Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 29 October 2014
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuro-education and neuro-rehabilitation View all 17 articles

Intracerebral functional connectivity-guided neurofeedback as a putative rehabilitative intervention for ameliorating auditory-related dysfunctions

  • 1Division Neuropsychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 2Center for Integrative Human Physiology, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 3International Normal Aging and Plasticity Imaging Center, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 4Research Unit for Plasticity and Learning of the Healthy Aging Brain, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 5Dynamic of Healthy Aging, University Research Priority Program University of Zurich, Switzerland
  • 6Department of Special Education, King Abdulaziz University, Jeddah, Saudi Arabia

Electroencephalography (EEG) constitutes one of the most eligible candidates for neurofeedback applications, principally due to its excellent temporal resolution best reflecting the natural dynamics of brain processes. In addition, EEG is easy to use and provides the opportunity for mobile applications. In the present opinion article, we pinpoint the advantages of using intracerebral functional connectivity (IFC) instead of quantitative scalp EEG for interventional applications. In fact, due to the convergence of multiple signals originating from different spatial locations and electrophysiological interactions, miscellaneous scalp signals are too unspecific for therapeutic neurofeedback applications. Otherwise, IFC opens novel perspectives for influencing brain activity in specific dysfunctional small- and large-scale neuronal networks with a reasonable spatial resolution. In the present article, we propose concrete interventional IFC applications that may be used to ameliorate auditory-related dysfunctions such as developmental dyslexia.

From Historical Brain Perspectives to Modern Neuroscientific Approaches

During the 19th century, brain researchers took advantage of individuals suffering from brain lesions in order to determine the contribution of specific brain areas to different aspects of behavior, including perception, speech processing, motor skills, and cognitive functions (Zolamorgan, 1995; Leff, 2004). During the same century, an intellectual quarreling raged between researchers who believed that brain functions are localized in separable brain areas (localization view) and those who argued that the entire or parts of the cortex contributes to behavior (network view; for an historical overview see for example Leff, 2004). Nowadays, one can draw some hazardous analogies between modern neuroscientific approaches and historical perspectives on brain functions depending on the imaging technique used. In fact, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has a very poor temporal resolution, leading to the illusory impression that specific brain functions are localized in distinct brain areas. Otherwise, due to the dynamic and blurred nature of electrical scalp signals (i.e., electroencephalography, EEG), one could naively come to the conclusion that the entire brain contributes to a specific behavior.

Currently, it is generally acknowledged that widely distributed, specialized, and dynamic cortical-subcortical networks form the fundamental basis of behavior (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009). Within this framework, EEG has gained more and more attention in the field of cognitive neuroscience, mainly due to its excellent temporal resolution (in the range of milliseconds) enabling to capture the dynamic dimension of brain functioning in a more realistic manner than neuroimaging does. In addition, based on novel mathematical applications it is now possible to estimate the intracerebral origin of scalp signals (Scherg, 1990; Pascual-Marqui et al., 1994) as well as to objectify intracerebral functional connectivity (IFC) in real-time (Canuet et al., 2011; Kühnis et al., 2014) with a reasonable spatial resolution (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1994; Phillips et al., 2002). Thus, EEG is particularly suitable for comprehending the dynamic interplay between specific brain regions within local and global neuronal networks in both natural and dysfunctional brain conditions.

Neuronal Networks: The Bearing Skeleton of Brain Functions

Currently, there is no doubt that cognition (Langer et al., 2013), motor functions (Jin et al., 2012), and perception (Wu et al., 2012) do not function in isolation but are embedded in neuronal assemblies consisting of networks influencing each other’s through excitatory and inhibitory signals (Fell and Axmacher, 2011). Such small- and large-scale neuronal networks can be represented by using both functional and structural data as well as by taking into account different parameters, like white matter integrity (Hagmann et al., 2007; Elmer et al., accepted), cortical thickness (Hanggi et al., 2011), cortical surface area or volume (Bonilha et al., 2004; Sanabria-Diaz et al., 2010), hemodynamic responses (Rehme et al., 2013), or even intracerebral oscillatory phase synchronization values (Langer et al., 2012; Kühnis et al., 2014). Such neuronal networks can for example be modeled by taking into account mathematical graph theories (i.e., small-world networks) where most nodes within a network can be reached from every other by a small number of steps. This implies that efficient systems with small-world topology are characterized by a high local clustering coefficient (i.e., the degree to which nodes in a graph tend to cluster together) and short path lengths between distant nodes (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009). The advantage of focusing on such networks rather than on localized brain characteristics is that the former can support both segregated/specialized as well as distributed/integrated information processing (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009).

From a functional perspective, it is assumed that brain regions that do the same at the same time are somehow interconnected (i.e., functional connectivity). Functional connectivity and systemic brain organization can be described by using dynamic causal modeling (Eickhoff et al., 2009), Granger causality (Jancke, 2012), or correlative analyses between brain signals (for example signal amplitude, current density, power, or phase synchronization) originating from different spatial locations (Langer et al., 2012). Even though it results evident that structural and functional brain properties are mutually related (Miranda-Dominguez et al., 2014), the advantage of focussing on functional connectivity is that it enables to capture the dynamic nature of the human brain in different time-scales, ranging from milliseconds (i.e., EEG) to several seconds (i.e., fMRI).

