Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 July 2014
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Non invasive stimulation techniques: "Modulating cognition" View all 7 articles

Hits and Misses: Leveraging tDCS to Advance Cognitive Research

\r\nMarian E. Berryhill*Marian E. Berryhill*Dwight J. PetersonDwight J. PetersonKevin T. JonesKevin T. JonesJaclyn A. StephensJaclyn A. Stephens
  • Program in Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, NV, USA

The popularity of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques in basic, commercial, and applied settings grew tremendously over the last decade. Here, we focus on one popular neurostimulation method: transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Many assumptions regarding the outcomes of tDCS are based on the results of stimulating motor cortex. For instance, the primary motor cortex is predictably suppressed by cathodal tDCS or made more excitable by anodal tDCS. However, wide-ranging studies testing cognition provide more complex and sometimes paradoxical results that challenge this heuristic. Here, we first summarize successful efforts in applying tDCS to cognitive questions, with a focus on working memory (WM). These recent findings indicate that tDCS can result in cognitive task improvement or impairment regardless of stimulation site or direction of current flow. We then report WM and response inhibition studies that failed to replicate and/or extend previously reported effects. From these opposing outcomes, we present a series of factors to consider that are intended to facilitate future use of tDCS when applied to cognitive questions. In short, common pitfalls include testing too few participants, using insufficiently challenging tasks, using heterogeneous participant populations, and including poorly motivated participants. Furthermore, the poorly understood underlying mechanism for long-lasting tDCS effects make it likely that other important factors predict responses. In conclusion, we argue that although tDCS can be used experimentally to understand brain function its greatest potential may be in applied or translational research.

Introduction

Studies applying transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) are growing in frequency due to appealing safety profiles, reasonable cost, and promising findings both for investigating cognition and as a therapeutic intervention. Indeed, a coarse PubMed search combining the search terms of tDCS and publication year 2000 produced four articles, whereas the search in 2013 produced 370 references. TDCS is in wide use in clinical populations such as stroke (Fregni et al., 2005; Hummel et al., 2005; Boggio et al., 2007b; Jo et al., 2009; Kang et al., 2009; Baker et al., 2010; Lindenberg et al., 2010; Chrysikou and Hamilton, 2011; Hamilton et al., 2011), Parkinson's (Boggio et al., 2006; Fregni et al., 2006c), Alzheimer's (Boggio et al., 2011, 2012), depression (Fregni et al., 2006b; Ferrucci et al., 2009; Loo et al., 2010; Kalu et al., 2012), and chronic pain (Fregni et al., 2006a; Lefaucheur et al., 2008). It is also applied to healthy participants in cognitive domains such as working memory (WM) (Marshall et al., 2005; Ohn et al., 2008; Berryhill et al., 2010; Andrews et al., 2011; Mulquiney et al., 2011; Berryhill and Jones, 2012; Jeon and Han, 2012; Jones and Berryhill, 2012; Hoy et al., 2013), episodic memory (Ross et al., 2010, 2011; Javadi and Walsh, 2012; Javadi and Cheng, 2013), perception (Antal et al., 2001, 2003, 2004, 2006; Antal and Paulus, 2008; Bachmann et al., 2010; Bolognini et al., 2011; Borckardt et al., 2012), and motor processing (Nitsche et al., 2005, 2007; Boros et al., 2008; Hunter et al., 2009; Antal et al., 2011).

However, emerging techniques require some measure of trial and error to determine when, where, and how they are best applied. In particular, tDCS faces a number of unknowns with regard to mechanism and implementation that can make experimental design challenging. For instance, short- and long-term mechanisms of tDCS remains poorly understood. Furthermore, there is no standard stimulation protocol intensity or duration (see Nitsche et al., 2008). Thus, the pattern associated with cognitive studies using tDCS is haphazard and difficult to patch together to create a comprehensive snapshot of the literature in a particular domain. In addition, the file-drawer problem may be a particular issue with regard to tDCS. Considerable knowledge may be gained from a more complete airing of these data. The purpose of the present paper is two-fold. In Part 1, we focus on our primary research area, WM, and summarize what WM-tDCS approaches have been successful. Next, we broaden these findings slightly to cognition more generally, although patterns are less clear and the diversity of paradigms and protocols more broad. These collective findings bring several factors to light when considering the use of tDCS to study cognitive questions. In Part 2, we present several studies, both including WM components, one with a primary focus in response inhibition, in which we failed to consider one or more of these factors and failed to observe significant effects. The goal of this article is to facilitate tDCS research in cognition in healthy individuals by sharing what has worked reliably, what has failed, and what lessons we have extracted.

Part 1: tDCS Successes

What Works in Working Memory?

WM plays a significant role in many cognitive tasks and is controlled by broad frontoparietal networks accessible to tDCS. These features have made it attractive to researchers interested in applying tDCS to studies of cognition. This approach has been successful. The most consistent set of WM data comes from the use of n-back tasks paired with left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) stimulation in healthy (Ohn et al., 2008; Andrews et al., 2011; Mulquiney et al., 2011; Zaehle et al., 2011) and special populations (Boggio et al., 2006; Jo et al., 2009); for a recent meta-analysis see (Brunoni and Vanderhasselt, 2014); see Table 1. In short, these findings reveal a robust pattern: anodal tDCS to the left DLPFC improves verbal n-back task performance when compared to either sham or cathodal tDCS. This pattern of tDCS-linked WM improvement remains constant across a variety of stimulus intensities, durations, and participant populations. Indeed, recent meta-analysis looking at these studies identified a reliable reaction time improvement during active tDCS in those studies using the n-back and stimulating the DLPFC (Brunoni and Vanderhasselt, 2014, but see also Jacobson et al., 2012).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Peer-reviewed studies of WM paired with tDCS.

This consistency highlights at least one notable exception. When we tested healthy older adults in a verbal and visual 2-back task we replicated the anodal tDCS benefit, but only in those with more education (Berryhill and Jones, 2012). Surprisingly, those with less than a college-degree received no benefit from tDCS. Instead their performance revealed a nearly equal and opposite decrement in performance. Furthermore, regardless of whether the stimuli were verbal or visual and whether stimulation site were the left or the right the differences in education predicted whether tDCS helped or hurt WM performance. This was the first paper, to our knowledge, showing that an inhomogeneous population significantly modulated tDCS effects in different directions. A recent paper has used current modeling to clarify that participants only benefited on a verbal 3-back WM task when the tDCS applying DLPFC during a verbal 3-back task current modeling indicates that those who benefited showed significant modulation in the DLPFC current, but those who did not improved did not show significant DLPFC modulation (Kim et al., 2014). They attributed these data to morphological differences in brain structure and where current flow went. This finding reveals the need for refining stimulation targeting by registering an individual's MRI scans with tDCS electrode placement as is the practice in TMS research. However, their data also showed that those who started with higher WM performance garnered greater benefits from tDCS.

