Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 17 October 2013
Sec. Cognitive Science

The meaning of additive reaction-time effects: some misconceptions

  • Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA

A commentary on
Additive factors do not imply discrete processing stages: a worked example using models of the Stroop task

by Stafford, T., and Gurney, K. N. (2011). Front. Psychol. 2:287. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00287

Additive Effects

Stafford and Gurney (2011) (S&G) discuss a beautiful set of reaction-time (RT) data (Figure 1) from an experiment using the Stroop effect in which they varied the saturation of the color in which the word was printed. They found persuasive additivity of the substantial effects on mean RT of two factors: saturation of the color to be named, and congruence of the word with the color name.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mean RT data from Stafford et al. (2011, Experiment 1) and fitted additive model. Standard errors (SEs) shown (with mean 5.0 ms), which can be used to assess goodness of fit, are based on deviations from additive models plus linear interactions fitted to the 15 mean RTs from each of the 20 subjects. The SE based on the residual mean square (114 df) in an anova, after removing the effects of subject, saturation, congruence, their two-way interactions, and the interaction with subjects of the linear interaction of saturation and congruence, is 8.2 ms.

Such additivity is consistent with the RT being generated by two stages (successive processes), C: color discrimination, whose duration is influenced by saturation but not congruence, and N: retrieval and pronunciation of the color name, whose duration is influenced by congruence but not saturation. (There must also be another process, W: word recognition, in which the word is identified, which perhaps ends before N and may operate in parallel with C.) S&G claim that their findings can also be explained by a model that contains a single process influenced by both factors. Because they interpret the additive factors method (AFM; Sternberg, 1969, 1984, 1998, 2001, 2011) to be associated with the idea that the additivity of the effects of two factors implies two stages, they conclude that the assumptions of the AFM are “untenable.”

Inference from Additivity

I believe that such additivity supports (increases belief in) a two-part hypothesis: processes organized serially (stages) together with selective influence of the factors on those stages, but doesn't imply that hypothesis (Sternberg, 2001, Table 3). If more than one model is consistent with some properties of a set of data, then selecting among models often requires a search for predictions of other properties that distinguish them, sometimes supplemented by further experiments and/or plausibility considerations. Examples are provided by Roberts and Sternberg (1993), who considered five data sets in relation to stage models together with two other models that can explain additive effects on mean RT: an “alternate pathways model” and a version of the cascade model (McClelland, 1979; Ashby, 1982). By assuming stochastic independence of stage durations to strengthen the stage models, and considering properties that depend on RT variance, they could discriminate among the alternatives. See also Schweickert et al. (2012, Ch. 6).

Implications of Alternative Models

Since the AFM was introduced (Sternberg, 1969), alternatives to stage models have been developed that are consistent with means additivity (Ashby, 1982; Roberts and Sternberg, 1993; Miller et al., 1995). However, in all such models thus far, the prediction of additivity derives from functionally distinct processses plus selective influence. Thus, the existence of these alternatives does not weaken the support provided by additivity for functionally distinct and separately modifiable processes, but does weaken support for those processes being organized as stages. However, S&G's single-process model has not been shown to produce additive effects, and seems implausible.

Are Effects Additive in S&G's “One-Stage” Model?

S&G made no attempt to fit models to their data. Even without the constraints this might impose, saturation and congruence interact systematically in their simulation of the single-process model (S&G figure 3) for which they claim additivity: for their five increasing saturation levels of 0.2, 0.4, 0.6, 0.8, and 1.0, the RT differences between incongruent and neutral conditions are 1.77, 1.10, 1.00, 1.12, and 1.47; those between neutral and congruent conditions are 2.15, 1.25, 1.04, 1.00, 1.04; both in arbitrary units. Given additivity, the differences in each set would all be equal.

Is S&G's “One-Stage” Model Plausible?

The single-process model for which S&G claim additive effects has “locked inputs”: to the extent that low saturation delays color discrimination it also delays word recognition. (S&G conclude that their models with unlocked inputs cannot produce the desired additivity). Because the time to recognize the word probably depends primarily on its luminance contrast (which was fixed) rather than its color saturation, a model with “locked inputs” seems implausible. Moreover, support for the idea that recognition of the word is not influenced by saturation is provided by the similarity of these data to the results of experiment 2 in Stafford et al. (2011), in which a white word was displayed at a different location from a patch whose color was named. If saturation had influenced the recognition of words in experiment 1, the results of the two experiments should have differed more.

More on AFM Reasoning

Others have expressed different misconceptions about the “logic” of the AFM and the nature of stage models. According to Van Zandt and Ratcliff (1995), “The use of additive mean RT's as support for a serial arrangement of subprocesses depends heavily on the assumption of selective influence …” (p. 34). Instead, the prediction of additivity should be thought of as depending on a two-part hypothesis: stages (seriality), and selective influence. Observation of additivity supports both parts, just as confirmation of a prediction from any theory supports that theory. Independent evidence for selective influence is not required.

What is a Stage Model?

One misconception about the kinds of models to which the AFM is relevant is perhaps due to confusion between flow-charts (with units that are subprocessES and arrows that indicate successiveness), and block diagrams (with units that are processORS and arrows that indicate information transfer). (Broadbent 1984) and (Coltheart 2011) have argued that a process whose subprocesses are organized in stages cannot involve feedback because it must be implemented by a “pipeline”: an ordered set of processors through which information passes in a fixed direction from input to output. They seem to misinterpret the boxes in diagrams of stage models as spatially arranged processORS, rather than temporally arranged processES.