EEG and Intracerebral Functional Connectivity

The discovery of EEG by Berger (1929) can be considered as one of the most important historical breakthrough in the field of neurology and cognitive neuroscience. This specific technique builds up on single electrodes that are fixed on the surface of the scalp for recording electrical brain activity. Through different applications in the field of electrical engineering (i.e., signal amplification, impedance reduction, etc.), it became possible to measure the summed electrical postsynaptic activity that is locked or unlocked to an external (for example auditory stimulation), or internal (for example imagery) event at the surface of the scalp. Such electrical brain activity can be quantified, for example, by evaluating the amplitude and timing of event-related potentials (ERPs), power spectra in different frequency ranges over time, or the degree of phase alignment (i.e., coherence) in a specific frequency band between single scalp electrodes.

In the last 15 years, novel mathematical applications render it possible to overcome the so called “inverse problem” of intracerebral EEG source estimation (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1994) however, with some drawbacks in terms of spatial resolutions (Phillips et al., 2002). Currently, several toolboxes and software (http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Comparison_of_neurofeedback_software) can be used for estimating intracerebral brain activity based on the electrical signal recorder from the surface of the scalp. These new technologies imply that for each signal measured on the surface of the scalp it becomes possible to estimate intracerebral brain activity for each voxel, Brodmann area, or region of interest (ROI) in the form of current-, or spectral-power density by retaining phase information. Therefore, all these measures can be taken for modeling IFC networks (see previous section).

In turn, we will provide two examples of practical applications of IFC in a specific group of experts, namely professional musicians. In a first study, we measured professional musicians and non-musicians by using EEG and IFC analyses. We postulated that auditory-specialization (Elmer et al., 2012; Marie et al., 2012; Kuhnis et al., 2013) and asymmetry (Schneider et al., 2002) in musicians should be dependent, at least in part, by the amount of interhemispheric communication between the left and right auditory-related cortex (ARC). Based on this assumption, we measured intracerebral phase synchronization (see Figure 1) in the theta, alpha, and beta frequency range between the two ARC in musicians and non-musicians. We found support for our hypothesis in that musicians showed increased IFC between the two ARC as well as a relationship between IFC and the amplitude of auditory-evoked potentials (Kühnis et al., 2014).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Intracerebral functional connectivity (IFC). This figure depicts the degree of phase alignment between two regions of interests (ROIs) in the left and right hemisphere. The bottom blue oscillation provides an example of perfect phase synchronization, whereas the red one shows a temporal lag in phase synchronicity.

A second example that depicts a relationship between IFC and expertise arises from a recent study of our group (Elmer et al., under revision) where we tried to integrate two apparently opposite perspectives on absolute pitch, that is the ability to recognize the chroma (i.e., pitch) of a tone without a reference tone (Levitin and Rogers, 2005). In this context, some researchers argue that this specific ability relies on an optimized “early categorical perception” at the processing level of the left ARC (i.e., perception; Siegel, 1974), whereas others suggest that the distinctive trait of AP more likely derives from mnemonic facilitation (Elmer et al., 2013) enabling “pitch labeling” mechanisms by recruiting left-sided prefrontal brain regions (i.e., cognition; Zatorre et al., 1998). By combining EEG and resting-state IFC, we evaluated phase synchronization between the left ARC and the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the theta (∼ 4–7 Hz) frequency range in a group of musicians with and without AP. Theta oscillations have previously been shown to reliably reflect mnemonic processes (Kahana et al., 1999; Caplan et al., 2001; Ward, 2003; Sauseng et al., 2005), information integration (Ward, 2003), and neuronal communication between distinct brain regions over long-range circuits (Ward, 2003; Polania et al., 2012). Results revealed that in AP musicians perceptual and cognitive subdivisions of the human brain are tightly coupled through oscillatory theta phase-alignment. In addition, within the AP group this specific electrophysiological marker was predictive of pitch-labeling performance by explaining about 30% of behavioral variance. These two EEG studies target at illustrating practical applications of IFC analyses for evaluating systemic brain reorganizations rather than focusing on localized brain functions in isolation. This point of view is also supported by a recent paper of Seither-Preisler et al. (2014) providing specific evidence for increased bilateral synchrony of the primary auditory evoked responses collected at the surface of the scalp in children undergoing musical training compared to children suffering from attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Interestingly, this functional dysalignment of auditory-evoked brain responses was accompanied by anatomical specificities of auditory-related brain regions.

