Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 12 May 2015
Sec. Cultural Psychology

A cultural look at moral purity: wiping the face clean

  • 1Department of Marketing, Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 2State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, and Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 3Department of Psychology, Renmin University of China, Beijing, China

Morality is associated with bodily purity in the custom of many societies. Does that imply moral purity is a universal psychological phenomenon? Empirically, it has never been examined, as all prior experimental data came from Western samples. Theoretically, we suggest the answer is not so straightforward—it depends on the kind of universality under consideration. Combining perspectives from cultural psychology and embodiment, we predict a culture-specific form of moral purification. Specifically, given East Asians' emphasis on the face as a representation of public self-image, we hypothesize that facial purification should have particularly potent moral effects in a face culture. Data show that face-cleaning (but not hands-cleaning) reduces guilt and regret most effectively against a salient East Asian cultural background. It frees East Asians from guilt-driven prosocial behavior. In the wake of their immorality, they find a face-cleaning product especially appealing and spontaneously choose to wipe their face clean. These patterns highlight both culturally variable and universal aspects of moral purification. They further suggest an organizing principle that informs the vigorous debate between embodied and amodal perspectives.

Introduction

Across human societies, bodily purity is intertwined with morality. For example, ritual purification of the physical body symbolizes moral purification in all major religions around the world, from baptism of Christianity and Mikvah of Judaism, to ablution of Islam and Buddhism, to bathing in the Ganges of Hinduism and Amrit of Sikhism. In cross-national surveys, purity concerns constitute one of the foundations of moral intuition on all continents (Graham et al., 2011). Physiological and neural studies further show that physical contamination and moral violation alike activate the same facial muscles (for oral–nasal rejection; Chapman et al., 2009; Cannon et al., 2010) and overlapping brain networks (Moll et al., 2005; Borg et al., 2008), indicating biological underpinnings of the link between impurity and immorality. These observations lead one to expect that this psychological phenomenon, “moral purity” (Douglas, 1966; Zhong and Liljenquist, 2006; Williams et al., 2009), is universal. Is it?

We suspect it is and it isn't, depending on the theoretical level of universality under consideration (Norenzayan and Heine, 2005). The mere existence of the notion of moral purity is likely to be universal, given the pancultural and panreligious moralization of bodily purity. Beyond this minimal level of existential universality, however, the scientific literature is in the dark about whether different cultures are comparable or incomparable in the moral functions of purification (e.g., guilt reduction; Zhong and Liljenquist, 2006) and the effective forms of purification (e.g., washing hands, rinsing mouth; Lee and Schwarz, 2010). Although there is a rapidly growing body of experimental evidence for the diverse consequences of physical cleansing on moral emotion, judgment, and behavior (e.g., Schnall et al., 2008; Liljenquist et al., 2010; Zhong et al., 2010; Helzer and Pizarro, 2011; Huang et al., 2011; Ritter and Preston, 2011; Cramwinckel et al., 2013a,b; Xu et al., 2014), this work is completely silent on issues of universality, because all the data have come exclusively from Western samples. On the other side of the world, cleansing may have different (or the same) moral functions, and even the same moral consequences may result from different (or the same) effective forms of cleansing.

Drawing on theoretical perspectives on morality, culture, and meaning representation, we predict a distinct form of moral purification in the East. To maintain moral order, societies often invoke culturally significant meanings such as face, honor, and dignity (Leung and Cohen, 2011). Mental representations of these meanings can be grounded in bodily states and sensorimotor modalities (Barsalou et al., 2003), such as the head-high, chin-up, straightback posture of honor (IJzerman and Cohen, 2011). Among East Asians, in particular, the face is a culturally highlighted body part that represents the public image of the self. It is highly prominent in their sociomoral discourse, to the point where theorists refer to them as face cultures. Distinct from honor and dignity cultures, members of face cultures are strongly avoidant of “losing face,” which happens when they are seen acting selfishly, disloyally, or inappropriately; and they are keen on “gaining face” by moving up the social hierarchy so that others “give face” to them (Ho, 1976; Leung and Cohen, 2011). Given the face's prominent role in moral regulation and its chronic salience, face-cleaning may have particularly potent moral effects in an East Asian context, more so than in a Western context, implying a culture-specific form, but culture-general function, of moral purification.

