012). Punishing food: what brain activity can tell us about the representation of food in recovered anorexia nervosa. Biol. Psychiatry 71, e31–e32.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vicario, C. M., and Ticini, L. F. (2012). Measuring your dependence: deranged corticobulbar excitability may uncover addiction disorders. Front. Psychiatry 3:107. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wahab, N. A., Jones, R. D., and Huckabee, M. L. (2010). Effects of olfactory and gustatory stimuli on neural excitability for swallowing. Physiol. Behav. 102, 485–490.

Watkins, K. E., Strafella, A. P., and Paus, T. (2003). Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production. Neuropsychologia 41, 989–994.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40, 655–664.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yamamoto, T., Fujiwara, T., Matsuo, R., and Kawamura, Y. (1982). Hypoglossal motor nerve activity elicited by taste and thermal stimuli applied to the tongue in rats. Brain Res. 238, 89–104.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Vicario CM, Kritikos A, Avenanti A and Rafal R (2013) Reward and punishment: investigating cortico-bulbar excitability to disclose the value of goods. Front. Psychology 4:39. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00039

Received: 30 October 2012; Accepted: 16 January 2013;
Published online: 05 February 2013.

Edited by:

Martin Reimann, University of Southern California, USA

Copyright © 2013 Vicario, Kritikos, Avenanti and Rafal. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: carmelo.vicario@uniroma1.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.