Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 18 April 2013
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Inhibition in the process of feature binding View all 12 articles

The process of feature binding

  • Psychology, Cognitive Science, Department of Humanities and Social Sciences, Indian Institute of Technology Delhi, New Delhi, India

The integration of different stimulus properties as an object is the process of feature binding. The research topic “Inhibition in the process of feature binding” aimed to debate which features bind together, why, and how; and what happens to features which are not integrated.

The authors of the review articles took up the debate in earnest. Wyatte et al. (2012) emphasize that the process of binding does not require any special neural substrates. The two mechanisms of inhibition and top-down feedback are sufficient to explain the process of object recognition. Their computational model describes how these established principles of neural processing work over time to solve the binding problem. Particularly critical of the idea of neural synchrony, often held forth as “the” mechanism underlying feature binding, they contend that it helps only to create a contrast between relevant and irrelevant features. It is top-down feedback, which reinforces relevant information.

The relevance of features is the key factor in the process of binding for Jaswal (2012). She attempts an integration of physiological and psychological literature, highlighting that only relevant features bind together in a rather slow process, which concomitantly ensures that the irrelevant features are actively inhibited. In a more expanded overview, Velik (2012) reaches similar conclusions—that irrelevant information is deleted from the system in the process of binding. However, whilst Jaswal (2012) bases her conclusions on empirical studies, Velik (2012) presents a computational model in which filter mechanisms work to suppress irrelevant information. Krummenacher and Müller (2012) review evidence from behavioral and ERP studies to confirm that separate feature dimensions are important in the pre-selective phase of feature binding. However, once the bound object emerges, feature based effects are not as dominant. This corroborates with the aforementioned views that only relevant features are carried forward.

Herzog et al. (2012) also address how we process features prior to and during the process of binding. Using the sequential metacontrast paradigm, they explore binding as a sub-process in the Gestalt experience of grouping elements into wholes. They contend that computation of features is in itself a process and not an instantaneous event. Stimulus representations are dynamic and binding probably occurs simultaneously with the processing of independent features.

Meier and Rey-Mermet (2012) discuss episodic context binding using bivalent stimuli. The ambiguity of bivalent stimuli is itself a “feature” that enters binding and influences subsequent behavior in consonance with the bivalency effect. It is interesting that Meier and Ray-Mermet propose that this conflict is not relevant to the task. In fact, this ambiguity and concomitant conflict is a cardinal feature of bivalent stimuli, which reactivates whenever the context is redintegrated.

Moving from features to objects, Dent et al. (2012) review behavioral and physiological evidence for the process of distracter inhibition in visual search. They postulate that it is a resource demanding active process, which is parallel in nature, such that all distracters are deleted, and the target alone remains as the item to be processed further.

The four articles contributing original research extend the study of binding to novel avenues. As merits empirical investigations, all studies not only document details of current work in these new areas, they also indicate hypotheses for future investigations.

Delogu et al. (2012) enter the arena of audition to investigate the link between location and temporal order. The recall of temporal order is weakened more than locations, whilst binding in visual as well as auditory domains. This suggests that location is encoded relatively automatically during binding, but recall of temporal order is resource demanding. Similar results across visual and auditory domains suggest that binding of location and temporal order may involve shared, modality-independent physiological mechanisms. This special link between “when” and “where” features, merits future explorations.

Giersch et al. (2012) scale up from features to explore grouping and regrouping between objects among schizophrenics and healthy controls. Patients were particularly slow to detect connected targets when the attentional focus was on unconnected pairs. This effect was found only for targets presented within the same hemifield. Thus, schizophrenics do not regroup stimuli in the same way as healthy controls. Speculations can be made regarding the role of connectivity between hemispheres in grouping. Further, grouping and regrouping are different (maybe parallel) mechanisms, the former relying on automatic processing, whilst the latter demand attentional resources.

The two original research articles based on computational models conceptualize binding as part of other processes. Schrobsdorff et al. (2012) propose that binding is an essential phase in inhibition. In their model of the negative priming effect, features are activated, bound into object entities, and related to their context. Inhibitory processes and changing thresholds implement the concept of selective attention. Thus, binding becomes a sub-process in a general model of inhibition. Davelaar (2013) evokes the concept of binding to explain the Gratton effect in the Eriksen flanker task. The Gratton effect is the reduction in the interference due to flankers after incongruent, as compared with congruent trials. Results of experiments that separate the contributions from target, flanker, and response repetition, show that flanker repetition alone is sufficient to produce congruency effects in sequences of trials. He postulates that representations of targets, flankers, and response(s) are associated in a task set. This work is an excellent example of how the concept of binding is used to understand other ideas and paradigms.

The aim of this research topic was to consider the process of binding from various perspectives such that an integrated view emerged to guide future research and theory. Challenging the assumption that feature binding is an automatic “event” driven by bottom up processes dependent on conjunctively coding neurons or synchrony, the collection of these articles yield the conclusion that the emergence of a bound object capable of further processing, is itself a sub-process, which is heavily contingent on re-entrant mechanisms and is probably resource demanding. Future investigations may explore feature binding as a basic process in myriad behavioral sequences of diverse populations.

References

Davelaar, E. J. (2013). When the ignored gets bound: sequential effects in the flanker task. Front. Psychol. 3:552. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00552

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Delogu, F., Nijboer, T. C. W., and Postma, A. (2012). Binding “when” and “where” impairs temporal, but not spatial recall in auditory and visual working memory. Front. Psychol. 3:62. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00062

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dent, K., Allen, H. A., Braithwaite, J. J., and Humphreys, G. W. (2012). Parallel distractor rejection as a binding mechanism in search. Front. Psychol. 3:278. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00278

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Giersch, A., van Assche, M., Capa, R. L., Marrer, C., and Gounot, D. (2012). Patients with schizophrenia do not preserve automatic grouping when mentally re-grouping figures: shedding light on an ignored difficulty. Front. Psychol. 3:274. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00274

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Herzog, M. H., Otto, T. U., and Ögmen, H. (2012). The fate of visible features of invisible elements. Front. Psychol. 3:119. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00119

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jaswal, S. (2012). The importance of being relevant. Front. Psychol. 3:309. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00309

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Krummenacher, J., and Müller, H. J. (2012). Dynamic weighting of feature dimensions in visual search: behavioral and psychophysiological evidence. Front. Psychol. 3:221. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00221

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meier, B., and Rey-Mermet, A. (2012). Beyond feature binding: interference from episodic context binding creates the bivalency effect in task-switching. Front. Psychol. 3:386. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00386

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schrobsdorff, H., Ihrke, M., Behrendt, J., Hasselhorn, M., and Herrmann, J. M. (2012). Inhibition in the dynamics of selective attention: an integrative model for negative priming. Front. Psychol. 3:491. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00491

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Velik, R. (2012). From simple receptors to complex multimodal percepts: a first global picture on the mechanisms involved in perceptual binding. Front. Psychol. 3:259. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00259

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wyatte, D., Herd, S., Mingus, B., and O'Reilly, R. (2012). The role of competitive inhibition and top-down feedback in binding during object recognition. Front. Psychol. 3:182. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00182

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Jaswal S (2013) The process of feature binding. Front. Psychol. 4:207. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00207

Received: 29 March 2013; Accepted: 03 April 2013;
Published online: 18 April 2013.

Edited by:

Eddy J. Davelaar, Birkbeck College, UK

Reviewed by:

Eddy J. Davelaar, Birkbeck College, UK

Copyright © 2013 Jaswal. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: sneh.jaswal@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.