To experimentally investigate this possibility, we first conducted a pilot study using the culture-priming approach (Oyserman and Lee, 2008). We sampled Chinese-Canadian biculturals, primed either Chinese or Canadian culture, and tested if face-cleaning effects on guilt depended on which culture was salient.

Does Face-cleaning Alleviate Guilt Most Effectively When an East Asian Culture is Salient?

In a 2 (Chinese- vs. Canadian culture-prime) × 2 (face- vs. hands-cleaning) between-subjects design (see Table 1 for sample characteristics), participants first saw the photos of six objects and places that were either characteristically Chinese (e.g., ping pong racket, soy sauce, Shanghai) or Canadian (e.g., ice hockey stick, maple syrup, Toronto), and were asked to use five words to describe the culture (Hong et al., 2000). Then all participants recalled and reported in detail a personal experience of doing something highly unethical. Next, under the pretense of a product evaluation survey, they were given an antiseptic wipe to evaluate and were either asked to try it on their face or on their hands.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics of all experiments.

Finally, all participants were asked, “How do you feel about the unethical behavior you described earlier?” They rated their feelings on seven items [0 = Not at all (emotion), 10 = Very (emotion)]. Guilty and regretful were the items of interest; they are self-conscious emotions (Lewis, 2000) of relevance to face as one's public self-image (Ho, 1976). They were embedded among filler items (sad, calm, angry, excited, anxious), none of which is a self-conscious emotion. Guilty and regretful ratings were highly correlated [r(105) = 0.77, p < 0.001] and averaged as the dependent measure of immoral feelings.

As predicted, participants who cleaned their face felt less immoral if they had been primed with Chinese (M = 3.98, SD = 2.45) than with Canadian culture (M = 5.38, SD = 3.00), planned contrast t(101) = 1.80, one-tailed p = 0.04. This pattern did not emerge among participants who cleaned their hands; instead, they showed a non-significant trend of feeling less immoral if they had been primed with Canadian (M = 4.12, SD = 2.96) than with Chinese culture (M = 5.18, SD = 2.47), t(101) = 1.46, p = 0.15. In short, the remorse-reducing effect of face-cleaning, but not of hands-cleaning, was contingent upon a salient East Asian culture [cleaning modality × primed culture F(1, 101) = 5.33, p = 0.02].

These pilot results are consistent with our prediction that wiping the face clean exerts stronger moral influence in an East Asian context. To test it more rigorously, we conducted an experiment with several goals in mind. First, to provide a neutral baseline for comparing the face- and hands-cleaning effects, we added a no-cleaning condition. Second, we included measures of immoral feelings prior to the cleaning manipulation to ensure that any cleaning effect would not be an artifact of pre-existing emotional differences between cleaning conditions. Third, going beyond immoral feelings, we examined guilt-driven prosocial behavior to see if actual behavior would be affected by physical cleansing in a culture-specific way. If cultures are identical in their effective forms of purification, then the absolving effects of hands-cleaning among Westerners—as consistently demonstrated in prior research—should generalize to East Asians. But if cultures have different effective forms of purification, then hands-cleaning may not affect East Asians' moral behavior, whereas face-cleaning may.

Does Face-cleaning in Particular Reduce Guilt-driven Prosociality among East Asians?

In a single-factor design with three (face- vs. hands- vs. no-cleaning) between-subjects conditions, participants first recalled and reported an unethical experience as in the pilot study (except that the instructions were presented on screen and the experience reported into a microphone rather than on paper). Then they rated their current feelings on the 15-item State Shame and Guilt Scale (e.g., “I want to sink into the floor and disappear”; 1 = not feeling this way at all, 5 = strongly feeling this way; Marschall et al., 1994). Next, they were given a skincare wipe allegedly for product evaluation and were asked to either try it on their face or hands or simply examine it. Afterwards, participants completed a filler arithmetic task that ostensibly concluded the experiment.

Upon “completion,” participants were asked if they would be willing to volunteer for another graduate student who was in desperate need of data but running out of budget. Whether they volunteered to help was the dependent measure. In this paradigm, helping would increase with guilt (Zhong and Liljenquist, 2006), and refusing to help would indicate less guilt.

Post-experimental probing revealed that one participant was familiar with moral purity research and suspicious about the experiment's purpose; two were interrupted by phone calls and quitted early; two failed to recall any unethical experience. They were excluded from analysis (final N = 90).