The Basic Principles of Intracerebral Functional Connectivity-Guided Neurofeedback

Neurofeedback bases on cybernetic models consisting of using information about the physiological state of an organism for changing it in a specific direction (Gunkelman and Johnstone, 2005). Such cybernetic models can be utilized when the system to be analyzed is assumed to rely on closed signal-loops. This means that a change in a biological system (in this case brain activity) generates specific changes in the environment (in our case the feedback) that on his part triggers a modulation of the biological system (i.e., brain activity, see Figure 2). Due to the high temporal resolution of EEG as well as to novel mathematical applications, it is now possible to modulate the own brain activity in quasi real-time based on a specific feedback (i.e., visual, auditory, haptic, etc.). Meanwhile, there is a vast body of literature describing neurofeedback applications in several fields of clinical neuroscience (Schoenberg and David, 2014), including the treatment of addiction (Dehghani-Arani et al., 2013), attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Maurizio et al., 2014), depression (Young et al., 2014), epilepsy (Tan et al., 2009), and much more (Schoenberg and David, 2014).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Neurofeedback. This figure provides a simplified overview of IFC-based neurofeedback. During EEG recording, the neurofeedback software provides information about the degree of phase alignment in a priori defined intracerebral ROIs [here the bilateral auditory-related cortex (ARC), red circles]. This information is visualized on a monitor by means of a brain–computer interface (activity visualization). Through the coupling of brain activity with a specific task (here video game), participants receive a visual feedback on the modulation of the own brain activity (feedback, here lagged phase synchronization).

An important prerequisite for clinical neurofeedback applications is to exactly know which intra- and extracerebral EEG parameters best reflect a specific natural or dysfunctional brain condition. In addition, an accurate identification of dysfunctional brain areas as well as of functional networks constitutes an important step toward evidence-based clinical applications. In the present article, we principally focus on IFC-guided neurofeedback rather than on the modulation of brain signals at the surface of the scalp. This line of argumentation is supported by the fact that scalp-signals are composed of miscellaneous and unspecific brain activity originating from a variety of brain regions. Furthermore, we are of the opinion that it is more efficient to dynamically change IFC between specific brain regions of interest instead of focusing on the modulation of restricted brain functions, than the former approach more likely takes into account the dynamic and interconnected nature of the human brain (see previous sections).

IFC guided neurofeedback applications base on exactly the same cybernetic models described in the previous section and depicted in Figure 2. However, in contrast to scalp-data based neurofedback, it is necessary to postulate clear assumptions on specific brain areas that are dysfunctional as well as on functional connectivity between these areas. Therefore, the first step of IFC-based neurofeedback is the identification of ROIs within a dysfunctional network. In addition, depending on the connectivity parameters to be trained (i.e., current density, phase synchronization, etc.) one should have a clear conception of the direction of modulations, that means increased or reduced intracerebral activity within the network of interest. In the case of IFC-based neurofeedback relying on the modulation of oscillatory phase alignment between different brain regions, specific knowledge about the relationships between brain functions and oscillations (i.e., delta, theta, alpha, beta, or gamma) is strictly required. In turn, we will describe the pathogenesis of a specific neurological disorder that is associated (at least in part) with auditory-related dysfunctions, namely developmental dyslexia. Based on a review of current research literature, we will propose concrete IFC-based neurofeedback applications relying on the entrainment of lagged phased synchronization. The modulation of lagged phase synchronization constitutes a fruitful approach in that this measure is supposed to reflect true connectivity by taking into account the delay of impulse propagation that is influenced by volume conduction (Pascual-Marqui et al., 2011). It is important to mention, that the ARC shows a huge inter-individual variability (Steinmetz et al., 1989; Zatorre, 2013) and asymmetry (Marie et al., 2013), and that this variability is additionally strongly influenced by training and expertise (Schlaug et al., 1995). Therefore, depending on the research question addressed and on the sample of subjects studied, it is important to take into account such influencing variables.

Developmental Dyslexia

The main purpose of the present work is to discuss concrete applications of IFC-based neurofeedback for the treatment of auditory-related dysfunctions. However, due to profound differences in the pathogenesis of such dysfunctions, here we will focus on developmental dyslexia only. It is important to mention that it is conceivable that similar approaches we present in association with dyslexia can be extended to other auditory-related dysfunctions, like for example tinnitus (Okamoto et al., 2010) or developmental language disorders (Heim et al., 2013).

Developmental dyslexia can be described as low reading and writing skills despite average intelligence, good educational support, and solid social background (Habib, 2000; Demonet et al., 2004). In the last three decades, several theories have been proposed for explaining the specific deficits in developmental dyslexia, including general perceptual/phonetic- (Tallal and Piercy, 1973; Merzenich et al., 1996; Stein, 2001; Goswami et al., 2002), attentional- (Bogon et al., 2014), and working memory deficits (Ahissar et al., 2006). Also visual (Lovegrove et al., 1980; Stein, 2012) and motor impairments (Nicolson and Fawcet, 1990) have been described.

Several of these theories postulate that developmental dyslexia is somehow related to auditory-related dysfunctions (Tallal and Piercy, 1973; Goswami et al., 2002). The “rapid processing deficit theory” proposed by Tallal and Piercy (1973) and Merzenich et al. (1996) postulates a specific impairment in the processing of fast-changing verbal cues, such as formant transitions and voice-onset time (VOT). In a similar way, Goswami et al. (2002) postulated that dyslexia is associated with a poor temporal resolution of speech sounds that specifically affects the processing of sound rise time. Other theories on dyslexia are rather centerd on phonological abilities (Stanovich, 1988; Serniclaes et al., 2001; Ramus, 2003; Ramus et al., 2013) and base on the assumption that dyslexic individuals are specifically impaired in building-up phonological representations (Stanovich, 1988). Finally, also impaired phonological awareness (Ramus, 2003; Ramus et al., 2013) and abnormal sensitivity to within phonemic category variations (Serniclaes et al., 2001) have previously been proposed to constitute the salient trait of developmental dyslexia. For a more comprehensive review of the literature on dyslexia, the reader is addressed to a previous work of Hamalainen et al. (2013).