There are several other papers pairing tDCS with different WM tasks (e.g., old/new recognition, recall, change detection) and parietal stimulation sites. These data are less clear. For example, we found that anodal tDCS to the right posterior parietal cortex (PPC: P4) selectively interfered with WM probed by old/new recognition, but not with WM probed by recall (Berryhill et al., 2010). We subsequently found that when participants performed two WM tasks of different difficulty levels in the same session, that tDCS effects were only apparent for the more challenging task (Jones and Berryhill, 2012). Importantly, here, again, there were significant and opposing patterns in the data such that young adults with high WM span benefited but those with low WM span performance was impaired after cathodal or anodal tDCS to the right PPC. In contrast, a similar study also applying anodal tDCS to the right PPC reported that those with low WM performance on the sham day performed better during the anodal session on a challenging change detection task (Tseng et al., 2012). However, this study lacked an independent measure of WM capacity to segment their participants. Instead, behavioral performance during the sham session was used. Thus, the observed effects are contaminated by regression to the mean because poor performers during the sham session were likely to perform better at another session regardless of tDCS presence. However, here, again are several data points indicating that population differences predict the direction and magnitude of tDCS effects on WM.

A second issue buried in these data is that effects are apparent when tasks are difficult. Apart from the WM papers just noted there is at least one other analysis that has found that tDCS effects were apparent only when the task demands were difficult. More specifically, in an associative memory task participants learned face-name and place-name pairs and received left or right anodal tDCS. In younger and older adults, tDCS provided a performance benefit only when the participants struggled to produce the correct face or place name, as evidenced by long reaction times (Ross et al., 2010, 2011). One way to think about this in terms of tDCS is that the extra stimulation can serve as a tipping factor. This is consistent with our understanding that tDCS induces changes the changes in neuronal excitability—cells become more (anodal) or less (cathodal) likely to fire action potentials. When tasks are easy, the outcome is clear and the addition of tDCS does not change performance. However, when tasks or even trials are very difficult, tDCS effects emerge. When designing a task to pair with tDCS, it is worth ensuring that the task demands are sufficiently challenging for participants and/or that the more challenging trials can be isolated for separate analysis.

A third emerging issue that becomes more apparent when reviewing the tDCS-WM papers is that the effect sizes tend to be small and the studies are underpowered. For example, although early studies report significant effects with 10 participants, more recent papers tend to include 20 or more participants. One speculation is that early studies tapped homogeneous populations, presumably available lab personnel, to participate and this meant resulted in more consistent performance and tDCS patterns.

A related concern is that as laboratories become more comfortable with the tDCS technique they are subject to added noise from poorly motivated participants. Although this is a problem for many experimental techniques, it may be particularly relevant if unmotivated participants do not engage during challenging tasks or during challenging trials. As mentioned above, the subtle response-shifts induced by tDCS may be particularly sensitive to contamination from poorly motivated participants. However, this notion must be considered as speculative because there are no explicit data testing the role that motivation plays in tDCS designs, although we are currently testing this hypothesis.

What Works in Cognition?

Apart from WM, there is broad use of tDCS to investigate wide-ranging cognitive topics (for a recent review see Coffman et al., 2012). A real challenge is the diversity in experimental paradigms and tasks makes it difficult to identify consistent patterns in the tDCS literature. Furthermore, a recent review paper highlighted “foundational” problems associated with tDCS and the impact of variability across participants, issues associated with cognitive set and performance, the reliability of effects over time and current dynamics (Horvath et al., 2014; see also Lopez-Alonso et al., 2014). However, in a few areas of upper-level cognitive domains some consistency is beginning to emerge. In Table 2, we provide an admittedly incomplete survey of cognitive studies employing tDCS reporting significant effects in healthy adults. As in WM, the majority of cognitive studies target frontal stimulation sites and thus, it may not be surprising that the papers describing significant effects of tDCS relate to upper-level cognitive tasks (e.g., response inhibition, memory, decision making). From these scattered findings, it can be difficult to predict the direction of effects in a tDCS study and it can be difficult to know why the effects are as they are. As noted above, these are the studies with positive findings and likely there are many other null findings that would be informative for the research population. In the following section, we raise several points to consider when developing tDCS studies of cognition.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Peer-reviewed studies of cognitive questions in healthy adults paired with tDCS.

Considerations when Applying tDCS to Cognitive Questions

Here, we summarize some of our observations as a set of factors to consider before using tDCS in a cognitive experiment. We also provide some rationales for strategically violating these recommendations while remaining successful.

Homogeneous Populations

Equal and opposite effects in different populations can easily obscure effects. Researchers interested in relating structure-function relationships can reduce the noise in their data by targeting a homogeneous population. More practically, armed with the knowledge that population differences are pertinent it is advisable to include measures of relevant factors such as WM capacity in a WM study. This permits incorporating some demographic or other factor in the analysis. This is particularly relevant for applied and translational applications of tDCS in development for general use. For this purpose, it is essential to identify population differences and use that information to predict who will garner the greatest benefit from tDCS.

Low Power

The effect sizes in cognitive studies of tDCS are modest. Thus, it is important to counter low power by enrolling sufficiently large cohorts. Because population differences have been reported showing equal and opposite effects of tDCS, it is likely others exist. Such differences may be adding significant noise and obscuring positive findings.

Challenging Tasks

The effects of tDCS are subtle. It is unlikely that tDCS could significantly influence supraliminal response patterns. As such, it is during near-threshold events that tDCS effect become apparent—for instance, when the task is really difficult. Experimental design should include tasks that are adaptive such that all participants are performing an effortful task. Analyses should be designed to permit separation of easy trials (e.g., high accuracy, fast responses) from more difficult trials.

Poor Motivation

This issue is related to the use of challenging tasks. We suspect that tDCS is particularly sensitive to participants with low motivation. This may be a particular problem when testing freely available undergraduate volunteers who value course credit more than the research. Low motivation may matter because tDCS effects are subtle and seem to shape performance only over the range in the heart of response functions where there is variability in the response outcome. In other words, tDCS will not change someone's response when it is 100%, but it may push responses from 49% in one direction to 52%.

Part 2: Proving the Point

Below we give examples for which we have concluded our criteria were not adequately met. We offer them in the hopes that our missteps will permit others to avoid them.

Grouping Mechanisms in Visual WM

Fault: poor motivation

One area of interest for tDCS relates to improving function, in particular visual WM (VWM). Some reports suggest that VWM can benefit from Gestalt principles of grouping (e.g., proximity, similarity, connectedness, common fate) as they facilitate visual perception (Wertheimer, 1950; Palmer and Rock, 1994). Specifically, incorporating similarity, proximity, common fate, common region, or uniform connectedness improves VWM performance in change detection tasks (Xu, 2002, 2006; Woodman et al., 2003; Xu and Chun, 2007, 2009; Brady and Tenenbaum, 2013; Peterson and Berryhill, 2013; Luria and Vogel, 2014). Moreover, a recent fMRI experiment found evidence that grouped arrays were associated with lower amplitude responses in the BOLD signal corresponding to the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) during maintenance when compared to ungrouped items (Xu and Chun, 2007). The inferior parietal regions that reflect increases in set size up to VWM capacity limits (e.g., Todd and Marois, 2004, 2005; Xu and Chun, 2006) register grouped items as intermediate steps rather than as full set size increases.