Face-cleaning participants were four times more likely than no-cleaning participants to refuse to help [33.3 vs. 6.7%; Wald − test(1) = 5.52, p = 0.02, 95% CI (0.06, 0.45)]. In contrast, hands-cleaning participants were not significantly different from no-cleaning participants in their refusal to help [16.7 vs. 6.7%; Wald − test(1) = 1.37, p = 0.24, 95% CI (−0.07, 0.28)]. This pattern could not be explained by pre-existing differences in immoral feelings because the three groups reported comparable levels of shame and guilt prior to the cleaning manipulation [Mface−cleaning = 3.02, SDface−cleaning = 0.39; Mhands−cleaning = 2.95, SDhands−cleaning = 0.39; Mno−cleaning = 3.00, SDno−cleaning = 0.38; F(2, 87) = 0.26, p = 0.77]. In short, among our East Asian participants, face-cleaning reduced guilt-driven prosocial behavior; contrary to all prior findings among Westerners, hands-cleaning did not.

The data so far suggest face-cleaning to be a culturally distinct form of purification, with consequences on guilt and prosociality. Is it such an effective coping mechanism that East Asians would even spontaneously wipe their face clean in the wake of their immorality? We tested this in a final experiment by observing people's cleaning behavior. In addition, we measured their desire and willingness to pay for a facial cleanser among a variety of consumer products, to verify if their urge to cleanse was specific to the face.

Does Immorality Specifically Elicit Face-cleaning Desire and Behavior among East Asians?

In a single-factor design with two (unethical vs. ethical recall) between-subjects conditions, participants first recalled and reported an experience of being either highly unethical or highly ethical. Three participants failed to report any unethical experience and were excluded from analysis (final N = 70). Then in an ostensibly unrelated consumer survey, they saw 20 products (Table 2), including facial cleanser, cleaning products for other specific body parts (e.g., hand wash, mouthwash), for the body in general (e.g., towel, soap), and for objects (detergent, washing powder), as well as non-cleaning products (e.g., battery, stapler). Participants indicated the desirability of each product (1 = completely undesirable, 7 = extremely desirable) and their willingness to pay for it (in Chinese ¥), which served as the first set of dependent measures.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Desirability of and willingness to pay (WTP) for each product as a function of unethical vs. ethical recall in Experiment 2.

Next, as a basic check of whether participants associated the cleaning products with various body parts in ways we expected, they rated the extent to which each bodily cleaning product was related to the face, hands, mouth, feet, and hair (1 = completely unrelated, 7 = highly related). Not surprisingly, participants across conditions rated the facial cleanser as highly related to the face, hand wash and hand moisturizer to the hands, mouthwash and toothpaste to the mouth, and other cleaning products to multiple body parts (see online Supplemental Material).

Finally, participants were given a skincare wipe for product evaluation and asked to try it (without being told how). Unbeknownst to them, which body part they started wiping was unobtrusively recorded by the experimenter. It turned out all participants started wiping either their face or hands, which served as a dichotomous behavioral measure.

As expected, recalling an unethical rather than ethical experience increased participants' desire for the facial cleanser [Munethical = 4.63, SDunethical = 1.54; Methical = 3.86, SDethical = 1.54; F(1, 68) = 4.41, p = 0.04, d = 0.51, 95% CI (0.04, 1.51)] and willingness to pay for it [log-transformed Munethical = 1.26, SDunethical = 0.22; Methical = 1.15, SDethical = 0.18; F(1, 68) = 4.93, p = 0.03, d = 0.54, 95% CI (0.01, 0.21)]. No significant effect was found on any other product (Table 2), suggesting that for East Asians immorality may specifically potentiate the appeal of face-cleaning. Indeed, participants who recalled an unethical rather than ethical experience were twice as likely to spontaneously wipe their face clean [46 vs. 23%; χ2(1, N = 70) = 4.06, p = 0.04, φ = 0.24, 95% CI (0.01, 0.42)].

General Discussion

Moral purification is manifest in a culture-specific form, distinct from all prior findings in the West. Given East Asians' emphasis on the face as a representation of their public self-image, making their cultural background salient renders face-cleaning especially effective for alleviating guilt and regret about their past misdeeds. Wiping the face clean frees them from guilt-driven compensatory prosociality. Its effectiveness as a coping mechanism is further demonstrated by their specific interest in a face-cleaning product and face-cleaning behavior in the wake of their immorality.