Interestingly, most of the theories described above are compatible, at least in part, with the view that dyslexic children often show functional (Blau et al., 2009, 2010; Kast et al., 2011) and structural (Hugdahl et al., 2003; Brambati et al., 2004; Bloom et al., 2013) variations in the left ARC, a brain region that is relatively strongly involved in the processing of fast changing verbal and non-verbal cues and phonemes (Zatorre and Belin, 2001; Griffiths and Warren, 2002; Hickok and Poeppel, 2007). In addition, previous fMRI (Zatorre and Belin, 2001; Griffiths and Warren, 2002; Shaywitz et al., 2003, 2007; Hickok and Poeppel, 2007), DTI (Hoeft et al., 2011), and EEG (Dujardin et al., 2011) studies provided evidence for a stronger recruitment of right-sided ARC in dyslexic individuals during speech processing, possibly for compensating poor left-sided temporal resolution.

In a recent multi-pattern neuroimaging study Boets et al. (2013) reported intact phonetic representations (in terms of robustness and distinctness) in the bilateral ARC in adults suffering from dyslexia. Most notably, by combining functional and structural connectivity analyses, the same authors’ revealed reduced connectivity between bilateral auditory-related brain regions as well as between the auditory cortices and the left inferior frontal gyrus, the latter region being involved in higher order cognitive functions. These results are interesting in that they open the possibility to consider dyslexia as a neuropsychological state where not phoneme representation per se, but rather the access to these representations, is dysfunctional. A similar perspective can be taken into account when considering a recent publication of Vandermosten et al. (2013) where the authors combined DTI and EEG measurements in a sample of adult dyslexic individuals and found evidence for reduced white matter lateralization in the left posterior supratemporal plane and arcuate fasciculus. In addition, white matter lateralization in the posterior superior temporal gyrus and white matter integrity in the posterior part of the corpus callosum were related to phase coherence in bilateral auditory-related brain regions in the frequency range roughly corresponding to phonemic-rate modulations (∼20 Hz, β). Meanwhile, there is even evidence from longitudinal studies (Langer et al., 2013) showing that functional connectivity can change after only few weeks of training.

Intracerebral Connectivity-Guided Neurofeedback as a Putative Rehabilitative Intervention for Developmental Dyslexia

As described in the previous section, there is strong evidence showing dysfunctional left-sided (Rumsey et al., 1992; Temple, 2002) and compensatory right-sided (Shaywitz et al., 2003, 2007; Dujardin et al., 2011) brain activity in the ARC of dyslexic individuals. In addition, recent data point to altered functional (Poelmans et al., 2012; Vandermosten et al., 2013) and structural (Boets et al., 2013) connectivity among bilateral auditory-related brain regions as well as between the bilateral ARC and the left inferior frontal gyrus (Boets et al., 2013). The latter brain region is supposed to be involved in accessing higher-order phonological representations. With these previous results in mind, we will propose specific IFC-guided neurofeedback protocols that may be useful for ameliorating the auditory-related impairments often observed in dyslexic individuals. Please consider that these neurofeedback protocols are ordered in a hierarchical manner that means from small- to large-scale network reorganization.

Training Protocol 1

Based on the often observed hypoactivity of the left ARC in conjunction with the compensatory hyperactivity of its right-sided homolog in dyslexic individuals, we propose a training protocol targeting at ameliorating the division of labor (i.e., intracerebral lagged phase synchronization) between these two perisylvian brain regions (i.e., Brodmann areas 41/42/22). The reasoning beyond this training protocol is that the amelioration of functional connectivity between bilateral auditory-related brain regions may improve the functional capacity of the left ARC and at the same time reduce right-sided compensatory activity (Figure 3, p1). Along this vein, it is conceivable that an increase in phase alignment in at least two frequency bands may possibly improve reading skills, namely theta (∼4–7 Hz) and beta (∼13–20 Hz). In fact, theta oscillations roughly overlap with the processing rate of syllables, whereas beta oscillations coincide with the temporal dynamics of single phonemes (Poeppel, 2003; Poelmans et al., 2012). This reasoning is in line with previous work showing that dyslexic individuals are characterized by poor phonological awareness (Ramus, 2003; Ramus et al., 2013), a condition that is strongly dependent on the segmentation of single words into smaller units, namely syllables and phonemes.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Training protocols. This figure provides an overview of the training protocols we propose for ameliorating auditory-related dysfunctions in dyslexic individuals. The red circles depict the intracerebral regions of interest, the blue arrows functional connectivity (lagged phase synchronization). P1–P4, protocol 1–4.

