Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 25 August 2014
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Self-control depletion is more than motivational switch from work to fun: the indispensable role of cognitive adaptation

  • 1Department of Psychology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
  • 2Department of Psychology, Peking University, Beijing, China
  • 3Research Center for Marketing and Consumer Science, Faculty of Business and Economics, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

A commentary on
Why self-control seems (but may not be) limited

by Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B. J., and Macrae, C. N. (2014). Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 127–133. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.009

It has been consistently demonstrated that people tend to perform more poorly on subsequent self-control tasks after completing an initial task that requires them to exert self-control (Hagger et al., 2010; Hagger and Chatzisarantis, 2014). The predominant explanation of such depletion effect claims that self-control taxes a limited resource that becomes drained with use (Muraven and Baumeister, 2000). Inzlicht and colleagues recently challenged the resource model by questioning the necessity and sufficiency of the resource metaphor for explaining self-control (Inzlicht et al., 2014). Instead, they presented a non-resource based process model. According to this model, self-control failure due to initial exertion is less about resource depletion but more about the motivated switching of task priorities from “have-to” and labor goals to “want-to” and leisure goals. We applaud such advance as the new account not only is evolutionarily and biologically more plausible but also can accommodate recent findings that are incompatible with the resource model (Inzlicht and Schmeichel, 2012; Inzlicht et al., 2014). However, we argue that the motivation-shift mechanism emphasized by the process model alone is not sufficient for explaining self-control depletion. A parallel cognitive adaptation mechanism must also be taken into account.

From a cognitive control perspective, the depletion effect is nothing mysterious but can be considered as a phenomenon similar to “switch costs” (Kiesel et al., 2010). The cognitive system is evolved to be able to actively adapt to given demands and buffer against situational changes. However, the inevitable cost is a reduced flexibility to promptly switch to a new demand. In a situation requiring consecutive exertion of effort, the control processes being recruited to adapt to the first self-control task would linger and hinder adaptation to the subsequent self-control task that requires different control processes (Botvinick et al., 2001; Dewitte et al., 2009). For example, if a dieting person is asked to control intake of palatable but unhealthy food after having performed emotion regulation, the recruitment of control processes for resisting temptation would be impeded as the control system is still geared toward regulating emotions.

The first implication of this reconceptualization is that engaging in a first self-control task could facilitate, rather than impair, self-control success in the second task when these two tasks require similar control processes because the control processes needed in the second task are already activated, as both experimental studies (Dewitte et al., 2009) and ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies (O'Connell et al., 2008) have attested.

The second implication is that even if the control processes on which the two consecutive tasks rely are different, allowing respondents sufficient time to adapt to the task demands would cancel the depletion effect. Consistent with this implication, recent research showed that adapting to either the first task (Dang et al., 2013) or the second task (Barutchu et al., 2013) removed the depletion effect without rest or additional motivation. Further, there was also evidence showing a negative correlation between the adaptation level and the depletion effect even when the time for adaptation was limited, such that the more respondents adapted to the first task, the less errors they made on the second task (Dang et al., 2013).

The third implication suggests factors that can reduce switch costs would help to overcome self-control depletion. It has been demonstrated that positive affect, which could enhance flexibility of switching to new cognitive sets by directing attention to novel information (Dreisbach and Goschke, 2004), successfully neutralized the depletion effect (Wenzel et al., 2013). In the meantime, preparation is also critical for attenuating switch cost (Kiesel et al., 2010). Studies have shown that adapting to a series of self-control tasks could counterintuitively offset the depletion effect because the requirement of continuous exertion would help respondents get more prepared for switching to the following demanding task (Converse and Deshon, 2009; Xiao et al., in press).

Since these findings can hardly be reconciled with the motivation-shift mechanism, we argue that the cognitive adaptation mechanism addresses another unique feature of self-control depletion and parallels the motivation-shift mechanism. Finding out how these two processes interact with each other during consecutive exertion is an important question for future research. We suggest both the interference of the lingered control processes after initial exertion and the decreased motivation to engage in further effortful work contribute to impaired performance on the subsequent task. At the same time, there is also a competition between the adaptation process and the motivational process such that successful adaptation, either to the initial or to the following task, would gradually reduce the role of motivation-shift as adaptation attenuates the aversiveness of effort exertion that necessitates the motivated switching of task priorities (Inzlicht et al., 2014), thus helping to overcome self-control depletion without recurring to additional motivation.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Barutchu, A., Carter, O., Hester, R., and Levy, N. (2013). Strength in cognitive self-regulation. Front. Psychol. 4:174. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00174

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108, 624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Converse, P. D., and Deshon, R. P. (2009). A tale of two tasks: reversing the self-regulatory resource depletion effect. J. Appl. Psychol. 94, 1318–1324. doi: 10.1037/a0014604

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dang, J., Dewitte, S., Mao, L., Xiao, S., and Shi, Y. (2013). Adapting to an initial self-regulatory task cancels the ego depletion effect. Conscious. Cogn. 22, 816–821. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.05.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dewitte, S., Bruyneel, S., and Geyskens, K. (2009). Self-regulating enhances self-regulation in subsequent consumer decisions involving similar response conflicts. J. Consum. Res. 36, 394–405. doi: 10.1086/598615

CrossRef Full Text

Dreisbach, G., and Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 30, 343–353. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.343

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hagger, M. S., and Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2014). It is premature to regard the ego depletion effect as “too incredible.” Front. Psychol. 5:298. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00298

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., and Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 136, 495–525. doi: 10.1037/a0019486

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Inzlicht, M., and Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 450–463. doi: 10.1177/1745691612454134

CrossRef Full Text

Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B. J., and Macrae, C. N. (2014). Why self-control seems (but may not be) limited. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 127–133. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., et al. (2010). Control and interference in task switching. Psychol. Bull. 136, 849–874. doi: 10.1037/a0019842

CrossRef Full Text

Muraven, M., and Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychol. Bull. 126, 247–259. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Connell, K. A., Schwartz, J. E., and Shiffman, S. (2008). Do resisted temptations during smoking cessation deplete or augment self-control resources? Psychol. Addict. Behav. 22, 486–495. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.22.4.486

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wenzel, M., Conner, T. S., and Kubiak, T. (2013). Understanding the limits of self-control: positive affect moderates the impact of task switching on consecutive self-control performance. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 43, 175–184. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1936

CrossRef Full Text

Xiao, S., Dang, J., Mao, L., and Liljedahl, S. (in press). When more depletion offsets the ego depletion effect. Soc. Psychol. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a00019

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: self-control, resource depletion, cognitive control, switch costs, adaptation

Citation: Dang J, Xiao S and Dewitte S (2014) Self-control depletion is more than motivational switch from work to fun: the indispensable role of cognitive adaptation. Front. Psychol. 5:933. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00933

Received: 12 July 2014; Accepted: 05 August 2014;
Published online: 25 August 2014.

Edited by:

Marcel Zentner, University of Innsbruck, Austria

Reviewed by:

Angela Lee Duckworth, University of Pennsylvania, USA

Copyright © 2014 Dang, Xiao and Dewitte. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: junhua.dang@psy.lu.se

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.