Implications for the Cultural Variability and Universality of Embodied Metaphors

Comparing and contrasting these findings with all prior experimental data on moral purity suggests that it is a psychological phenomenon with both culturally variable and potentially universal aspects. At one level, the moral benefits of hands-cleaning are clearer among Westerners than East Asians, for whom face-cleaning works better (presumably because the facial modality is chronically salient; see next section). This pattern indicates potential cultural variability in the most effective forms of purification. At another level, immorality does elicit cleansing desire and actual cleansing does reduce guilt among East Asians as well. Even though the strongest claim of universality is to be withheld until worldwide experimentation (Norenzayan and Heine, 2005), finding that physical cleansing produces the same kind of affective, judgmental, and behavioral consequences among Easterners and Westerners alike—despite their substantially different values, beliefs, self-views, relational styles, cognitive, and perceptual foci (Oyserman et al., 2002)—reinforces the possibility that moral purification, while culture-specific in form, is culture-general in functions and existence.

Embodied metaphors such as Morality Is Purity, Affection Is Warmth, and Importance Is Weight may appear to be universal phenomena, given their supposed evolutionary origins (Williams et al., 2009) or pancultural presence in languages, taboos, or rituals (Douglas, 1966; Kövecses, 2000; Zhong and Liljenquist, 2006). Even if their existence is universal, however, their psychological functions or effective forms may still vary. The level at which these and other embodied metaphors fundamental to social life are culture-specific or culture-general remains a wide-open empirical question.

Implications for Embodied vs. Amodal Perspectives

Facial cleaning has particularly potent effects among East Asians, for whom the face is a chronically highlighted modality. Juxtaposing these and prior findings suggests a possible organizing principle: moral purification is specific to the salient modality. The salience can be chronic, as in a face culture. Or it can be temporary, as when saying vs. doing something unethical makes people want to clean their “dirty mouth” vs. “dirty hands” (oral vs. manual modality; Lee and Schwarz, 2010).

This principle of modality salience is compatible with models that ground knowledge in modality-specific systems (Barsalou et al., 2003). It may be generalized to predict that embodied metaphors exert the strongest influence via whichever modality is most salient in a situation, to an individual, or for a culture. For example, moral effects of hands-cleaning are particularly powerful for individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder who are chronically compelled to wash their hands (Reuven et al., 2013). Because the causal role of modality salience is predicted a priori by embodied models but only accommodated post hoc in amodal models, providing evidence for or against its robustness holds promise for informing the vigorous debate between embodied and amodal perspectives.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

SL acknowledges generous support by the Netherlands Institute for Advanced Study while preparing this article. This research was also supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31170971, 61210010) and the Major Project of National Social Science Foundation of China (12&ZD228) to CL, and the Open Research Fund of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning (CNLYB1212) to XM. We are grateful to Chen-Bo Zhong, Phoebe C. Ellsworth, and Julie Y. Huang for thoughtful comments on earlier drafts. SL and HT made equal contributions. SL developed the research concept. The pilot experiment was designed by SL and JW; testing, data collection, and analysis were performed by JW. Experiments 1 and 2 were designed by SL, HT, and CL; testing, data collection, and analysis were performed by HT, XM, and CL. SL drafted the paper and other authors provided revisions. All authors approved the final version of the paper for submission.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00577/abstract

References

Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, K. W., Barbey, A. K., and Wilson, C. D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 84–91. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00029-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borg, J. S., Lieberman, D., and Kiehl, K. A. (2008). Infection, incest, and iniquity: investigating the neural correlates of disgust and morality. J. Cognitive Neurosci. 20, 1529–1546. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cannon, P. R., Schnall, S., and White, M. (2010). Transgressions and expressions: affective facial muscle activity predicts moral judgments. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2, 325–331. doi: 10.1177/1948550610390525

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, H. A., Kim, D. A., Susskind, J. M., and Anderson, A. K. (2009). In bad taste: evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science 323, 1222–1226. doi: 10.1126/science.1165565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cramwinckel, F. M., De Cremer, D., and van Dijke, M. (2013a). Dirty hands make dirty leaders?! The effects of touching dirty objects on rewarding unethical subordinates as a function of a leader's self-interest. J. Bus. Ethics 115, 93–100. doi: 10.1007/s10551-012-1385-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cramwinckel, F. M., van Dijk, E., Scheepers, D., and van den Bos, K. (2013b). The threat of moral refusers for one's self-concept and the protective function of physical cleansing. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 1049–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, M. (1966). Purity and Danger: An Analysis of Concepts of Taboo and Pollution. London: Routledge & K. Paul. doi: 10.4324/9780203361832

