Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 13 October 2015
Sec. Cognitive Science

What is special about our own face? Commentary: Tuning of temporo-occipital activity by frontal oscillations during virtual mirror exposure causes erroneous self-recognition

  • Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, UK

A commentary on
Tuning of temporo-occipital activity by frontal oscillations during virtual mirror exposure causes erroneous self-recognition

by Serino, A., Sforza, A. L., Kanayama, N., van Elk, M., Kaliuzhna, M., Herbelin, B., et al. (2015). Eur. J. Neurosci. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13029. [Epub ahead of print].

‘What are you doing?’, my wife asked, seeing me linger, unusually in front of the mirror.
‘Nothing’, I replied. ‘Just looking at myself, at my nose, here, inside this nostril. When I press it, I feel a little pain.’
My wife smiled and said: ‘I thought you were looking to see which way it tilts.’
I wheeled around like a dog whose tail has been stepped on.
‘Tilts? My nose?’

Pirandello (1990)

Observing one's face in the mirror provides an immediate strong sense of self. The ability to recognize one's face has been taken as an index of self-awareness and has been considered a fundamental aspect within the spectrum of selfhood (Gallup, 1970; Rochat, 2009). However, what is considered the most representative instance of personal identity is probably the less reliable representation of the self; the rather infrequent encounters we have with our own face are in fact mediated by a reflecting surface and likely distorted by a variety of expectations shaped by ourselves and others (Rochat and Zahavi, 2011). How do we balance this information in order to maintain a contentment and stable mental representation of our own identity? A new study by Serino et al. (2015) provides evidence of the neural mechanisms that mediate the balance between online multisensory cues and offline stored self-representation for the update of self-face recognition.

That self-face representation is plastic and susceptible to changes is not to be news. Recent studies (Tsakiris, 2008; Paladino et al., 2010; Sforza et al., 2010; Tajadura-Jiménez et al., 2012a,b) have applied the principles of multisensory integration of body-related signals (e.g., Botvinick and Cohen, 1998; Tsakiris and Haggard, 2005) to induce a sense of identification with a new face by creating the Enfacement Illusion (EI). In the EI, the sight of an unfamiliar face being touched on her cheek together with the participant's own face creates a “mirror-like” experience and elicits a change in self-face recognition. The perfect correspondence between what participants see and what they feel on their face updates the offline stored self-representation, albeit the face identity they see is a different one. As a result, the other person's face observed during the Enfacement becomes included in the mental representation of one's own face (Tsakiris, 2008). While Apps et al. (2015) used fMRI to explore the neural underpinnings of self-identification and self-updating, Serino et al. (2015) now provide further evidence of the role of online multisensory processing in self-face recognition. This research makes a relevant contribution to the state of art in two fundamental ways. First, by employing visuo-motor information (rather than the classic visuo-tactile feedback used in the EI), the authors investigate the role of the most commonly experienced multisensory information when facing one's own mirror reflection. By adopting a virtual-mirror setup, Serino et al. (2015) show that the visuo-motor correspondence between self and avatar's movements elicits a change in self-face recognition. In line with the EI, after being exposed to visuo-motor synchrony, participants report the other face as resembling more one's own face. Second, this study provides crucial evidence of the mediating effect of the online multisensory mechanism on the offline self-representation by showing that sensory-motor and inferotemporal-occipital areas of the cortex are both involved in self-face recognition (Serino et al., 2015).

These converging neuroimaging findings corroborate the existing evidence that in adults the mental representation of one's face can be altered by multisensory processing, but what happens when this offline self-representation is not yet in place? Different psychological accounts have historically tried to explain the developmental markers of self-face recognition. The reference paradigm used in most of these studies is the “mirror-test,” which consists in placing a red spot in the child's forehead or nose and test whether, when faced with a mirror, the child notices the spot (Gallup, 1970; Amsterdam, 1972). Children's successful attempt to reach for the spot is taken as evidence of self-recognition. Despite the extensive research done using Gallup's mirror-test for the investigation of self-recognition in chimpanzees (Gallup, 1970), dolphins (Parker et al., 1994), elephants (Plotnik et al., 2006), and human infants (e.g., Amsterdam, 1972; Bertenthal and Fischer, 1978; Povinelli et al., 1996), the paradigm has been criticized because of the high variability of performance across ages (Rochat, 2007, 2009) and cultures (Broesch et al., 2010), and its strict criteria (Bahrick et al., 1996). With regard to the latter point, the idea that self-consciousness is a unitary phenomenon, which can be measured through a “yes/no” task, has been rejected by psychologists and philosophers. In fact, in order to recognize themselves, infants must rely on the exploration of the perfect correspondence of sensory information and match the seen featural cues of the visual appearance to the self. The mirror-test disregards the relationship between these two complex components. While recent evidence suggests that some predisposition to multisensory body-related cues is present soon after birth (Filippetti et al., 2013, 2015), the presence of an in-built representation of self-appearance is unlikely, and infants encountering a mirror must match the motor information with the reflected visual output and eventually link the featural cues of the reflection with themselves.