Finally, it is important to mention that two previous studies of our group highlighted a relationship between the superiority of professional musicians (compared to non-musicians) in processing segmental speech cues (i.e., syllables varying in VOT and vowels) and functional (Kühnis et al., 2014) as well as structural (Elmer et al., accepted) connectivity among bilateral auditory-related brain regions. Based on these previous results, we believe that the improved and optimized auditory system of musicians can provide fruitful information for developing novel rehabilitative neurofeedback strategies targeting at optimizing auditory-related dysfunctions in dyslexic individuals. Certainly, future studies focusing on the validation of the training protocols we present in the present work are strictly required for optimizing rehabilitative power.

Training Protocol 2

Previous work has described reduced functional and structural connectivity between the left ARC and the left inferior frontal gyrus in dyslexic individuals (Boets et al., 2013). Therefore, we may speculate whether in dyslexic individuals (probably mainly adults) not phoneme representation per se, but rather the access to these mnemonic representations in the left inferior frontal gyrus, is dysfunctional (Boets et al., 2013; Vandermosten et al., 2013). Based on previous work showing that neuronal oscillations in the theta-frequency range (∼ 4–7 Hz) reflect mnemonic processes (Kahana et al., 1999; Caplan et al., 2001; Ward, 2003; Sauseng et al., 2005; Elmer et al., under revision), information integration (Ward, 2003), and neuronal communication between distinct brain regions over long-range circuits (Ward, 2003; Polania et al., 2012), we propose the possibility to ameliorate the recruitment of higher order phonetic representations by increasing theta phase synchronization between the left ARC (BA 41/42/22) and the left inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44/45/47). See Figure 3, p2.

Training Protocol 3

The third training protocol we propose here is an extension of “training protocol 2” (Figure 3, p3). Subjects are trained to increase intracerebral phase synchronization in the theta frequency range simultaneously between both the left and right ARC and the left inferior frontal gyrus (Boets et al., 2013).

Training Protocol 4

This protocol implies a simultaneous combination of training protocols 1 and 3 (Figure 3, p4).

Future Perspectives

In the present opinion paper we discussed the possibility to ameliorate auditory-related dysfunctions by using IFC-based neurofeedback application targeting at changing the systemic functional brain organization rather than focusing on brain functions in isolation. It is important to mention that here we only addressed some putative application without any claim to completeness. In addition, we want to emphasize that future studies are strictly required for evaluating the rehabilitative relevance of the single training protocols we propose. We explicitly abstained from providing indications on specific training parameters (i.e., training duration and frequency) because we are of the opinion that neurofeedback therapists are best skilled for arranging and optimizing the training protocols we propose. Finally, it is important to remark that in our opinion a better understanding of simple connectivity circuits should be the first step. Only after having collected enough evidence for valid therapeutic applications in small-brain circuits, it makes sense to consider more systemic brain reorganization.

Connectivity Toolboxes

For an overview of different neurofeedback applications, the reader is addressed to the following Wikipedia page: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Comparison_of_neurofeedback_software

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

This research was supported by the Swiss National Foundation (SNF Grants 320030-120661, 4-62341-05, and 320030B_138668/1 to Lutz Jäncke).

References

Ahissar, M., Lubin, Y., Putter-Katz, H., and Banai, K. (2006). Dyslexia and the failure to form a perceptual anchor. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1558–1564. doi: 10.1038/n n1800

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, H. (1929). Über das Elektrenkephalogramm des Menschen. Arch. Psychiat. Nervenkr. 87, 527–570. doi: 10.1007/BF01797193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blau, V., Reithler, J., van Atteveldt, N., Seitz, J., Gerretsen, P., Goebel, R.,et al. (2010). Deviant processing of letters and speech sounds as proximate cause of reading failure: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of dyslexic children. Brain 133, 868–879. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp308

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blau, V., van Atteveldt, N., Ekkebus, M., Goebel, R., and Blomert, L. (2009). Reduced neural integration of letters and speech sounds inks phonological and reading deficits in adult dyslexia. Curr. Biol. 19, 503–508. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.065

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloom, J. S., Garcia-Barrera, M. A., Miller, C. J., Miller, S. R., and Hynd, G. W. (2013). Planum temporale morphology in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 51, 1684–1692. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.05.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boets, B., Op de Beeck, H. P., Vandermosten, M., Scott, S. K., Gillebert, C. R., Mantini, D.,et al. (2013). Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science 342, 1251–1254. doi: 10.1126/science.1244333

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogon, J., Finke, K., Schulte-Körne, G., Müller, H. J., Schneider W. X., and Stenneken, P. (2014). Parameter-based assessment of disturbed and intact components of visual attention in children with developmental dyslexia. Dev. Sci. 17, 697–713. doi: 10.1111/desc.12150

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonilha, L., Rorden, C., Castellano, G., Pereira, F., Rio, P. A., Cendes, F.,et al. (2004). Voxel-based morphometry reveals gray matter network atrophy in refractory medial temporal lobe epilepsy. Arch. Neurol. 61, 1379–1384. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.9.1379

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brambati, S. M., Termine, C., Ruffino, M., Stella, G., Fazio, F., Cappa, S. F.,et al. (2004). Regional reductions of gray matter volume in familial dyslexia. Neurology 63, 742–745. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000134673.95020.EE