Consequently, we hypothesized that tDCS targeting the right IPS would modulate grouping benefits associated with VWM performance. Our previous work had already identified VWM disruption after cathodal tDCS (1.5 mA, 10 min) to this same parietal site (Berryhill et al., 2010; Tanoue et al., 2013). Specifically, we predicted that cathodal tDCS would interrupt VWM grouping. We also anticipated that the interruption would be more pronounced in those with high WM capacity as preliminary data showed that these individuals benefited from grouping more than low WM capacity individuals. Furthermore, we had previously identified enhanced tDCS effects for challenging tasks in high WM capacity individuals (Jones and Berryhill, 2012). Thus, we anticipated the possibility of observing different patterns of effects as a function of high or low WM capacity.

Method

Participants and tDCS protocol

Thirty-three right-handed, neurologically intact graduate and undergraduate students with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated in the current experiment (Mean age = 21.3, 25 female) and received $15 per hour. On separate days, participants completed the VWM task after sham or cathodal tDCS (right posterior parietal scalp site: P4, 10 min, 1.5 mA; Eldith Magstim GMbH, Ilmenau, Germany). During the sham session, current was ramped up and down for the first and last 20 s of the stimulation interval to mimic sensations associated with current change. The anode was placed on the contralateral cheek. Session order was counterbalanced across participants. Active/sham stimulation occurred prior to the task, while participants completed practice trials. Electrodes were removed prior to the start of the task. All experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Nevada.

Stimuli

The task design slightly modified a paradigm previously published that showed significant VWM grouping benefits (Xu and Chun, 2007). We summarize these methods here. Stimuli were gray, symmetrical novel shapes (2.6 × 2.6° visual angle) presented on black rectangles (6.9 × 18° of visual angle) against a gray background. There were three experimental conditions varying the stimulus grouping: 2-ungrouped (2 shapes in separate black rectangles), 3-ungrouped (3 shapes in separate black rectangles), and 3-grouped (2 shapes in one black rectangle and one in a separate rectangle). The stimuli were presented at 57 cm using EPrime® (Psychology Software Tools, Sharpsburg, PA) software running on a Dell © desktop computer and were presented on a 20.5″ by 13″ widescreen monitor running at a refresh rate of 60 Hz.

Procedure

To insure focused attention, trials began with a fixation task (1000 ms), in which participants viewed a rapidly changing shape (e.g., triangle, circle, square, diamond, 200 ms/item) and were required to make a key press response each time the diamond shape appeared; see Figure 1. Next, the stimulus display of the VWM task appeared (200 ms), followed by a delay (1000 ms), and finally a single probe item (2500 ms). Participants were instructed to press the “o” key if the probe item had been shown in the same location during the stimulus array (50%) and the “n” key if the probed item changed from sample to test. A feedback display followed (1300 ms: “Correct” in blue font, “Incorrect” or “No Response Detected” in red font). Participants completed 15 practice trials to familiarize themselves with the task prior to beginning the experiment and completed 150 experimental trials (50 trials per condition). During the sham session, the forward and backward digit spans were administered as an independent measure of WM capacity (e.g., combined digit span score; WAIS-IV, Wechsler, 2008). The experimental task lasted approximately 20 min.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experiment 1 task design. After 10 min of sham or 1.0 mA anodal tDCS to the right PPC (P4), the experimental task began. Participants viewed three types of arrays: 2 items (2-ungrouped), 3 items in two boxes (3-grouped), or 3 items in three boxes (3-ungrouped). After a WM delay period (1000 ms), a single probe item appeared and participants indicated whether that stimulus had previously appeared in that location.

Results

For each condition and each participant we estimated WM capacity by calculating Cowan's K: [K = set size* (hit rate—false alarm rate)]. The first question was to determine whether cathodal tDCS to the right parietal lobe interfered with WM grouping mechanisms. The K-values were subjected to repeated-measures ANOVA with the factors of condition [2-ungrouped (2-UG), 3-grouped (3-G), 3-ungrouped (3-UG)] and tDCS session (active, sham). The data replicated the expected behavioral grouping benefit [F(2, 64) = 5.39, p = 0.007, η2p = 0.14, β = 0.83] such that capacity was significantly larger in the 3-G condition compared to the 2-UG condition (3-G = 1.49, 2-UG = 1.29, p = 0.004, Bonferroni corrected here and throughout); see Figure 2. Additional pairwise comparisons indicated that there were no differences between the 2-ungrouped and 3-ungrouped conditions (3-UG = 1.38, p = 0.26) or between the 3-ungrouped and 3-grouped conditions (p = 0.44). In other words, grouping numerically enhanced performance on the 3-grouped condition, but not significantly. However, the answer to the primary question was that there was no significant effect of tDCS on VWM performance as there was no main effect of tDCS [F(1, 32) = 0.07, p = 0.79] and no significant condition × tDCS interaction [F(2, 64) = 0.18, p = 0.83].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. VWM performance in Experiment 1. The three experimental conditions are displayed along the abscissa, while estimated capacity (based on Cowan's K formula, 2001) is depicted along the ordinate. The error bars represent the standard errors of the means in each condition for each tDCS session.

Our concerns regarding participant homogeneity prompted dividing participants into high and low WM capacity groups based on the independent WM measure of combined forward + backward digit span scores (high N = 16: combined digit span score >13; low N = 17: combined digit span score <13). The inclusion of WM capacity in the ANOVA did not change the pattern of the results. The grouping benefit remained [F(2, 62) = 5.26, p = 0.008, η2p = 0.15, β = 0.82]. As before, pairwise comparisons reveled significant differences between the 2-ungrouped and the 3-grouped condition (p = 0.005), but not between the 2-ungrouped and the 3-ungrouped conditions (p = 0.26) or between the 3-grouped and the 3-ungrouped conditions (p = 0.47). As before, there was no main effect of tDCS [F(1, 31) = 0.05, p = 0.83], and no two- or three-way interactions (all p's > 0.13). In summary, there was no difference in VWM performance in response to grouping via common region between high and low WM capacity individuals. Additionally, diverging from our predictions, there was no effect of cathodal tDCS relative to sham tDCS.

Discussion

Here, we tested the hypothesis that cathodal tDCS targeting parietal regions involved in grouping processes would disrupt these processes. Contrary to our predictions, tDCS did not modulate grouping benefits in VWM. Furthermore, WM capacity did not contribute to the current results as cathodal tDCS failed to interrupt grouping benefits to WM performance in either high or low WM capacity participants. These findings were unexpected because we previously interrupted WM using cathodal tDCS to the right parietal lobe in a VWM recognition task (Berryhill et al., 2010). One possibility to account for these null results is that participants may not have been effortfully engaged in the task. This interpretation is supported by low VWM performance (e.g., mean K-values for the 2-UG, 3-UG, 3-G conditions: 1.29, 1.38, 1.49 items compared to the ~1.9, 2.3, 2.6 items per condition reported by Xu and Chun, 2007). Anecdotally, in our previous successful study, the participants were largely graduate students known to the experimenter, rather than undergraduates interested in obtaining course extra-credit.

