Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 14 July 2015
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Impact of Learning to Read on Visual Processing View all 10 articles

Editorial: The impact of learning to read on visual processing

  • 1Faculdade de Psicologia, Universidade de Lisboa, Lisboa, Portugal
  • 2Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique - FNRS, Brussels, Belgium
  • 3Unité de Recherche en Neurosciences Cognitives, Center for Research in Cognition and Neurosciences, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium

In 1892, Déjerine published the first report of pure alexia (Déjerine, 1892). Monsieur C. became unable to read in the absence of any other cognitive disorder (even writing was preserved) after a lesion of the inferior occipitotemporal cortex, a neural region dedicated to visual recognition. Although reading is an intense visual ability, the relation between reading and visual processing has often been sell short. It was only ~100 years after the report of Monsieur C. that part of this occipitotemporal region was coined visual word-form area, VWFA (Warrington and Shallice, 1980; see also Cohen et al., 2000; Polk and Farah, 2002). Since then an emergent bulk of research has demonstrated that learning to read, not only leads to the emergence of a specialized neurocognitive circuitry, but also impacts on the evolutionary older and pre-existing neurocognitive system of visual (non-linguistic) object recognition. Many questions regarding the exact nature, locus, and consequences of this impact are in debate or still unanswered. This Research Topic was aimed at setting a landmark forum on which researchers present and discuss recent work, their proposals, and open novel questions. We have compiled nine excellent articles on the relation between visual processing and literacy acquisition, reading development, and developmental dyslexia. This research topic is organized into three parts.

In the first part, opening this research topic, in an opinion article, Zhou et al. (2014) consider the relation between visual skills and learning to read, and the moderator role of the visual complexity of the written script in this equation (e.g., Chinese makes stronger demands of visual skills due to its complexity than alphabetic scripts). Qian and Bi (2014) argue that the visual complexity of the script modulates the expression of visual processing deficits (namely, in magnocellular processing) in developmental dyslexia. They examined the association between motion processing (in a coherent motion task, underpinned by V5/MT functioning) and reading (in a visual lexical decision task) in Chinese dyslexic children and chronological-age controls.

Second, regarding the emergence of a neurocognitive system specialized in letter processing, in a hypothesis and theory article, Lachmann and van Leeuwen (2014) propose the functional coordination approach. According to this hypothesis learning to read captures the analytic strategy of visual processing, which was already available before literacy took place, but then becomes the preference mode in letter processing. In their research article, Lachmann et al. (2014) used the Navon test to examine whether, when the hierarchical stimulus (a global figure composed of local figures) is presented at fixation with dimensions close to those in written text, letters compared to non-letters are processed using an analytic strategy instead of the usual holistic strategy adopted on hierarchical stimuli.

In the last part of this Research Topic, the impact of literacy on non-linguistic visual processing is considered. Indeed, according to the neuronal recycling hypothesis (Dehaene, 2009) the ventral occipitotemporal regions, originally devoted to object recognition, are partially recycled to accommodate literacy, with spillover effects on the former function. In a large-scale developmental study, Santi et al. (2015) show that the impact of learning to read on visual skills is not observed at a macro behavioral level assessed with general educational/neuropsychological tests. Note, however, that studies that reported an impact of literacy on general spatial skills have examined children learning to read scripts differing on visual complexity (e.g., Zhou et al., 2014, in this research topic), but this was not the case in Santi et al.: all children were learning the alphabetic English orthography. This might seem, however, inconsistent with the neuronal recycling hypothesis (Dehaene, 2009). Indeed, a key question, discussed in the last four articles of this collection, is to understand which aspects of visual processing are actually affected by literacy acquisition and why. Possibly only the visual properties that collide with learning to read are affected. This is the case of mirror invariance: lateral mirror images, such as d and b, are originally processed as equivalent percepts. Kolinsky and Fernandes (2014; following the prior work of Pegado et al., 2014) examined whether learning to read is able to modify the object recognition system as expressed by a loss of mirror invariance, by comparing the orientation cost for mirror images (e.g., ⌉-⌈) vs. plane-rotations (to which the visual system is originally sensitive to; e.g., ⌉-⌊), in identity-based same-different judgments of illiterate, late literate, and early literate adults. In the same vein, using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) during identity-based same-different judgments, Nakamura et al. (2014) demonstrated the causal role of the left occipitotemporal cortex (comprising the VWFA) in mirror discrimination of visual words by literate Japanese adults. In their opinion article, Pegado et al. (2014) set a multisystem learning framework to answer how mirror discrimination is acquired during learning to read. They propose that a tight functional link between the visual and motor systems is crucial for this acquisition. Finally, given that literacy acquisition also impacts on face recognition due to competition for neural space (cf. Dehaene, 2009), in an opinion article, Ventura (2014) reviews these evidence, discusses the possible reasons for this competition, and proposes new directions considering literacy as a form of visual expertise.

