Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 12 January 2015
Sec. Cognitive Science

A consideration of what is meant by automaticity and better ways to measure it

  • Health Psychology and Behavioural Medicine Group, School of Psychology and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Perth, WA, Australia

A commentary on
Predicting automaticity in exercise behaviour: the role of perceived behavioural control, affect, intention, action planning, and behaviour

by de Bruijn, G.-J., Gardner, B., van Osch, L., and Sniehotta, F. (2014). Int. J. Behav. Med. 21, 767–774. doi: 10.1007/s12529-013-9348-4

Existing models of exercise behavior are insufficient in predicting outcomes, this point is shown by the relatively high levels of unexplained variance in exercise behavior in meta-analyses of social cognitive theories and models (Chatzisarantis et al., 2003; Hagger and Chatzisarantis, 2009). Researchers are beginning to recognize the importance of implicit, automatic processes in the prediction of health behaviors (Dimmock and Banting, 2009; Keatley et al., 2012, 2013b). The research by de Bruijn et al. (2014) is useful for highlighting the importance of automaticity in exercise behavior. We commend the authors on investigating an important approach to automaticity and exercise behavior. There were, however, some points with which we disagree. We think that the authors do not provide a clear account of what they mean by automaticity–an issue that is essential for the operationalization of the construct. Bargh (1994), for instance, suggested automaticity has four characteristics: awareness, intention, efficiency, and control; it is not clear whether de Bruijn and colleagues automaticity adheres to this. In particular, we contend that the explicit measure of automaticity used in their research is not an optimal way to assess implicit, impulsive processes. Furthermore, we contend that implicit measures, such as the implicit association test (IAT; Greenwald et al., 1998) would be better positioned as measures of non-conscious processes. The present commentary focuses on pre-behavior automatic associations, which we contend are better assessed by existing implicit measures, rather than during-behavior automatic “processes.”

Taking our perspective from existing implicit theories and models (Strack and Deutsch, 2004; Levesque et al., 2008), we suggest that the authors should clearly outline that their definition of automaticity reflects more conscious processes and self-reporting. De Bruijn and colleagues' proposal that automaticity is determined by the number of times a person has made a decision and goes through the processes does not necessarily imply automaticity. The experience of automaticity in de Bruijn and colleagues' study is self-reported and does not necessarily measure the reality of automaticity; people may report they do things automatically, but, in reality a lot of conscious effort goes into their actions (see Hagger et al., 2014).

What are Implicit Processes?

Given the recent rise in research focusing on non-conscious, automatic, or implicit processes, clear definitions of what each researcher means by their terms is fundamental in reducing confusion between what is meant by each term and how to measure it (Payne and Gawronski, 2010). The explicit measure used in the de Bruijn et al.'s study does not appear to assess implicit or impulsive processes for either measurement type or psychological attribute. One way of defining and using the term “implicit” refers to the assessment of individuals' psychological attributes without the need for explicitly asking people (Fazio and Olson, 2003); de Bruijn and colleagues' automaticity measure is not consistent with this definition. Other researchers have defined automaticity as constructs or systems that are assessed by particular types of measures that do not require explicit, conscious introspection (Banaji, 2001). Instead, de Bruijn and colleagues' measure appears to be something of a meta-cognitive variable that reflects participant deliberations on the extent to which their exercise behavior is automatic. We suggest that the type of automaticity de Bruijn and colleagues referred to may actually be better measured by existing implicit measures, such as the IAT.

Automaticity as Associations

de Bruijn et al. outline that “repetition in stable contexts strengthens associations between the situational cue and the behavioural action” (p. 768). While we fully endorse this interpretation of existing findings in the literature, we suggest that existing implicit tests would be a better measure of these associations (Dimmock and Banting, 2009; Conroy et al., 2010). Implicit measures provide a clear indication of whether the associations between cue and action are influenced by automatic, implicit or deliberative processes. Measures of implicit constructs such as the IAT (Greenwald et al., 1998) or go no-go tasks (Nosek and Banaji, 2001), for example, that measure the speed of association between targets and categories would be more fit-for-purpose to tap automaticity as it is defined by de Bruijn and colleagues. Such tests could be adopted to measure the extent to which associations between contexts and behaviors (e.g., leisure time context and exercise behaviors) are automatic; this could be used concurrently with the measure of automaticity used by de Bruijn and colleagues' to provide some concurrent validity.

Measurement Issues

One pertinent issue in research involving implicit and explicit measures is measurement correspondence. In de Bruijn and colleagues' study, it may be that the explicit measure of automaticity is predicted by the explicit predictor variables due to the correspondence of measurement type. The current trend appears to be that implicit measures better predict spontaneous outcomes and behaviors than planned, self-reported behaviors (Perugini et al., 2010, 2011). Similarly, explicit, self-report measures seem better suited to predicting reflective, self-report behaviors. In de Bruijn and colleagues study, even though the explicit measure of automaticity was measured 2 weeks after the baseline assessment of past behavior and social cognitive variables, the study could not exclude the possibility that common method variance (Spector, 2006) and semantic similarity (Arnulf et al., 2014) inflated the associations between the study variables. It is important that future research should include implicit measures when examining the relationship between non-conscious processes and social cognitive variables. Furthermore, previous research has shown that objectively-measured behaviors (e.g., accelerometer for physical activity, free choice task for motivation) are better predicted by implicit measures (Keatley et al., 2013a). Future research should therefore incorporate alternative objectively-measured outcome variables, which may be better measured by implicit measures. However, it should be noted that implicit measures, such as the IAT, are not process-pure, they are subject to the influence of control processes and biases. There are, however, models (e.g., process dissociation analyses) that have been developed to “purify” the measure of automatic associations (e.g., Payne, 2001).