Alternatively, previous fMRI findings revealed bilateral IPS activity during this task (Xu and Chun, 2007). Thus, some might argue that a unilateral stimulation protocol might not have sufficient power to prevent some contralateral compensatory mechanism. However, we think that this is unlikely for several reasons. First, there is evidence supporting right IPL in attending to stimuli across both visual hemifields (e.g., Sheremata et al., 2010; Szczepanski and Kastner, 2013), making it more likely to see disrupted performance in the VWM task after right lateralized stimulation. Second, and perhaps of greater relevance, we previously interrupted VWM using the identical tDCS protocol but different VWM tasks (Berryhill et al., 2010; Tanoue et al., 2013). Thus, we suspect that participants' engagement was the most important factor in this particular experiment.

Reducing ADHD Impulsivity

Faults: low power, heterogeneous population, low task difficulty

The familiar symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) include impulsivity, restlessness, and difficulty concentrating (Faraone and Biederman, 2005). Recent findings suggest that there is abnormal brain structure and function in the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) in ADHD. When people with ADHD perform tasks requiring response inhibition they have smaller activations in the pre-SMA (Mulligan et al., 2011). These findings suggest that the pre-SMA is not sufficiently activated during response inhibition tasks. Recently, in healthy adults response inhibition was predictably modulated by tDCS to the pre-SMA: anodal improved performance whereas cathodal tDCS impaired performance (Hsu et al., 2011). Other reports show tDCS-linked improvement in response inhibition tasks in those with major depressive disorder (Boggio et al., 2007a) and stroke (Kang et al., 2009). Thus, we tested whether directing anodal tDCS to the pre-SMA would modulate performance in a response inhibition task, the Go/No-Go task. First, based on the logic just described, we anticipated that anodal tDCS would improve Go/No-Go task performance in young adults with low or high ADHD symptomology, and thereby replicating the Hsu et al. findings in healthy young adults using the stop-signal task (Hsu et al., 2011). We anticipated an interaction such that those with high symptomology would garner greater tDCS benefits than the low symptomology group. We also completed two WM tasks: the operation span task and a spatial n-back task. These tasks were included to clarify the specificity of tDCS influences. Both WM tasks engage frontoparietal networks, but were not expected to show modulation by tDCS to the pre-SMA. Here, we tested an unmedicated undergraduate population to look at ADHD symptomology because we do not apply tDCS to people taking stimulants or anti-depressants (e.g., those prescribed for ADHD).

Method

The University Institutional Review Board approved all protocols. Volunteers completed the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS—v1.1; Kessler et al., 2005). This short screen was developed to identify ADHD symptomology in adults and has been validated in adult (Adler et al., 2006) and college-aged (Fuller-Killgore et al., 2013) populations. Scores were derived from the six-screener questions in Part A of the ASRS, the most predictive of ADHD. Questions probed the frequency with which participants forgot appointments, completed tasks, or felt distracted Responses were converted from verbal labels (e.g., “never,” “sometimes,” “rarely,” “often,” “very often”) to point values (1–5). Questions 1–3 required doubling the point value when answers of “sometimes,” “often,” or “very often” were recorded; questions 4–6 required doubling the point value when answers “often” or “very often” were recorded. Four or more answers requiring doubling meet the criteria for “high likelihood” of ADHD (Kessler et al., 2005). To identify participants we recruited participants high (ASRS scores 39–58) and low (ASRS scores <19) in ADHD symptomology. This high ADHD group met the criteria of high likelihood of ADHD. Thirty-six right-handed, normal, neurotypical participants were subsequently enrolled (age 18–37, 14 male). Participants were screened to ensure they were not taking medications that modulate the excitability of the brain (e.g., stimulants). Participants completed anodal, cathodal and sham tDCS sessions, in counterbalanced order across 3 separate days with a minimum washout period of 24 h. Current was administered using a commercial stimulator (Eldith Magstim GMbH, Ilmenau, Germany). To target the right pre-SMA, we placed the electrode 2 cm to the right of FZ (10–20 system) and the reference electrode was placed on the contralateral cheek. Participants received 10 min of 1.5 mA tDCS during anodal and cathodal sessions and during sham stimulation current was ramped up and down for the initial and final 20 s of the period. Participants completed practice trials of each task during stimulation and after the electrodes were removed, they began the experimental trials of the tasks described below.

Behavioral tasks

Go/No-Go Task. In this measure of response inhibition and impulsivity participants view a letter stream (400 ms) and respond when they see a target letter (“x” or “y”) unless the same target preceded the current target letter. In other words, in the sequence “x-t-c-b-x-g-y,” the first “x” and the “y” require responses, but the second “x” requires response inhibition. One to six distractor letters were presented between each target letter. There were 440 target letter trials and the task lasted approximately 12 min.

Automated Operation Span (OSpan). The OSpan requires participants to solve arithmetic problems while also maintaining letters in WM (Unsworth et al., 2005). Participants report remembered letters after completing arithmetic problems. The task consisted of 9 sets of letters and arithmetic problems and lasted approximately 10 min.

Spatial 2-back. Participants completed a spatial 2-back WM task in which a green circle (4.8°, 500 ms, ITI: 1500 ms) was presented in 9 possible locations. Participants reported whether the current item matched what was shown two-items previously via button press (match: “j,” non-match: “f”). Participants completed 450 trials (150 match; 300 non-match), which lasted approximately 15 min.

Results

Performance on each task was subjected to a 2 ADHD group (low, high symptomology) × 3 tDCS condition (sham, anodal, cathodal) repeated measures ANOVA. Performance accuracy on the Go/No-Go task revealed no significant main effect of group [F(1, 17) = 1.01, p = 0.33], no main effect of tDCS [F(2, 34) = 0.996, p = 0.38], and no interaction [F(2, 34) = 1.145, p = 0.33]; see Figure 3A. The reaction time data were also analyzed and they followed the same pattern of null results {group [F(1, 17) = 0.246, p = 0.626]; tDCS [F(2, 34) = 0.060, p = 0.942], interaction [F(2, 34) = 1.643, p = 0.208]}. Four participants were eliminated from the OSPAN and 2-back WM tasks because they pressed the same button for all responses. Performance on the OSPAN task followed the same pattern, with no significant main effects of group [F(1, 15) = 1.635, p = 0.22], or tDCS [F(2, 30) = 0.151, p = 0.860], and no interaction [F(2, 30) = 0.213, p = 0.810]; see Figure 3B. This pattern was also true for performance accuracy on the spatial 2-back WM task. There were no main effects of group [F(1, 15) = 0.015, p = 0.904] or tDCS [F(2, 30) = 0.466, p = 0.632] and no significant interaction [F(2, 30) = 0.750, p = 0.481]; see Figure 3C.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Experiment 2 performance per task as a function of tDCS condition and ADHD symptomology: (A) Go/No-Go, (B) OSPAN, and (C) Spatial 2-back task. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.