Taken together, these articles represent an update overview and demonstrate the diversity of approaches in this research topic: miscellaneous scientific backgrounds (e.g., neuroscience, in Nakamura et al., 2014; neuropsychology, in Qian and Bi, 2014; developmental psychology, in Santi et al., 2015; experimental psychology; in Kolinsky and Fernandes, 2014), several techniques (e.g., TMS, Nakamura et al., 2014; behavioral tests, Lachmann et al., 2014; item response models, Santi et al., 2015), various written scripts considered (i.e., studies with alphabetic and non-alphabetic scripts; e.g., Lachmann et al., 2014; Nakamura et al., 2014, respectively), different populations examined (typical vs. dyslexic readers, in Qian and Bi, 2014; adults of varying schooling and literacy levels, in Kolinsky and Fernandes, 2014). These articles are Dejerine's legacy as pieces of the (still incomplete) puzzle on the impact of literacy on visual processing, which will hopefully contribute to understand the reasons behind this impact.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this Research Topic and TF are supported by IF 2013 Program of the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology, FCT (ref IF/00886/2013/CP1194/CT0002). RK is Research Director of the Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique-FNRS, Belgium, and her work is supported by the Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique-FNRS under grant FRFC 2.4515.12 and by an Interuniversity Attraction Poles grant “IAP 7/33,” Belspo. We are very grateful to all authors that have contributed to this research topic.

References

Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Lehericy, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Henaff, M. A., et al. (2000). The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain 123(Pt 2), 291–307. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.2.291

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the Brain: The New Science of How We Read. New York, NY: Penguin.

Déjerine, J. J. (1892). Contribution à l'étude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des différentes variétés de cécité verbale. Mém. Soc. Biol. 4, 61–90.

Kolinsky, R., and Fernandes, T. (2014). A cultural side effect: learning to read interferes with identity processing of familiar objects. Front. Psychol. 5:1224. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lachmann, T., Schmitt, A., Braet, W., and van Leeuwen, C. (2014). Letters in the forest: global precedence effect disappears for letters but not for non-letters under reading-like conditions. Front. Psychol. 5:705. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lachmann, T., and van Leeuwen, C. (2014). Reading as functional coordination: not recycling but a novel synthesis. Front. Psychol. 5:1046. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, K., Makuuchi, M., and Nakajima, Y. (2014). Mirror-image discrimination in the literate brain: a causal role for the left occpitotemporal cortex. Front. Psychol. 5:478. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pegado, F., Nakamura, K., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Filho, G. N., Pallier, C., et al. (2014). Literacy breaks mirror invariance for visual stimuli: a behavioral study with adult illiterates. J. Exp. Psychol. 143, 887–894. doi: 10.1037/a0033198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polk, T. A., and Farah, M. L. (2002). Functional MRI evidence for an abstract, not perceptual, word-form area. J. Exp. Psychol. 131, 65–72. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qian, Y., and Bi, H. Y. (2014). The visual magnocellular deficit in Chinese-speaking children with developmental dyslexia. Front. Psychol. 5:692. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santi, K. L., Kulesz, P. A., Khalaf, S., and Francis, D. J. (2015). Developmental changes in reading do not alter the development of visual processing skills: an application of explanatory item response models in grades K-2. Front. Psychol. 6:116. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ventura, P. (2014). Let's face it: reading acquisition, face and word processing. Front. Psychol. 5:787. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warrington, E. K., and Shallice, T. (1980). Word-form dyslexia. Brain 103, 99–112.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Zhou, Y., McBride-Chang, C., and Wong, N. (2014). What is the role of visual skills in learning to read? Front. Psychol. 5:776. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: the impact of learning to read on visual processing, literacy acquisition, reading development, developmental dyslexia, visual processing, visual object recognition

Citation: Fernandes T and Kolinsky R (2015) Editorial: The impact of learning to read on visual processing. Front. Psychol. 6:985. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00985

Received: 08 June 2015; Accepted: 29 June 2015;
Published: 14 July 2015.

Edited by:

Jessica S. Horst, University of Sussex, UK

Reviewed by:

Carmel Houston-Price, University of Reading Malaysia, Malaysia

Copyright © 2015 Fernandes and Kolinsky. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tânia Fernandes, taniapgfernandes@gmail.com; tpfernandes@psicologia.ulisboa.pt

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.