Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 17 July 2015
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Frontiers in the Acquisition of Literacy View all 14 articles

Editorial: Frontiers in the acquisition of literacy

  • School of Psychology, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand

Reading and writing are fundamental to full participation in our societies, yet how children acquire such a large system of interconnected representations of print words, their meanings, and phonology in the brain remains unclear. As the teaching of literacy takes up a large proportion of classroom time in the early years, increasing knowledge about children's learning processes should result in better approaches to the teaching of reading and spelling. These insights would be particularly useful from a clinical perspective for the treatment of developmental disabilities, such as dyslexia and dysgraphia.

Important questions need to be addressed, and given the many different influences and overlapping processes on literacy learning, the answers are not straightforward. What are the learning processes? Are they the same across different orthographies, or do different orthographies require different skills and learning processes? What is the relationship between reading and spelling? How do they interact and augment each other? What is the effect of different teaching approaches on children's emerging reading system? Do early reading comprehension problems disappear over time? What are the predictors of children's reading attainment?

This E-Book responds to some of these general questions in the form of original research and opinion articles. Taken together the articles contribute new perspectives and challenges to current reading acquisition theories, and present new research on early reading skills, reading instruction, and spelling.

Thompson (2014a) claims that most reading acquisition theories are limited by their specification of letter-sound requirements to a particular class of teaching approaches. Acknowledging such limitations is an important step in the development of reading acquisition theories that are potentially more useful. Fletcher-Flinn (2014) merges ideas from dynamic systems theory with developmental data from a precocious reader. Reference is made to Knowledge Systems theory, which offers a more varied range of theoretical applicability.

Share (2014) asserts that there is a general belief in the superiority of alphabetic writing systems that has hindered progress in the development of a universal model of learning to read. Some counterevidence is presented, and in the context of a more general question on “optimality,” Share proposes a universal model of reading based on a broader novice-to-expert dualism. Nag (2014) maintains that specification of the learning mechanisms involved in reading akshara units, the symbols used in many writing systems (alphasyllabaries) of Southern Asia, present a challenge to alphabet-based theories of reading acquisition. The akshara units, unlike alphabet letters, map onto multiple sublexical levels of phonology determined by context. In order to ensure the development of an inclusive reading science, and a more comprehensive and universal theory of literacy learning, Nag argues that consideration of these orthographic-specific features of reading are needed. At the same time, it is possible that some general cognitive features of information processing, as they relate to reading acquisition, may be orthography-independent. Colé et al. (2014) provides evidence that early progress in young French children in both word reading and reading comprehension was related to cognitive flexibility in the coordination of orthographic, phonological, and semantic information.

How early, and in what form does SES as a distal predictor of reading achievement manifest itself? Robins et al. (2014) found that lower SES parents of preschoolers asked fewer questions about letters, and focused more on memorizing sequences of the alphabet than higher SES parents. The persistence of a conversational focus on letters within the child's name also differentiated the groups. These differences could put lower SES children at a disadvantage when entering school, resulting in poor rates of literacy. Tse and Nicholson (2014) addressed the performance gap of low SES children in New Zealand schools with an intervention comparing three teaching approaches: Big Book (shared reading), explicit instruction in phoneme awareness and phonics, and a combined approach. The latter produced better results on a range of literacy measures compared with the combined averaged scores of the other two groups. Thompson (2014b) took the opportunity to comment on ambiguities that are often unrecognized but affect the validity of such intervention research, and Nicholson and Tse (2015) provide a rebuttal. These discussions are thoughtful contributions to methodological issues in intervention research.

How do fluent readers distinguish between words that look similar but whose meaning differ? Using masked form priming, Bhide et al. (2014) found no evidence that increases in print vocabulary size predicted precise orthographic representations, and suggested spelling skill might be more important. Ouellette and Tims (2014) examined whether this “spelling advantage” might be due to the motoric component of writing. There was no effect of modality (printing or typing) for Grade two children, suggesting that stored orthographic detail is independent of input. Of interest, pre-existing keyboard skills affected learning.

With regard to literacy impairments, Critten et al. (2014) found no difference for the spelling of words with inflectional morphemes by children with specific language impairment (SLI) and spelling-matched controls. However, the SLI group was less accurate when spelling words with derivational morphemes. The authors conclude that this indicates a specific impairment when making orthographic and phonological shifts from base words. This outcome has useful teaching applications for SLI children. Ricketts et al. (2014) showed that children identified at 9 years with poor reading comprehension had lower educational achievement at 11 and 16 years than a reading (decoding and comprehension), and non-verbal reasoning matched group, and were below national performance norms. They point out that these children are at risk from an early age of a compromised future with regard to further training and employment.

This E-Book contains an excellent collection of cutting edge scientific research and opinions at the frontiers of literacy acquisition. The reader will find new perspectives and questions derived from the reported findings, and these can serve as a springboard for new research in this field.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

I thank all of the contributors to this research topic and reviewers for their time, effort, and particularly for sharing their research and opinions to make this a successful project.

References

Bhide, A., Schlaggar, B. L., and Barnes, K. A. (2014). Developmental differences in masked form priming are not driven by vocabulary growth. Front. Psychol. 5:667. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colé, P., Duncan, L. G., and Blaye, A. (2014). Cognitive flexibility predicts early reading skills. Front. Psychol. 5:565. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critten, S., Connelly, V., Dockrell, J. E., and Walter, K. (2014). Inflectional and derivational morphological spelling abilities of children with specific language impairment. Front. Psychol. 5:948. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fletcher-Flinn, C. M. (2014). Learning to read as the formation of a dynamic system: evidence for dynamic stability in phonological recoding. Front. Psychol. 5:660. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nag, S. (2014). Alphabetism and the science of reading: from the perspective of the akshara languages. Front. Psychol. 5:866. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholson, T., and Tse, L. (2015). What are the criteria for a good intervention study? Response: “Unrecognized ambiguities in validity of intervention research: an example on explicit phonics and text-centered teaching.” Front. Psychol. 6:508. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouellette, G., and Tims, T. (2014). The write way to spell: printing vs. typing effects on orthographic learning. Front. Psychol. 5:117. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ricketts, J., Sperring, R., and Nation, K. (2014). Educational attainment in poor comprehenders. Front. Psychol. 5:445. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robins, S., Ghosh, D., Rosales, N., and Treiman, R. (2014). Letter knowledge in parent–child conversations: differences between families differing in socio-economic status. Front. Psychol. 5:632. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Share, D. (2014). Alphabetism in reading science. Front. Psychol. 5:752. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, G. B. (2014a). Discovering and accounting for limitations in applications of theories of word reading acquisition. Front. Psychol. 5:579. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, G. B. (2014b). Unrecognized ambiguities in validity of intervention research: an example on explicit phonics and text-centered teaching. Front. Psychol. 5:1535. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tse, L., and Nicholson, T. (2014). The effect of phonics-enhanced Big Book reading on the language and literacy skills of 6-year-old pupils of different reading ability attending lower SES schools. Front. Psychol. 5:1222. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: reading acquisition theory, alphabetism, predictors of reading, spelling, reading intervention and methodology, reading comprehension

Citation: Fletcher-Flinn CM (2015) Editorial: Frontiers in the acquisition of literacy. Front. Psychol. 6:1019. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01019

Received: 12 May 2015; Accepted: 06 July 2015;
Published: 17 July 2015.

Edited and reviewed by: Eddy J. Davelaar, Birkbeck, University of London, UK

Copyright © 2015 Fletcher-Flinn. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Claire M. Fletcher-Flinn, cm.fletcher-flinn@auckland.ac.nz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.