Discussion

Here, we failed to replicate an anodal tDCS benefit on a response inhibition task performance after stimulation to the pre-SMA (Hsu et al., 2011), and to extend it into those with high/low ADHD symptomology using the go/no-go task. Others have had luck with response inhibition and tDCS, particularly in suppressing responses (reviewed in Juan and Muggleton, 2012). We may have tapped a particularly heterogeneous population and used an insufficiently difficult response inhibition task that did not allow us to discriminate between easy and challenging trials. We may have selected a stimulation site that was too lateralized and an intensity that did not effectively reach the pre-SMA. These issues may have clouded tDCS effects. Future studies should include a more difficult response inhibition task, as participants approached ceiling. In the Hsu et al. (2011) paper, the stop-signal task they employed required more finely titrating the timing of the stop signal to ensure that the task difficulty was tailored to the participant. Finally, because the participants completed a self-assessment rather than complete a clinical interview and ADHD diagnosis, a different pattern of results may emerge in a clinically diagnosed ADHD population.

In the two WM tasks, the OSPAN and spatial 2-back tasks, likewise, we did not observe any significant modulation after tDCS. These later results were expected, but difficult to interpret as site-specific tDCS effects given the null findings in the primary task of interest, the go/no-go task.

General Discussion

Neurostimulation via tDCS is a useful tool for investigating aspects of cognition. As with any tool there are some things that work better than others, and a set of appropriate situations for a given tool. Here, we have summarized some findings demonstrating successful applications of tDCS in studies of WM (see Table 1) and other cognitive modalities (see Table 2). Although these studies all report positive findings there is still considerable variability in terms of the pattern of effects, paradigms used and tDCS parameters. For instance, stimulus intensity, duration, tDCS electrode montage are inconsistent. The most consistent pattern in the published literature has been to report significant improvements in WM tested in verbal n-back tasks and anodal tDCS to the left DLPFC. In other cognitive realms a patchwork of findings is emerging revealing consistent effects in memory, deception, and cognitive control. However, there are exceptions and forays into different tasks, populations, and parameters have produced different patterns of results. We also note that the proliferation of neurostimulation effects (e.g., tACS, TRNS) will be certain to raise additional issues to maximize their experimental and applied usefulness (for a recent review see Kuo and Nitsche, 2012; see also Snowball et al., 2013).

We think the file-drawer problem is a particular challenge here. We offered several of our own missteps and several factors to consider when applying tDCS to cognitive questions. We believe that appropriate consideration of these factors will facilitate future experimental design and serve to increase interpretable outcomes. Ideally, the following is available: a homogenous, highly motivated, large population engaging in a challenging cognitive task that permits selective identification and analysis of the most difficult trials. When one or more of these factors is overlooked there may be little to report.

What do These Considerations Reveal about tDCS?

The considerations we noted above point us toward an emerging research question. Why do group differences matter? The underlying mechanism of tDCS remains unclear, although long-term neuroplasticity is indicated (Rosenkranz et al., 2000; Nitsche et al., 2003). Some pharmacological work in humans indicates that blocking sodium or calcium channels prevents longer-lasting effects of anodal tDCS and antagonizing NMDA receptors prevents longer-lasting effects of anodal or cathodal tDCS, at least in motor cortex (Nitsche et al., 2003). Furthermore, at a larger scale, the way current flows through a particular individual's brain certainly varies and has tremendous implications for applied and experimental use of tDCS (Bikson et al., 2013). Incorporating other variables, including the structural and functional connectivity of stimulated networks and their temporal dynamics will also improve experimental success when applying tDCS. We suspect that the answers to these questions will also clarify why some groups benefit more than others and why homogeneous groups provide more consistent data. For an individualized approach to tDCS application, the underlying molecular mechanism must be fully clarified and this in turn will advance our understanding at the network level. At this point, another way to counter these differences is simply through brute power and by running sufficient participants.

The role of task difficulty is important. As noted in the introduction, when a participant knows the answer definitively tDCS is unlikely to have an effect. This means tasks that are too easy do not show any effect of tDCS. It is only in the challenging, borderline cases where tDCS serves as a tipping factor. As we age, we encounter more of these near-threshold cases and this population may be the one to benefit most from tDCS-linked cognitive interventions. In particular, in the realm of WM, small improvements can benefit quality of life and are worth the effort to gain them.

Conclusions

tDCS is a safe, affordable neurostimulation technique that is well-tolerated in healthy and special populations. Regular tDCS is not well-positioned to target carefully defined cortical regions for studies of structure-function relationships. The current article provided several considerations for review when preparing a tDCS study. Because tDCS provides the greatest effect at threshold level decisions, challenging tasks are necessary. Participants must be sufficiently engaged and motivated. Finally, the nature of the participants also should be carefully considered as group differences can predict nearly equal and opposite responses to tDCS. Furthermore, whereas early tDCS studies often targeted motor and visual regions, studies of cognition require sufficient power and larger numbers of participants. When these considerations were sufficiently weighed, we have succeeded. We offer several cautionary tales detailing our failed efforts when these considerations were ignored. Finally, in conclusion, the best use of tDCS may well be in translational and applied settings. In this arena, tDCS already shows promise as a way to maintain and/or restore cognitive function across various populations. Clearly, there is great interest in effective cognitive interventions as the aging population grows and tDCS may be a part of the answer.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by an Institutional Development Award (IDeA) from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the NIH P20GM103650 (PI Michael Webster, Project Leader Marian Berryhill), and NEI R15EY022775 (to Marian Berryhill and Gideon Caplovitz). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not represent the official views of the NIH, the NIGMS, or the NEI.

References

Adler, L. A., Spencer, T., Faraone, S. V., Kessler, R. C., Howes, M. J., Biederman, J., et al. (2006). Validity of pilot Adult ADHD Self- Report Scale (ASRS) to rate adult ADHD symptoms. Ann. Clin. psychiatry 18, 145–148. doi: 10.1080/10401230600801077

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Andrews, S. C., Hoy, K. E., Enticott, P. G., Daskalakis, Z. J., and Fitzgerald, P. B. (2011). Improving working memory: the effect of combining cognitive activity and anodal transcranial direct current stimulation to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Brain Stimul. 4, 84–89. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2010.06.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Antal, A., Kincses, T. Z., Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2003). Modulation of moving phosphene thresholds by transcranial direct current stimulation of V1 in human. Neuropsychologia 41, 1802–1807. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00181-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Antal, A., Nitsche, M. A., Kruse, W., Kincses, T. Z., Hoffmann, K. P., and Paulus, W. (2004). Direct current stimulation over V5 enhances visuomotor coordination by improving motion perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 521–527. doi: 10.1162/089892904323057263

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Antal, A., Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2001). External modulation of visual perception in humans. Neuroreport 12, 3553–3555. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200111160-00036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Antal, A., Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2006). Transcranial direct current stimulation and the visual cortex. Brain Res. Bull. 68, 459–463. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.10.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation and visual perception. Perception 37, 367–374. doi: 10.1068/p5872