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canuet, L., Ishii, R., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Iwase, M., Kurimoto, R., Aoki, Y.,et al. (2011). Resting-state EEG source localization and functional connectivity in schizophrenia- like psychosis of epilepsy. PLoS ONE 6:e27863. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027863

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caplan, J. B., Madsen, J. R., Raghavachari, S., and Kahana, M. J. (2001). Distinct patterns of brain oscillations underlie two basic parameters of human maze learning. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 368–380.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehghani-Arani, F., Rostami, R., and Nadali, H. (2013). Neurofeedback training for opiate addiction: improvement of mental health and craving. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 38, 133–141. doi: 10.1007/s10484-013-9218-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demonet, J. F., Taylor, M. J., and Chaix, Y. (2004). Developmental dyslexia. Lancet 363, 1451–1460. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)16106-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dujardin, T., Etienne, Y., Contentin, C., Bernard, C., Largy, P., Mellier, D.et al. (2011). Behavioral performances in participants with phonological dyslexia and different patterns on the N170 component. Brain Cogn. 75, 91–100. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.10.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eickhoff, S. B., Heim, S., Zilles, K., and Amunts, K. (2009). A systems perspective on the effective connectivity of overt speech production. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 367, 2399–2421. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2008.0287

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elmer, S., Meyer, M., and Jancke, L. (2012). Neurofunctional and behavioral correlates of phonetic and temporal categorization in musically trained and untrained subjects. Cereb. Cortex 22, 650–658. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr142

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elmer, S., Sollberger, S., Meyer, M., and Jancke, L. (2013). An empirical reevaluation of absolute Pitch: behavioral and electrophysiological measurements. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 1736–1753. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00410

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fell, J., and Axmacher, N. (2011). The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 105–118. doi: 10.1038/nrn2979

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S.,et al. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: a new hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 10911–10916. doi: 10.1073/pnas.122368599

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, T. D., and Warren, J. D. (2002). The planum temporale as a computational hub. Trends Neurosci. 25, 348–353. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(02)02191-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunkelman, J. D., and Johnstone, J. (2005). Neurofeedback and the brain. J. Adult Dev. 12, 93–98. doi: 10.1007/s10804-005-7024-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Habib, M. (2000). The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia–an overview and working hypothesis. Brain 123, 2373–2399. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.12.2373

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagmann, P., Kurant, M., Gigandet, X., Thiran, P., Wedeen, V. J., Meuli, R.,et al. (2007). Mapping human whole-brain structural networks with diffusion MRI. PLoS ONE 2:e597. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000597

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamalainen, J. A., Salminen, H. K., and Leppanen, P. H. T. (2013). Basic auditory processing deficits in dyslexia: systematic review of the behavioral and event-related potential/field evidence. J. Learn. Disabil. 46, 413–427. doi: 10.1177/0022219411436213

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanggi, J., Wotruba, D., and Jancke, L. (2011). Globally altered structural brain network topology in grapheme-color synesthesia. J. Neurosci. 31, 5816–5828. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0964-10.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heim, S., Keil, A., Choudhury, N., Friedman, J. T., and Benasich, A. A. (2013). Early gamma oscillations during rapid auditory processing in children with a language-learning impairment: changes in neural mass activity after training. Neuropsychologia 51, 990–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.01.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G., and Poeppel, D. (2007). Opinion – The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 393–402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoeft, F., McCandliss, B. D., Black, J. M., Gantman, A., Zakerani, N., Hulme, C.,et al. (2011). Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 361–366. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008950108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hugdahl, K., Heiervang, E., Ersland, L., Lundervold, A., Steinmetz, H., and Smievoll, A. I. (2003). Significant relation between MR measures of planum temporale area and dichotic processing of syllables in dyslexic children. Neuropsychologia 41, 666–675. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00224-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jancke, L. (2012). The dynamic audio-motor system in pianists. Neurosci. Music Learn. Mem. 1252, 246–252. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06416.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, S. H., Lin, P., and Hallett, M. (2012). Reorganization of brain functional small-world networks during finger movements. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 861–872. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21253

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahana, M. J., Sekuler, R., Caplan, J. B., Kirschen, M., and Madsen, J. R. (1999). Human theta oscillations exhibit task dependence during virtual maze navigation. Nature 399, 781–784. doi: 10.1038/21645

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kast, M., Bezzola, L., Jancke, L., and Meyer, M. (2011). Multi- and unisensory decoding of words and non-words result in differential brain responses in dyslexic and non-dyslexic adults. Brain Lang. 119, 136–148. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.04.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kühnis, J., Elmer, S., and Jäncke, L. (2014). Auditory Evoked Responses in Musicians during Passive Vowel Listening Are Modulated by Functional Connectivity between Bilateral Auditory-related Brain Regions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 4, 1–2. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00674

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhnis, J., Elmer, S., Meyer, M., and Jancke, L. (2013). The encoding of vowels and temporal speech cues in the auditory cortex of professional musicians: an EEG study. Neuropsychologia 51, 1608–1618. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langer, N., Pedroni, A., Gianotti, L. R., Hanggi, J., Knoch, D., and Jancke, L. (2012). Functional brain network efficiency predicts intelligence. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 1393–1406. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21297