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., and Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 366–385. doi: 10.1037/a0021847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IJzerman, H., and Cohen, D. (2011). Grounding cultural syndromes: body comportment and values in honor and dignity cultures. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 41, 456–467. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.806

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helzer, E. G., and Pizarro, D. A. (2011). Dirty liberals! Reminders of physical cleanliness influence moral and political attitudes. Psychol. Sci. 22, 517–522. doi: 10.1177/0956797611402514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, D. Y. (1976). On the concept of face. Am. J. Sociol. 81, 867–884. doi: 10.1086/226145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C., and Benet-Martínez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: a dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. Am. Psychol. 55, 709–720. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.7.709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, J. Y., Sedlovskaya, A., Ackerman, J. M., and Bargh, J. A. (2011). Immunizing against prejudice: effects of disease protection on attitudes toward out-groups. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1550–1556. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kövecses, Z. (2000). Metaphor and Emotion: Language, Culture, and Body in Human Feeling. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Lee, S. W. S., and Schwarz, N. (2010). Dirty hands and dirty mouths: embodiment of the moral-purity metaphor is specific to the motor modality involved in moral transgression. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1423–1425. doi: 10.1177/0956797610382788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, A. K. Y., and Cohen, D. (2011). Within-and between-culture variation: individual differences and the cultural logics of honor, face, and dignity cultures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 507–526. doi: 10.1037/a0022151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, M. (2000). “Self-conscious emotions: embarrassment, pride, shame, and guilt,” in Handbook of Emotions, eds M. Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones (New York, NY: Guilford), 623–636.

Liljenquist, K., Zhong, C. B., and Galinsky, A. D. (2010). The smell of virtue: clean scents promote reciprocity and charity. Psychol. Sci. 21, 381–383. doi: 10.1177/0956797610361426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marschall, D., Sanftner, J., and Tangney, J. (1994). The State Shame and Guilt Scale. Fairfax, VA: George Mason University.

Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, F. T., Ignácio, F. A., Bramati, I. E., Caparelli-Dáquer, E. M., et al. (2005). The moral affiliations of disgust: a functional MRI study. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 18, 68–78. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000152236.46475.a7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norenzayan, A., and Heine, S. J. (2005). Psychological universals: what are they and how can we know? Psychol. Bull. 131, 763–784. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., and Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychol. Bull. 128, 3–72. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oyserman, D., and Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychol. Bull. 134, 311–342. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuven, O., Liberman, N., and Dar, R. (2013). The effect of physical cleaning on threatened morality in individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2, 224–229. doi: 10.1177/2167702613485565

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritter, R. S., and Preston, J. L. (2011). Gross gods and icky atheism: disgust responses to rejected religious beliefs. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 1225–1230. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnall, S., Benton, J., and Harvey, S. (2008). With a clean conscience: cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1219–1222. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02227.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, L. E., Huang, J. Y., and Bargh, J. A. (2009). The scaffolded mind: higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 39, 1257–1267. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, H., Bègue, L., and Bushman, B. J. (2014). Washing the guilt away: effects of personal versus vicarious cleansing on guilty feelings and prosocial behavior. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:97. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhong, C. B., and Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science 313, 1451. doi: 10.1126/science.1130726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhong, C.-B., Strejcek, B., and Sivanathan, N. (2010). A clean self can render harsh moral judgment. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 859–862. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: morality, purity, embodiment, metaphor, face, culture

Citation: Lee SWS, Tang H, Wan J, Mai X and Liu C (2015) A cultural look at moral purity: wiping the face clean. Front. Psychol. 6:577. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00577

Received: 05 December 2014; Accepted: 20 April 2015;
Published: 12 May 2015.

Edited by:

Andrea Bender, University of Bergen, Norway

Reviewed by:

Chris Sinha, Lund University, Sweden
John A. Bargh, Yale University, USA

Copyright © 2015 Lee, Tang, Wan, Mai and Liu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Spike W. S. Lee, Department of Marketing, Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto, 105 St. George Street, Toronto, ON M5S 3E6, Canada, spike.lee@utoronto.ca;
Chao Liu, Beijing Normal University, 19 Xinjiekou Wai Street, 100875 Beijing, China, liuchao@bnu.edu.cn

The first two authors contributed equally.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.