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Antal, A., Polania, R., Schmidt-Samoa, C., Dechent, P., and Paulus, W. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation over the primary motor cortex during fMRI. Neuroimage 55, 590–596. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.085

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bachmann, C. G., Muschinsky, S., Nitsche, M. A., Rolke, R., Magerl, W., Treede, R. D., et al. (2010). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor cortex induces distinct changes in thermal and mechanical sensory percepts. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 2083–2089. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baker, J. M., Rorden, C., and Fridriksson, J. (2010). Using transcranial direct-current stimulation to treat stroke patients with aphasia. Stroke 41, 1229–1236. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.576785

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berryhill, M. E., and Jones, K. T. (2012). tDCS selectively improves working memory in older adults with more education. Neurosci. Lett. 521, 148–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.05.074

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berryhill, M. E., Wencil, E. B., Coslett, H. B., and Olson, I. R. (2010). A selective working memory impairment after transcranial direct current stimulation to the right parietal lobe. Neurosci. Lett. 479, 312–316. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bikson, M., Name, A., and Rahman, A. (2013). Origins of specificity during tDCS: anatomical, activity-selective, and input-bias mechanisms. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:688. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00688

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Bermpohl, F., Vergara, A. O., Muniz, A. L., Nahas, F. H., Leme, P. B., et al. (2007a). Go-no-go task performance improvement after anodal transcranial DC stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 101, 91–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.10.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Campanha, C., Valasek, C. A., Fecteau, S., Pascual-Leone, A., and Fregni, F. (2010). Modulation of decision-making in a gambling task in older adults with transcranial direct current stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 593–597. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07080.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Ferrucci, R., Mameli, F., Martins, D., Martins, O., Vergari, M., et al. (2012). Prolonged visual memory enhancement after direct current stimulation in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Stimul. 5, 223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.06.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Ferrucci, R., Rigonatti, S. P., Covre, P., Nitsche, M., Pascual-Leone, A., et al. (2006). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory in patients with Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 249, 31–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2006.05.062

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Nunes, A., Rigonatti, S. P., Nitsche, M. A., Pascual-Leone, A., and Fregni, F. (2007b). Repeated sessions of noninvasive brain DC stimulation is associated with motor function improvement in stroke patients. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 25, 123–129.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Rocha, R. R., Da Silva, M. T., and Fregni, F. (2008). Differential modulatory effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on a facial expression go-no-go task in males and females. Neurosci. Lett. 447, 101–105. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.10.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Valasek, C. A., Campanha, C., Giglio, A. C., Baptista, N. I., Lapenta, O. M., et al. (2011). Non-invasive brain stimulation to assess and modulate neuroplasticity in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 21, 703–716. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2011.617943

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Zaghi, S., and Fregni, F. (2009). Modulation of emotions associated with images of human pain using anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuropsychologia 47, 212–217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bolognini, N., Fregni, F., Casati, C., Olgiati, E., and Vallar, G. (2010). Brain polarization of parietal cortex augments training-induced improvement of visual exploratory and attentional skills. Brain Res. 1349, 76–89. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.053

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bolognini, N., Rossetti, A., Casati, C., Mancini, F., and Vallar, G. (2011). Neuromodulation of multisensory perception: a tDCS study of the sound-induced flash illusion. Neuropsychologia 49, 231–237. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Borckardt, J. J., Bikson, M., Frohman, H., Reeves, S. T., Datta, A., Bansal, V., et al. (2012). A pilot study of the tolerability and effects of high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS) on pain perception. J. Pain 13, 112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2011.07.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boros, K., Poreisz, C., Munchau, A., Paulus, W., and Nitsche, M. A. (2008). Premotor transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) affects primary motor excitability in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 1292–1300. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06090.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brady, T. F., and Tenenbaum, J. B. (2013). A probabilistic model of visual working memory: incorporating higher order regularities into working memory capacity estimates. Psychol. Rev. 120, 85–109. doi: 10.1037/a0030779

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brunoni, A. R., and Vanderhasselt, M. A. (2014). Working memory improvement with non-invasive brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Cogn. 86C, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cerruti, C., and Schlaug, G. (2009). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex enhances complex verbal associative thought. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1980–1987. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21143

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chi, R. P., Fregni, F., and Snyder, A. W. (2010). Visual memory improved by non-invasive brain stimulation. Brain Res. 1353, 168–175. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.07.062

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chrysikou, E. G., and Hamilton, R. H. (2011). Noninvasive brain stimulation in the treatment of aphasia: exploring interhemispheric relationships and their implications for neurorehabilitation. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 29, 375–394. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2011-0610

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coffman, B. A., Trumbo, M. C., Flores, R. A., Garcia, C. M., Van Der Merwe, A. J., Wassermann, E. M., et al. (2012). Impact of tDCS on performance and learning of target detection: interaction with stimulus characteristics and experimental design. Neuropsychologia 50, 1594–1602. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.03.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

De Vries, M. H., Barth, A. C., Maiworm, S., Knecht, S., Zwitserlood, P., and Floel, A. (2010). Electrical stimulation of Broca's area enhances implicit learning of an artificial grammar. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2427–2436. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21385

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Faraone, S. V., and Biederman, J. (2005). What is the prevalence of adult ADHD? results of a population screen of 966 adults. J. Atten. Disord. 9, 384–391. doi: 10.1177/1087054705281478

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fecteau, S., Boggio, P., Fregni, F., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2012). Modulation of untruthful responses with non-invasive brain stimulation. Front. Psychiatry 3:97. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00097

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fecteau, S., Pascual-Leone, A., Zald, D. H., Liguori, P., Theoret, H., Boggio, P. S., et al. (2007). Activation of prefrontal cortex by transcranial direct current stimulation reduces appetite for risk during ambiguous decision making. J. Neurosci. 27, 6212–6218. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0314-07.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferrucci, R., Bortolomasi, M., Vergari, M., Tadini, L., Salvoro, B., Giacopuzzi, M., et al. (2009). Transcranial direct current stimulation in severe, drug-resistant major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 118, 215–219. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.02.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferrucci, R., Mameli, F., Guidi, I., Mrakic-Sposta, S., Vergari, M., Marceglia, S., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation improves recognition memory in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 71, 493–498. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000317060.43722.a3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Floel, A., Rosser, N., Michka, O., Knecht, S., and Breitenstein, C. (2008). Noninvasive brain stimulation improves language learning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1415–1422. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20098

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., Lima, M. C., Ferreira, M. J., Wagner, T., Rigonatti, S. P., et al. (2006a). A sham-controlled, phase II trial of transcranial direct current stimulation for the treatment of central pain in traumatic spinal cord injury. Pain 122, 197–209. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.02.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., Mansur, C. G., Wagner, T., Ferreira, M. J., Lima, M. C., et al. (2005). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere in stroke patients. Neuroreport 16, 1551–1555. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000177010.44602.5e