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langer, N., von Bastian, C. C., Wirz, H., Oberauer, K., and Jancke, L. (2013). The effects of working memory training on functional brain network efficiency. Cortex 49, 2424–2438. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.01.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leff, A. (2004). A historical review of the representation of the visual field in primary visual cortex with special reference to the neural mechanisms underlying macular sparing. Brain Lang. 88, 268–278. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00161-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levitin, D. J., and Rogers, S. E. (2005). Absolute pitch: perception, coding, and controversies. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovegrove, W. J., Bowling, A., Badcock, D., and Blackwood, M. (1980). Specific reading- disability – differences in contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial-frequency. Science 210, 439–440. doi: 10.1126/science.7433985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marie, C., Kujala, T., and Besson, M. (2012). Musical and linguistic expertise influence pre- attentive and attentive processing of non-speech sounds. Cortex 48, 447–457. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marie, D., Jobard, G., Crivello, F., Perchey, G., Petit, L., Mellet, E.,et al. (2013). Descriptive anatomy of heschl’s gyris in 430 healthy volunteers, including 198 left-handers. Brain Struct. Funct. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0680-x [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maurizio, S., Liechti, M. D., Heinrich, H., Jancke, L., Steinhausen, H. C., Walitza, S.et al. (2014). Comparing tomographic EEG neurofeedback and EMG biofeedback in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychol. 95, 31–44. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.10.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merzenich, M. M., Jenkins, W. M., Johnston, P., Schreiner, C., Miller, S. L., and Tallal, P. (1996). Temporal processing deficits of language-learning impaired children ameliorated by training. Science 271, 77–81. doi: 10.1126/science.271.5245.77

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda-Dominguez, O., Mills, B. D., Grayson, D., Woodall, A., Grant, K. A., Kroenke, C. D.,et al. (2014). Bridging the Gap between the Human and Macaque Connectome: A Quantitative Comparison of Global Interspecies Structure-Function Relationships and Network Topology. J. Neurosci. 34, 5552–5563. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4229-13.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolson, R. I., and Fawcet, A. J. (1990). Automaticity: a new framework for dyslexia research? Cognition 35, 159–182. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90013-A

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okamoto, H., Stracke, H., Stoll, W., and Pantev, C. (2010). Listening to tailor-made notched music reduces tinnitus loudness and tinnitus-related auditory cortex activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 1207–1210. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911268107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Lehmann, D., Koukkou, M., Kochi, K., Anderer, P., Saletu, B.,et al. (2011). Assessing interactions in the brain with exact low-resolution electromagnetic tomography. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 369, 3768–3784. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2011.0081

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Michel, C. M., and Lehmann, D. (1994). Low-resolution electromagnetic tomography – a new method for localizing electrical-activity in the brain. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 18, 49–65. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(84)90014-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, C., Rugg, M. D., and Friston, K. J. (2002). Anatomically informed basis functions for EEG source localization: combining functional and anatomical constraints. Neuroimage 16, 678–695. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1143

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poelmans, H., Lust, H., Vandermosten, M., Boets, B., Ghesquière, P., and Wouters, J. (2012). Auditory steady state cortical responses indicate deviant phonemic-rate processing in adults With dyslexia. Ear Hear. 33, 134–143. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31822c26b9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poeppel, D. (2003). The analysis of speech in different temporal integration windows: cerebral lateralization as “asymmetric sampling in time”. Speech Commun. 41, 245–255. doi: 10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00107-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polania, R., Nitsche, M. A., Korman, C., Batsikadze, G., and Paulus, W. (2012). The importance of timing in segregated theta phase-coupling for cognitive performance. Curr. Biol. 22, 1314–1318. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 212–218. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00035-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramus, F., Marshall, C. R., Rosen, S., and van der Lely, H. K. J. (2013). Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: toward a multidimensional model. Brain 136, 630–645. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws356

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rehme, A. K., Eickhoff, S. B., and Grefkes, C. (2013). State-dependent differences between functional and effective connectivity of the human cortical motor system. Neuroimage 67, 237–246. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rumsey, J. M., Andreason, P., Zametkin, A. J., Aquino, T., King, A. C., Hamburger, S. D.,et al. (1992). Failure to activate the left temporoparietal cortex in dyslexia – an oxygen 15 positron emission tomographic study. Arch. Neurol. 49, 527–534. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530290115020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanabria-Diaz, G., Melie-Garcia, L., Iturria-Medina, Y., Aleman-Gomez, Y., Hernandez- Gonzalez, G., Valdes-Urrutia, L.,et al. (2010). Surface area and cortical thickness descriptors reveal different attributes of the structural human brain networks. Neuroimage 50, 1497–1510. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010. 01.028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Schabus, M., and Doppelmayr, M. (2005). Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 57, 97–103. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005. 03.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherg, M. (1990). “Fundamentals of dipole source potential analysis,” in Auditory Evoked Magnetic Fields And Electric Potentials (Advances in Audiology), Vol. 6, eds F. Grandori, M. Hoke, and G. L. Romani (Basel: Karger), 40–69.

