Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 11 July 2014
Sec. Consciousness Research

Are there really autonomous “unconscious” goals that drive behavior? An event-control approach to goals and actions

  • Centre of Behavioural and Cognitive Sciences, University of Allahabad, Allahabad, India

A commentary on
The selfish goal: autonomously operating motivational structures as the proximate cause of human judgment and behavior

by Huang, J.Y., and Bargh, J.A. (2014). Behav. Brain Sci. 37, 121–135. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000290

The article by Huang and Bargh (2014) discusses the role played by conscious and unconscious goals in the behavior of a person. The authors argue for the existence of unconscious goals. In addition, they argue that both conscious and unconscious goals have similar effects on behavior. In this commentary, we focus on the implications of an event-control approach to the conscious or unconscious nature of goals and their influence on perception and action.

The notion of goals and actions performed to achieve those goals indicates a close link between goals, actions performed to achieve the desired goals, and consequent perceptual effect. Such perception-action loops operating at multiple levels form the core of the event-control approach that has been proposed to understand consciousness, self and agency (Jordan, 2003; Kumar and Srinivasan, 2012, 2013, 2014). In this approach, the control system is theorized as a nested hierarchical system in which the lower levels are nested within the higher levels of the system. The conscious goals or intentions would operate at the higher level. The levels also differ in terms of the spatiotemporal extent of events on which control is exercised with the higher levels linked to more distal perception-action events and the lower levels linked to more proximal perception-action events. According to the event-control approach, activity at the higher level constrains activity at the lower level and the amount of control achieved given these constraints at the lower level is passed to the higher level further enabling the change in constraints passed again to the lower level. The nature of self depends on the highest level at which the control is achieved.

The event-control approach has potential implications for the principles proposed in the Huang and Bargh (2014) in discussing the manner in which conscious and unconscious goals influence other mental processes and behavior. Even assuming the existence of unconscious goals (the event-control approach can be neutral about that possibility), the event-control approach has a lot to say about the potential ways in which conscious or unconscious goals influence other processes.

While Huang and Bargh (2014) focus on multiple goals (conscious or otherwise) that potentially compete with each other, it misses the fact that the mind is a nested hierarchy with possible goals at each level in the control hierarchy (Jordan, 2003; Hurley, 2008; Kumar and Srinivasan, 2012, 2014). In addition, both perception and actions are hierarchically organized. Each action (or perception) is composed of other actions (or perceptions) resulting in a hierarchical structure.

According to the event-control approach, a specific priming effect might occur because of the goals at the lower levels and processes that are part of the lower level are activated by the typically conscious higher level goal. This interpretation implies that even if the lower level goal is unconscious, there is a priming effect only when it is activated indirectly by a conscious goal making it non-autonomous even if it produces a subliminal effect on behavior. The effect of the unconscious goal would be indirect, at best arguing against the automaticity principle. Given that the control processes at different levels operate at different spatio-temporal scales, the lower level goals would influence proximal behavior (like immediate priming effects) more than the higher level conscious goals. The influence of these lower level goals would be reduced or negated in dual task or high load conditions indicating that the putative unconscious goals would be susceptible to attentional influences elicited by the conscious task being performed.

The event-control approach argues that not only a higher level goal but relevant sub-goals might also be active at a given time. The reconfiguration principle under-specifies what goals or sub-goals are active assuming only one active (conscious?) goal at a time. The event-control approach does agree with the notion that the active (high level conscious) goal would constrain information processing at the lower levels of the hierarchical control system. In addition, as long as the actions themselves do not compete with each other, simultaneous multiple goal achievement is possible.

In addition, it has to be kept in mind that establishing the consciousness or unconsciousness of goals is not trivial and depends on the methods available for measuring consciousness or unconsciousness (Seth et al., 2008). The notion of unconscious effects based on priming have been questioned in the literature (Newell and Shanks, 2014) based on potential problems with the priming methodology (Rouder et al., 2007). While attempts have been made to refine the priming methodology to produce subliminal priming effects reliably (Finkbeiner, 2011), most studies simply report a null effect in a separate classification task performed with the prime stimuli (Marien et al., 2012). Given these difficulties, it is not clear that the unconscious nature of the goals has been rigorously established in empirical studies so far.

In conclusion, we argue that more rigorous demonstration of unconscious goals is necessary. In addition, it is not clear that an unconscious goal autonomously influences behavior unless it is possibly activated by a conscious goal (which is present even in a subliminal priming task). The notion of nested hierarchical control with multiple sub-goals at lower levels is important for understanding the role of conscious or unconscious goals and intentions in performing actions.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Finkbeiner, M. (2011). Subliminal priming with nearly perfect performance in the prime-classification task. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 73, 1255–1265. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0088-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Huang, J. Y., and Bargh, J. A. (2014). The selfish goal: autonomously operating motivational structures as the proximate cause of human judgment and behavior. Behav. Brain Sci. 37, 121–135. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13000290

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hurley, S. (2008). The shared circuit model (SCM): how control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation, and mindreading. Behav. Brain Sci. 31, 1–22. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07003123

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jordan, S. J. (2003). Emergence of self and other in perception and action: an event-control approach. Conscious. Cogn. 4, 633–646. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00075-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kumar, D., and Srinivasan, N. (2012). Hierarchical event-control and subjective experience of agency. Front. Psychol. 3:410. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00410

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kumar, D., and Srinivasan, N. (2013). “Hierarchical control and sense of agency: differential effects of control on implicit and explicit measures of agency,” in Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (Berlin), 2790–2795.

Kumar, D., and Srinivasan, N. (2014). Naturalizing sense of agency with a hierarchical event-control approach. PLoS ONE 9:e92431. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092431

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marien, H., Custers, R., Hassin, R. R., and Aarts, H. (2012). Unconscious goal activation and the hijacking of the executive function. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 103, 399–415. doi: 10.1037/a0028955

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Newell, B. R., and Shanks, D. R. (2014). Unconscious influences on decision making: a critical review. Behav. Brain Sci. 37, 1–19. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12003214

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Speckman, P. L., and Pratte, M. S. (2007). Detecting chance: a solution to the null sensitivity problem in subliminal priming. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14, 597–605. doi: 10.3758/BF03196808

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Seth, A. K., Dienes, Z., Cleeremans, A., Overgaard, M., and Pessoa, L. (2008). Measuring consciousness: relating behavioural and neurophysiological approaches. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 314–321. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: consciousness, unconscious, goals, event-control, control hierarchy, actions

Citation: Srinivasan N (2014) Are there really autonomous “unconscious” goals that drive behavior? An event-control approach to goals and actions. Front. Psychol. 5:723. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00723

Received: 04 May 2014; Paper pending published: 02 June 2014;
Accepted: 23 June 2014; Published online: 11 July 2014.

Edited by:

Olivia Carter, University of Melbourne, Australia

Reviewed by:

Jim Parkinson, Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, UK

Copyright © 2014 Srinivasan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: nsrini@cbcs.ac.in

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.