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., Nitsche, M. A., Rigonatti, S. P., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2006b). Cognitive effects of repeated sessions of transcranial direct current stimulation in patients with depression. Depress. Anxiety 23, 482–484. doi: 10.1002/da.20201.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., Santos, M. C., Lima, M., Vieira, A. L., Rigonatti, S. P., et al. (2006c). Noninvasive cortical stimulation with transcranial direct current stimulation in Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 21, 1693–1702. doi: 10.1002/mds.21012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fuller-Killgore, M. D., Burlison, J., and Dwyer, W. (2013). Comparison of three ADHD screening instruments in college students of varying cognitive ability. J. Atten. Disord. 17, 449–454. doi: 10.1177/1087054712438136

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hamilton, R. H., Chrysikou, E. G., and Coslett, B. (2011). Mechanisms of aphasia recovery after stroke and the role of noninvasive brain stimulation. Brain Lang. 118, 40–50. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.02.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Horvath, J. C., Carter, O., and Forte, J. D. (2014). Transcranial direct current stimulation: five important issues we aren't discussing (but probably should be). Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:2. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hoy, K. E., Emonson, M. R., Arnold, S. L., Thomson, R. H., Daskalakis, Z. J., and Fitzgerald, P. B. (2013). Testing the limits: investigating the effect of tDCS dose on working memory enhancement in healthy controls. Neuropsychologia 51, 1777–1784. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.05.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hsu, T. Y., Tseng, L. Y., Yu, J. X., Kuo, W. J., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J., et al. (2011). Modulating inhibitory control with direct current stimulation of the superior medial frontal cortex. Neuroimage 56, 2249–2257. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.059

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hummel, F., Celnik, P., Giraux, P., Floel, A., Wu, W. H., Gerloff, C., et al. (2005). Effects of non-invasive cortical stimulation on skilled motor function in chronic stroke. Brain 128, 490–499. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh369

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hunter, T., Sacco, P., Nitsche, M. A., and Turner, D. L. (2009). Modulation of internal model formation during force field-induced motor learning by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of primary motor cortex. J. Physiol. 587, 2949–2961. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.169284

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Iyer, M. B., Mattu, U., Grafman, J., Lomarev, M., Sato, S., and Wassermann, E. M. (2005). Safety and cognitive effect of frontal DC brain polarization in healthy individuals. Neurology 64, 872–875. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000152986.07469.E9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jacobson, L., Javitt, D. C., and Lavidor, M. (2011). Activation of inhibition: diminishing impulsive behavior by direct current stimulation over the inferior frontal gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3380–3387. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jacobson, L., Koslowsky, M., and Lavidor, M. (2012). tDCS polarity effects in motor and cognitive domains: a meta-analytical review. Exp. Brain Res. 216, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2891-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Javadi, A. H., and Cheng, P. (2013). Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) enhances reconsolidation of long-term memory. Brain Stimul. 6, 668–674. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.10.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Javadi, A. H., and Walsh, V. (2012). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex modulates declarative memory. Brain Stimul. 5, 231–241. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.06.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jeon, S. Y., and Han, S. J. (2012). Improvement of the working memory and naming by transcranial direct current stimulation. Ann. Rehabil. Med. 36, 585–595. doi: 10.5535/arm.2012.36.5.585

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jo, J. M., Kim, Y.-H., Ko, M.-H., Ohn, S. H., Joen, B., and Lee, K. H. (2009). Enhancing the working memory of stroke patients using tDCS. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 88, 404–409. doi: 10.1097/PHM.0b013e3181a0e4cb

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jones, K. T., and Berryhill, M. E. (2012). Parietal contributions to visual working memory depend on task difficulty. Front. Psychiatry 3:81. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00081

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Juan, C. H., and Muggleton, N. G. (2012). Brain stimulation and inhibitory control. Brain Stimul. 5, 63–69. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kalu, U. G., Sexton, C. E., Loo, C. K., and Ebmeier, K. P. (2012). Transcranial direct current stimulation in the treatment of major depression: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 42, 1791–1800. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711003059

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kang, E. K., Baek, M. J., Kim, S., and Paik, N. J. (2009). Non-invasive cortical stimulation improves post-stroke attention decline. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 27, 645–650. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2009-0514

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Karim, A. A., Schneider, M., Lotze, M., Veit, R., Sauseng, P., Braun, C., et al. (2010). The truth about lying: inhibition of the anterior prefrontal cortex improves deceptive behavior. Cereb. Cortex 20, 205–213. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp090

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Ames, M., Demler, O., Faraone, S., Hiripi, E., et al. (2005). The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol. Med. 35, 245–256. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002892

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, J. H., Kim, D. W., Chang, W. H., Kim, Y. H., Kim, K., and Im, C. H. (2014). Inconsistent outcomes of transcranial direct current stimulation may originate from anatomical differences among individuals: electric field simulation using individual MRI data. Neurosci. Lett. 564, 6–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.01.054

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kincses, T. Z., Antal, A., Nitsche, M. A., Bartfai, O., and Paulus, W. (2004). Facilitation of probabilistic classification learning by transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex in the human. Neuropsychologia 42, 113–117. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00124-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Knoch, D., Nitsche, M. A., Fischbacher, U., Eisenegger, C., Pascual-Leone, A., and Fehr, E. (2008). Studying the neurobiology of social interaction with transcranial direct current stimulation–the example of punishing unfairness. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1987–1990. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm237

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kuo, M. F., and Nitsche, M. A. (2012). Effects of transcranial electrical stimulation on cognition. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 43, 192–199. doi: 10.1177/1550059412444975

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lefaucheur, J. P., Antal, A., Ahdab, R., Ciampi De Andrade, D., Fregni, F., Khedr, E. M., et al. (2008). The use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to relieve pain. Brain Stimul. 1, 337–344. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.07.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lindenberg, R., Renga, V., Zhu, L. L., Nair, D., and Schlaug, G. (2010). Bihemispheric brain stimulation facilitates motor recovery in chronic stroke patients. Neurology 75, 2176–2184. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318202013a

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Loo, C. K., Sachdev, P., Martin, D., Pigot, M., Alonzo, A., Malhi, G. S., et al. (2010). A double-blind, sham-controlled trial of transcranial direct current stimulation for the treatment of depression. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 13, 61–69. doi: 10.1017/S1461145709990411

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lopez-Alonso, V., Cheeran, B., Rio-Rodriguez, D., and Fernandez-Del-Olmo, M. (2014). Inter-individual variability in response to non-invasive brain stimulation paradigms. Brain Stimul. 7, 372–380. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.02.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Luria, R., and Vogel, E. K. (2014). Come together, right now: dynamic overwriting of an object's history through common fate. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 1819–1828. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00584

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mameli, F., Mrakic-Sposta, S., Vergari, M., Fumagalli, M., Macis, M., Ferrucci, R., et al. (2010). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex specifically processes general - but not personal - knowledge deception: multiple brain networks for lying. Behav. Brain Res. 211, 164–168. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.03.024