Serino and colleagues' findings (2015) provide a fundamental starting point for research on the developmental processes involved in self-recognition and self-other differentiation. On one hand, their evidence of the presence of neural mechanisms responsible of the mediation between online and offline self-recognition in adults can pave the way for future investigation on the brain processes involved in the developing self in infancy and childhood. Additionally, evidence from neuroimaging can disentangle the respective roles of physical (e.g., self-reflection) and social (e.g., imitative interaction) mirrors, by exploring how the infant brain responds to variations in multisensory correspondences and visual appearance. At present, the developmental origins of self/other differentiation are still unknown and one of the pressing questions is whether the self emerges as a consequence of repetitive mirroring interactions with others, rather than as a-priori fundament of human beings (Prinz, 2013). While significant information about the self can be exploited from interactions with others, I posit that self-recognition is the result of both self-exploration and exchanges with others. Through a process of balance between multisensory processing and experience with one's facial appearance, sensory-motor and face-related areas of the cortex mediate this interaction. The findings from Serino et al. (2015) provide the starting point of investigation for future developmental research in this direction.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Prof. Manos Tsakiris for his insightful comments. This work was supported by the European Research Council Starting Investigator Grant (ERC-2010-StG-262853).

References

Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before age two. Dev. Psychobiol. 5, 297–305. doi: 10.1002/dev.420050403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Apps, M. A., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Sereno, M., Blanke, O., and Tsakiris, M. (2015). Plasticity in unimodal and multimodal brain areas reflects multisensory changes in self-face identification. Cereb. Cortex 25, 46–55. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahrick, L. E., Moss, L., and Fadil, C. (1996). The development of visual self-recognition in infancy. Ecol. Psychol. 8, 189–208. doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco0803_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertenthal, B. I., and Fischer, K. W. (1978). Development of self-recognition in the infant. Dev. Psychol. 14, 44. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.14.1.44

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M., and Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands' feel'touch that eyes see. Nature 391, 756–756. doi: 10.1038/35784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broesch, T., Callaghan, T., Henrich, J., Murphy, C., and Rochat, P. (2010). Cultural variations in children's mirror self-recognition. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 42, 1018–1029. doi: 10.1177/0022022110381114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filippetti, M. L., Johnson, M. H., Lloyd-Fox, S., Dragovic, D., and Farroni, T. (2013). Body perception in newborns. Curr. Biol. 23, 2413–2416. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filippetti, M. L., Orioli, G., Johnson, M. H., and Farroni, T. (2015). Newborn body perception: sensitivity to spatial congruency. Infancy 20, 455–465. doi: 10.1111/infa.12083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallup, G. G. Jr. (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science 167, 86–87. doi: 10.1126/science.167.3914.86

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paladino, M. P., Mazzurega, M., Pavani, F., and Schubert, T. W. (2010). Synchronous multisensory stimulation blurs self-other boundaries. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1202–1207. doi: 10.1177/0956797610379234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. W., and Boccia, M. L. (1994). “Expanding dimensions of the self: through the looking glass and beyond,” in Self-Awareness in Animals and Humans–Developmental Perspectives, eds S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, and M. L. Boccia (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 166–186.

Pirandello, L. (1990). One, No One and One Hundred Thousand. Transl. by W. Weaver. Boston, MA: Eridanos Press, Inc.

Plotnik, J. M., de Waal, F. B., and Reiss, D. (2006). Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 103, 17053–17057. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608062103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Povinelli, D. J., Landau, K. R., and Perilloux, H. K. (1996). Self-recognition in young children using delayed versus live feedback: evidence of a developmental asynchrony. Child Dev. 67, 1540–1554. doi: 10.2307/1131717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prinz, W. (2013). Self in the mirror. Conscious. Cogn. 22, 1105–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rochat, P. (2007). Intentional action arises from early reciprocal exchanges. Acta Psychol. 124, 8–25. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rochat, P. (2009). Others in Mind: Social Origins of Self-Consciousness. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Rochat, P., and Zahavi, D. (2011). The uncanny mirror: a re-framing of mirror self-experience. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 204–213. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serino, A., Sforza, A. L., Kanayama, N., van Elk, M., Kaliuzhna, M., Herbelin, B., et al. (2015). Tuning of temporo-occipital activity by frontal oscillations during virtual mirror exposure causes erroneous self-recognition. Eur. J. Neurosci. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13029. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sforza, A., Bufalari, I., Haggard, P., and Aglioti, S. M. (2010). My face in yours: visuo-tactile facial stimulation influences sense of identity. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 148–162. doi: 10.1080/17470910903205503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Grehl, S., and Tsakiris, M. (2012a). The other in me: interpersonal multisensory stimulation changes the mental representation of the self. PLoS ONE 7:e40682. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Longo, M. R., Coleman, R., and Tsakiris, M. (2012b). The person in the mirror: using the enfacement illusion to investigate the experiential structure of self-identification. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 1725–1738. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiris, M. (2008). Looking for myself: current multisensory input alters self-face recognition. PLoS ONE 3:e4040. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiris, M., and Haggard, P. (2005). The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 31:80. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.80

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: self-recognition, body perception, multisensory integration, self-updating, body awareness, face recognition

Citation: Filippetti ML (2015) What is special about our own face? Commentary: Tuning of temporo-occipital activity by frontal oscillations during virtual mirror exposure causes erroneous self-recognition. Front. Psychol. 6:1551. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01551

Received: 11 September 2015; Accepted: 24 September 2015;
Published: 13 October 2015.

Edited by:

Gesualdo M. Zucco, University of Padua, Italy

Reviewed by:

Konstantinos Priftis, University of Padua, Italy

Copyright © 2015 Filippetti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maria L. Filippetti, maria.filippetti@rhul.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.