Stage models partition processing operations into temporally successive (and functionally distinct) components. Nothing prevents a later stage from using added information (feedback) to re-process evidence that was extracted earlier. For example, in reading a word, the initial perception of a letter might be revised, based on contextual letters; the revision process could be a processING stage in which a letter-perception processOR is re-used. Also, not all stages require stimulus representations provided by earlier stages as input: consider the serial operations that may underlie visual search for a feature conjunction, in which each comparison between the target and a displayed item can be thought of as a stage. For the distinction among three kinds of stage (completion-controlled, outcome-contingent, and data-dependent), see (Sternberg 1984). Neurophysiological evidence for data-dependent stages is discussed by (Schall 2003), Woodman et al. (2008), Schall et al. (2011), and (Sternberg 2011, Section 3.2), and references therein. Neurophysiological evidence for completion-controlled stages is provided by Sigman and Dehaene (2008).

Acknowledgments

I thank Eddy J. Davelaar for inviting me to contribute this commentary, and Tom Stafford for providing numerical values for data and simulations.

Note

This article published in Frontiers in Psychology, under the section of Cognitive Science, was invited by the Chief Editor Professor Eddy Davelaar.

References

Ashby, F. G. (1982). Deriving exact predictions from the cascade model. Psychol. Rev. 89, 599–607. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.89.5.599

CrossRef Full Text

Broadbent, D. E. (1984). The Maltese cross: a new simplistic model for memory. Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 55–94. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00026121

CrossRef Full Text

Coltheart, M. (2011). Methods for modular modelling: additive factors and cognitive neuropsychology. Cogn. Neuropsych. 28, 224–240. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2011.587794

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McClelland, J. L. (1979). On the time relations of mental processes: An examination of systems of processes in cascade. Psychol. Rev. 86, 287–330. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.86.4.287

CrossRef Full Text

Miller, J., van der Ham, F., and Sanders, A. F. (1995). Overlapping stage models and reaction time additivity: effects of the activation equation. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 90, 11–28. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(95)00028-S

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roberts, S., and Sternberg, S. (1993). “The meaning of additive reaction-time effects: tests of three alternatives,” in Attention and Performance XIV: Synergies in Experimental Psychology, Artificial Intelligence, and Cognitive Neuroscience. eds D. E. Meyer and S. Kornblum (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 611–653.

Schall, J. D. (2003). Neural correlates of decision processes: neural and mental chronometry. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 182–186. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00039-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schall, J. D., Purcell, B. A., Heitz, R. P., Logan, G. D., and Palmeri, T. J. (2011). Neural mechanisms of saccade target selection: gated accumulator model of the visual-motor cascade. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 1991–2002. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07715.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schweickert, R., Fisher, D. L., and Sung, K. (2012). Discovering Cognitive Architecture by Selectively Influencing Mental Processes. Singapore: World Scientific. doi: 10.1142/7344

CrossRef Full Text

Sigman, M., and Dehaene, S. (2008). Brain mechanisms of serial and parallel processing during dual-task performance. J. Neurosci. 28, 7585–7598. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0948-08.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stafford, T., and Gurney, K. N. (2011). Additive factors do not imply discrete processing stages: a worked example using models of the Stroop task. Front. Psychol. 2:287. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00287

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stafford, T., Ingram, L., and Gurney, K. N. (2011). Piéron's law holds during Stroop conflict: insights into the architecture of decision making. Cogn. Sci. 35, 1553–1566. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01195.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sternberg, S. (1969). “The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method,” in Attention and Performance II. Acta Psych. ed W. G. Koster, 276–315. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(69)90055-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sternberg, S. (1984). Stage models of mental processing and the additive-factor method. Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 82–84. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00026285

CrossRef Full Text

Sternberg, S. (1998). “Discovering mental processing stages: the method of additive factors,” in An Invitation to Cognitive Science: Methods, Models, and Conceptual Issues, Vol. 4, eds D. Scarborough and S. Sternberg (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 703–863.

Sternberg, S. (2001). Separate modifiability, mental modules, and the use of pure and composite measures to reveal them. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 106, 147–246. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(00)00045-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sternberg, S. (2011). Modular processes in mind and brain. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 28, 156–208. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2011.557231

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Zandt, T., and Ratcliff, R. (1995). Statistical mimicking of reaction time data: single-process models, parameter variability, and mixtures. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2, 20–54. doi: 10.3758/BF03214411

CrossRef Full Text

Woodman, G. F., Kang, M.-S., Thompson, K., and Schall, J. D. (2008). The effect of visual search efficiency on response preparation: neurophysiological evidence for discrete flow. Psychol. Sci. 19, 128–136. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02058.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: stage models, additive-factors method, reaction time, RT, Stroop effect, factorial experiment, additivity

Citation: Sternberg S (2013) The meaning of additive reaction-time effects: some misconceptions. Front. Psychol. 4:744. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00744

Received: 30 August 2013; Accepted: 24 September 2013;
Published online: 17 October 2013.

Edited by:

Dietmar Heinke, University of Birmingham, UK

Copyright © 2013 Sternberg. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: saul@psych.upenn.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.