CrossRef Full Text

Marshall, L., Molle, M., Hallschmid, M., and Born, J. (2004). Transcranial direct current stimulation during sleep improves declarative memory. J. Neurosci. 24, 9985–9992. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2725-04.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marshall, L., Molle, M., Siebner, H. R., and Born, J. (2005). Bifrontal transcranial direct current stimulation slows reaction time in a working memory task. BMC Neurosci. 6:23. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-6-23

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mulligan, R. C., Knopik, V. S., Sweet, L. H., Fischer, M., Seidenberg, M., and Rao, S. M. (2011). Neural correlates of inhibitory control in adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence from the Milwaukee longitudinal sample. Psychiatry Res. 194, 119–129. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mulquiney, P. G., Hoy, K. E., Daskalakis, Z. J., and Fitzgerald, P. B. (2011). Improving working memory: exploring the effect of transcranial random noise stimulation and transcranial direct current stimulation on the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 2384–2389. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.05.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mungee, A., Kazzer, P., Feeser, M., Nitsche, M. A., Schiller, D., and Bajbouj, M. (2014). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex: a means to modulate fear memories. Neuroreport 25, 480–484. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000119

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Fricke, K., Henschke, U., Schlitterlau, A., Liebetanz, D., Lang, N., et al. (2003). Pharmacological modulation of cortical excitability shifts induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in humans. J. Physiol. 553(Pt 1), 293–301. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.049916

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 1, 206–223. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Roth, A., Kuo, M. F., Fischer, A. K., Liebetanz, D., Lang, N., et al. (2007). Timing-dependent modulation of associative plasticity by general network excitability in the human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 27, 3807–3812. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5348-06.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Seeber, A., Frommann, K., Klein, C. C., Rochford, C., Nitsche, M. S., et al. (2005). Modulating parameters of excitability during and after transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 568, 291–303. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.092429

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ohn, S. H., Park, C. I., Yoo, W. K., Ko, M. H., Choi, K. P., Kim, G. M., et al. (2008). Time-dependent effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on the enhancement of working memory. Neuroreport 19, 43–47. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f2adfd

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Palmer, S., and Rock, I. (1994). Rethinking perceptual organization: the role of uniform connectedness. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1, 29–55. doi: 10.3758/BF03200760

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Penolazzi, B., Stramaccia, D. F., Braga, M., Mondini, S., and Galfano, G. (2014). Human memory retrieval and inhibitory control in the brain: beyond correlational evidence. J. Neurosci. 34, 6606–6610. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0349-14.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Peterson, D. J., and Berryhill, M. E. (2013). The Gestalt principle of similarity benefits visual working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 1282–1289. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0460-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rosenkranz, K., Nitsche, M. A., Tergau, F., and Paulus, W. (2000). Diminution of training-induced transient motor cortex plasticity by weak transcranial direct current stimulation in the human. Neurosci. Lett. 296, 61–63. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01621-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ross, L. A., Mccoy, D., Coslett, H. B., Olson, I. R., and Wolk, D. A. (2011). Improved proper name recall in aging after electrical stimulation of the anterior temporal lobes. Front. Aging Neurosci. 3:16. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2011.00016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ross, L. A., Mccoy, D., Wolk, D. A., Coslett, H. B., and Olson, I. R. (2010). Improved proper name recall by electrical stimulation of the anterior temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia 48, 3671–3674. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sheremata, S. L., Bettencourt, K. C., and Somers, D. C. (2010). Hemispheric asymmetry in visuotopic posterior parietal cortex emerges with visual short-term memory load. J. Neurosci. 30, 12581–12588. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2689-10.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Snowball, A., Tachtsidis, I., Popescu, T., Thompson, J., Delazer, M., Zamarian, L., et al. (2013). Long-term enhancement of brain function and cognition using cognitive training and brain stimulation. Curr. Biol. 23, 987–992. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.045

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stone, D. B., and Tesche, C. D. (2009). Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention. Neuroreport 20, 1115–1119. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832e9aa2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Szczepanski, S. M., and Kastner, S. (2013). Shifting attentional priorities: control of spatial attention through hemispheric competition. J. Neurosci. 33, 5411–5421. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4089-12.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tanoue, R. T., Jones, K. T., Peterson, D. J., and Berryhill, M. E. (2013). Differential frontal involvement in shifts of internal and perceptual attention. Brain Stimul. 6, 675–682. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.11.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Todd, J. J., and Marois, R. (2004). Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature 428, 751–754. doi: 10.1038/nature02466

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Todd, J. J., and Marois, R. (2005). Posterior parietal cortex activity predicts individual differences in visual short-term memory capacity. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 144–155. doi: 10.3758/CABN.5.2.144

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tseng, P., Hsu, T. Y., Chang, C. F., Tzeng, O. J., Hung, D. L., Muggleton, N. G., et al. (2012). Unleashing potential: transcranial direct current stimulation over the right posterior parietal cortex improves change detection in low-performing individuals. J. Neurosci. 32, 10554–10561. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0362-12.2012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., and Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behav. Res. Methods 37, 498–505. doi: 10.3758/BF03192720

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th Edn. Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Wertheimer, M. (1950). “Gestalt theory,” in A Sourcebook of Gestalt Psychology, ed W. D. Ellis (New York, NY: Humanities Press), 1–11. (Original work published 1924).

Woodman, G. F., Vecera, S. P., and Luck, S. J. (2003). Perceptual organization influences visual working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 10, 80–87. doi: 10.3758/BF03196470

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Xu, Y. (2002). Limitations of object-based feature encoding in visual short-term memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 28, 458–468. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.28.2.458

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Xu, Y. (2006). Understanding the object benefit in visual short-term memory: the roles of feature proximity and connectedness. Percept. Psychophys. 68, 815–828. doi: 10.3758/BF03193704

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Xu, Y., and Chun, M. M. (2006). Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects. Nature 440, 91–95. doi: 10.1038/nature04262

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Xu, Y., and Chun, M. M. (2007). Visual grouping in human parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 18766–18771. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705618104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Xu, Y., and Chun, M. M. (2009). Selecting and perceiving multiple visual objects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 167–174. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zaehle, T., Sandmann, P., Thorne, J. D., Jancke, L., and Herrmann, C. S. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory performance: combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. BMC Neurosci. 12:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: working memory, Gestalt grouping, response inhibition, tDCS, cognitive neuroscience

Citation: Berryhill ME, Peterson DJ, Jones KT and Stephens JA (2014) Hits and misses: leveraging tDCS to advance cognitive research. Front. Psychol. 5:800. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00800

Received: 08 April 2014; Accepted: 07 July 2014;
Published online: 25 July 2014.

Edited by:

Olga Lucía Gamboa Arana, Goethe-University Frankfurt am Main, Germany

Reviewed by:

Lisa Marshall, University of Luebeck, Germany
Ankita Sharma, Indian Institute of Technology Jodhpur, India

Copyright © 2014 Berryhill, Peterson, Jones and Stephens. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marian E. Berryhill, Program in Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, 1664 N. Virginia Street, Mail Stop 296, Reno, NV 89557, USA e-mail: mberryhill@unr.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.