Google Scholar

Schlaug, G., Jäncke, L., Huang, Y., Steinmetz, H. (1995). In vivo evidence of structural brain asymmetry in musicians. Science 267, 699–701. doi: 10.1126/science.7839149

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, P., Scherg, M., Dosch, H. G., Specht, H. J., Gutschalk, A., and Rupp, A. (2002). Morphology of Heschl’s gyrus reflects enhanced activation in the auditory cortex of musicians. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 688–694. doi: 10.1038/nn871

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoenberg, P. L. A., and David, A. S. (2014). Biofeedback for psychiatric disorders: a systematic review. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 39, 109–135. doi: 10.1007/s10484-014-9246-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seither-Preisler, A., Parncutt, R., and Schneider, P. (2014). Size and synchronization of auditory cortex promotes musical, literacy, and attentional skills in children. J. Neurosci. 34, 10937–10949. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5315-13.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., Carre, R., and Demonet, J. F. (2001). Perceptual discrimination of speech sounds in developmental dyslexia. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 44, 384–399. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/032)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaywitz, B. A., Skudlarski, P., Holahan, J. M., Marchione, K. E., Constable, R. T., Fulbright, R. K.,et al. (2007). Age-related changes in reading systems of dyslexic children. Ann. Neurol. 61, 363–370. doi: 10.1002/ana.21093

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., Mencl, W. E., Constable, R. T.,et al. (2003). Neural systems for compensation and persistence: young adult outcome of childhood reading disability. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 25–33. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01836-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, J. A. (1974). Sensory and Verbal Coding Strategies in Subjects with Absolute Pitch. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 55, S9. doi: 10.1121/1.1919544

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden- variety poor reader – the phonological-core variable-difference model. J. Learn. Disabil. 21, 590–604. doi: 10.1177/002221948802101003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, J. (2001). The sensory basis of reading problems. Dev. Neuropsychol. 20, 509–534. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2002_4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, J. (2012). The magnocellular theory of visual dyslexia. Perception 41, 15.

Google Scholar

Steinmetz, H., Rademacher, J., Huang, Y. X., Zilles, K., thron, A., and Frenud, H. J. (1989). Cerebral asymmetry: MR planimetry of the human planum temporale. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 13, 996–1005. doi: 10.1097/00004728-198911000-00011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallal, P., and Piercy, M. (1973). Developmental aphasia: impaired rate of non-verbal processing as a function of sensory modality. Neuropsycholgia 11, 389–398. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(73)90025-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, G., Thornby, J., Hammond, D. C., Strehl, U., Canady, B., Arnemann, K.et al. (2009). Meta-analysis of EEG biofeedback in treating epilepsy. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 40, 173–179. doi: 10.1177/155005940904000310

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Temple, E. (2002). Brain mechanisms in normal and dyslexic readers. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 12, 178–183. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00303-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandermosten, M., Poelmans, H., Sunaert, S., Ghesquiere, P., and Wouters, J. (2013). White matter lateralization and interhemispheric coherence to auditory modulations in normal reading and dyslexic adults. Neuropsychologia 51, 2087–2099. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.07.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, L. M. (2003). Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 553–559. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, J. J., Zhang, J. S., Liu, C., Liu, D. W., Ding, X. J., and Zhou, C. L. (2012). Graph theoretical analysis of EEG functional connectivity during music perception. Brain Res. 1483, 71–81. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.09.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, K. D., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Misaki, M., Yuan, H., Drevets, W. C.,et al. (2014). Real-time fMRI neurofeedback training of amygdala activity in patients with major depressive disorder. PLoS ONE 9:e88785. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088785

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Predispositions and plasticity in music and speech learning: neural correlates and implications. Science 342, 585–589. doi: 10.1126/science.1238414

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., and Belin, P. (2001). Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 11, 946–953. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.10.946

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., Perry, D. W., Beckett, C. A., Westbury, C. F., and Evans, A. C. (1998). Functional anatomy of musical processing in listeners with absolute pitch and relative pitch. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 3172–3177. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3172

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zolamorgan, S. (1995). Localization of brain-function – the legacy of Gall, Franz, Joseph (1758–1828). Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 359–383. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.002043

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: functional connectivity, EEG, neurofeedback, developmental dyslexia, rehabilitation, auditory-related cortex

Citation: Elmer S and Jäncke L (2014) Intracerebral functional connectivity-guided neurofeedback as a putative rehabilitative intervention for ameliorating auditory-related dysfunctions. Front. Psychol. 5:1227. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01227

Received: 19 August 2014; Accepted: 09 October 2014;
Published online: 29 October 2014.

Edited by:

Mireille Besson, CNRS – Institut de Neurosciences Cognitives de la Meditarranée, France

Reviewed by:

Peter Schneider, Heidelberg Medical School, Germany
Michel Habib, Resodys Aix-Marseille University, France

Copyright © 2014 Elmer and Jäncke. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stefan Elmer, Division Neuropsychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Zurich, Binzmühlestrasse 14/25, CH-8050 Zurich, Switzerland e-mail: s.elmer@psychologie.uzh.ch

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.