Conclusion

While we support the research conducted by de Bruijn and colleagues, we contend that there are limitations in the conceptualization of automaticity and the results presented. Given the definition of automaticity outlined by the authors, existing measure of implicit associations appear to be better suited to measure automaticity. We therefore think the research would be improved by the inclusion of implicit measures. Furthermore, we encourage future research to include outcome variables of automaticity that may be better predicted by related implicit measures. Finally, researchers using longitudinal designs should be mindful to not allow mere measurement effects or measure correspondence or common method variance to affect results.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Arnulf, J. K., Larsen, K. R., Martinsen, Ø. L., and Bong, C. H. (2014). Predicting survey responses: how and why semantics shape survey statistics on organizational behaviour. PLoS ONE 9:e106361. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106361

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banaji, M. R. (2001). “Implicit attitudes can be measured,” in The Nature of Remembering: Essays in Remembering Robert G. Crowder, eds H. L. Roediger, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, and A. Surprenant (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association). 117–150.

Google Scholar

Bargh, J. A. (1994). “The four horsemen of automaticity: awareness, efficiency, intention, and control in social cognition,” in Handbook of Social Cognition, 2nd Edn., eds R. S. Jr. Wyer and T. K. Srull (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 1–40.

Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hagger, M. S., Biddle, S. J. H., Smith, B., and Wang, J. C. K. (2003). A meta-analysis of perceived locus of causality in exercise, sport, and physical education contexts. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 25, 284–306.

Google Scholar

Conroy, D. E., Hyde, A. L., Doerksen, S. E., and Ribeiro, N. F. (2010). Implicit attitudes and explicit motivation prospectively predict physical activity. Ann. Behav. Med. 39, 112–118. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9161-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Bruijn, G. J., Gardner, B., van Osch, L., and Sniehotta, F. F. (2014). Predicting automaticity in exercise behaviour: the role of perceived behavioural control, affect, intention, action planning, and behaviour. Int. J. Behav. Med. 21, 767–774. doi: 10.1007/s12529-013-9348-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dimmock, J. A., and Banting, L. K. (2009). The influence of implicit cognitive processes on physical activity: how the theory of planned behaviour and self-determination theory can provide a platform for our understanding. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2, 3–22. doi: 10.1080/17509840802657337

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fazio, R. H., and Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: their meaning and use. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54, 297–327. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., and Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1464–1480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., and Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2009). Integrating the theory of planned behaviour and self-determination theory in health behaviour: a meta-analysis. Br. J. Health Psychol. 14, 275–302. doi: 10.1348/135910708X373959

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., Rebar, A. L., Mullan, B., Lipp, O. V., and Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2014). The subjective experience of habit captured by self-report indexes may lead to inaccuracies in the measurement of habitual action - commentary on Gardner (2014). Health Psychol. Rev. 1–12. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2014.959728. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keatley, D. A., Clarke, D. D., and Hagger, M. S. (2012). Investigating the predictive validity of implicit and explicit measures of motivation on condom use, physical activity and healthy eating. Psychol. Health 27, 550–569. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2011.605451

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keatley, D. A., Clarke, D. D., and Hagger, M. S. (2013a). Investigating the predictive validity of implicit and explicit measures of motivation in problem-solving behavioural tasks. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 52, 510–524. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.2012.02107.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keatley, D. A., Clarke, D. D., and Hagger, M. S. (2013b). The predictive validity of implicit measures of self-determined motivation across health-related behaviours. Br. J. Health Psychol. 18, 2–17. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8287.2011.02063.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levesque, C., Copeland, K. J., and Sutcliffe, R. A. (2008). Conscious and nonconscious processes: implications for self-determination theory. Can. Psychol. Can. 49, 218–224. doi: 10.1037/a0012756

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nosek, B. A., and Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Soc. Cogn. 19, 625–666. doi: 10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: the role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 181–192. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, B. K., and Gawronski, B. (2010). “A history of implicit social cognition: where is it coming from? where is it now? where is it going?,” in Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Application, eds B. Gawronski and B. K. Payne (London: Guilford Press).

Google Scholar

Perugini, M., Conner, M., and O'Gorman, R. (2011). Automatic activation of individual differences: a test of the gatekeeper model in the domain of spontaneous helping. Eur. J. Pers. 25, 465–476. doi: 10.1002/per.826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perugini, M., Richetin, J., and Zogmaister, C. (2010). “Prediction of behavior,” in Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications, Vol. 10, eds B. Gawronski and B. K. Payne 255–278.

Google Scholar

Spector, P. E. (2006). Method variance in organizational research truth or urban legend? Organ. Res. Methods 9, 221–232. doi: 10.1177/1094428105284955

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strack, F., and Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8, 220–247. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: implicit, implicit association test, automaticity, methods, habit

Citation: Keatley DA, Chan DKC, Caudwell K, Chatzisarantis NLD and Hagger MS (2015) A consideration of what is meant by automaticity and better ways to measure it. Front. Psychol. 5:1537. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01537

Received: 22 October 2014; Accepted: 12 December 2014;
Published online: 12 January 2015.

Edited by:

Dietmar Heinke, University of Birmingham, UK

Reviewed by:

Chin Ming Hui, Chinese University of Hong Kong, China

Copyright © 2015 Keatley, Chan, Caudwell, Chatzisarantis and Hagger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: david.keatley@curtin.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.