Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 07 March 2016
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Motor skills and their Foundational Role for Perceptual, Social, and Cognitive Development View all 28 articles

The Impact of Poor Motor Skills on Perceptual, Social and Cognitive Development: The Case of Developmental Coordination Disorder

  • School of Psychology, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Surrey, Surrey, UK

Throughout development, we gain increasing control over our bodies, allowing us to move around our environment and manipulate and use objects. This developing motor control is key to our understanding of the properties of our environment (Piaget, 1953). Being able to crawl or walk affects infants' understanding of the distance between objects, as well as the feel of different surfaces and slopes (Adolph and Joh, 2007). They will also be exposed to different risks in the environment, leading to a change in the relationships with their caregivers as they learn to use social information, such as facial expressions or tone of voice, to guide their exploration (Campos et al., 2000). The development of motor skills can therefore be viewed as part of an interactive developmental process with perceptual, social, and cognitive abilities (Thelen, 1992), which is subject to the constraints of the body and the environment.

One theme of research which could help to provide more insight into these typical developmental processes is the study of neurodevelopmental disorders, in which expected interactions between different abilities may be disrupted. Motor difficulties have been highlighted in a number of neurodevelopmental disorders, including autism spectrum disorder (Provost et al., 2007), specific language impairment (Hill, 2001) and dyslexia (Nicolson et al., 2001), all of which have core deficits in other domains, such as language or social communication. The current article will focus on developmental coordination disorder (DCD), in which motor impairments are central to the diagnosis (see Table 1). DCD is relatively understudied and under-recognized in comparison to many other neurodevelopmental disorders, despite its prevalence of around 5–6% in the population (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). However, given the close connections between motor development and the other skills outlined above, this article will argue that DCD provides a model case for investigating the impact of poor motor skills on perceptual, social and cognitive development. A review of pertinent research will demonstrate the impact that poor motor skills can have on functioning in a number of different domains. Finally, the article will suggest that raising awareness of the relationships between these skills is a vital next step to aid earlier intervention for a range of perceptual, social and cognitive difficulties in individuals with motor difficulties. This will not only benefit those with DCD, but could also improve outcomes in a range of other neurodevelopmental disorders.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for developmental coordination disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Action, Perception, and Cognition

The development of motor control has long been treated as the poor relation of “higher-order” skills such as perception and cognition (Rosenbaum, 2005). More recent theories of embodiment, however, stress the ongoing interactions between the brain, the body and the environment in every situation (e.g., Iverson and Thelen, 1999; Smith, 2005), demonstrating that perception and cognition guide movement and action as much as action influences perception and cognition (Adolph and Joh, 2007). Motor skills act as constraints on our actions, which affects our interactions with the environment and the way in which we learn about the correlations between our different senses and the information they provide (e.g., Smith, 2005). When motor control is affected, as it is in DCD, we can therefore expect to see differences in the way in which the environment is perceived and processed.

Studies of visuospatial and cognitive processes in DCD have reported wide-ranging difficulties and atypicalities. For example, individuals with DCD demonstrate poorer form and motion detection, as well as difficulties in a range of tasks assessing memory and cognitive control (see Wilson et al., 2013, for a meta-analysis). If perception, cognition, and motor control are viewed as functionally separate systems, it could be a challenge to explain these difficulties in a disorder diagnosed on the basis of motor impairments. In this case, it might be tempting to regard DCD as a more general learning disability, or to describe the perceptual and cognitive difficulties as reflections of co-occurrence of DCD with other neurodevelopmental disorders, such as attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. If we focus on the integration of these symptoms, however, using an embodied approach, the relationships between motor, perceptual and cognitive impairments become clearer.

The close links between motor control and perception are demonstrated in the seemingly simple act of visually tracking a moving object. The act of smooth pursuit of an object improves over the first four to five months of life, and requires both control over posture (i.e., the ability to hold the weight of the head) and control over the eyes, as well as the coordination of eye and head movements (Von Hofsten, 2004). Smooth pursuit is intrinsically linked with object knowledge and the understanding of the physical properties of the environment. For example, when a moving object is momentarily hidden behind an occluder, infants must have a representation of the object and an understanding that it still exists in order to continue the smooth pursuit, and predictively look at the point from which the object will reappear (Adolph and Joh, 2007). The development of this ability relies on both experience with moving objects in the environment, and the rapid formation of new connections in the central nervous system, with brain areas of particular importance likely to be the parietal and prefrontal cortices, medial temporal areas of the visual cortex, and the cerebellum (Von Hofsten, 2004). Although brain imaging research is relatively limited within the DCD literature, these areas have all been implicated in the difficulties seen in DCD (Zwicker et al., 2009). Furthermore, although no research has been conducted with infants with DCD due to its identification during the school years, children of 7–12 years with a diagnosis demonstrate difficulties in smooth pursuit (Robert et al., 2014; Sumner et al., 2015). This suggests that early differences in brain development in these areas could constrain the typical improvement in smooth pursuit in infancy, which could have an impact on a range of perceptual and cognitive outcomes in DCD.

The ability to anticipate or predict the movement trajectory of an object is central not only to smooth pursuit, but also to the successful completion of actions toward objects (e.g., Wolpert, 1997). As an example, when an individual reaches for an object, it is proposed that the predictive system (or forward model) estimates the end point of the action, including the sensory information associated with that end point (such as the position of the hand in space, the feel of the object, etc.). This allows a comparison to be made during the action between the predicted and the actual sensory information being provided, and means that online, prospective changes can be made to ensure the success of the action, even when the environment is changing (Wolpert et al., 2001). If this predictive system were impaired, then individuals would have to rely on a slower feedback mechanism, which would be less effective in a dynamic, changing environment (Wilson, 2015). In DCD, it has been suggested that difficulties in motor planning (van Swieten et al., 2010; Pratt et al., 2014) and the online adaptation of movements (Hyde and Wilson, 2011; Ruddock et al., 2015) could reflect these underlying impairments in predictive control. Other issues with this system may also be evident in the problems with voluntary, effortful control over cognition (known as “executive functions”: Diamond, 2013), which are prevalent amongst individuals with DCD (see Leonard and Hill, 2015, for a review). As in smooth pursuit, both motor actions and executive functioning are subserved by underlying structural and functional connections between parietal and prefrontal cortices and the cerebellum (e.g., Diamond, 2000; Koziol et al., 2012; Ramnani, 2012). Thus, in DCD, atypical structure or functioning of these areas (e.g., Kashiwagi et al., 2009; Debrabant et al., 2013), reduced functional connectivity within this network (e.g., Zwicker et al., 2011), or an immature coupling between frontal and posterior control areas (Ruddock et al., 2015) could explain the difficulties seen in the disorder across a range of predictive control tasks. Further research into DCD provides an opportunity for investigating potential shared mechanisms underlying these forms of predictive control, as well as their neural bases, in both typical and atypical development.

Motor Skills, Executive Control, and Social Functioning

As outlined in the introduction to this article, developing motor control provides infants with opportunities for interacting with the social environment as well as their physical surroundings. As children grow older, motor skills are central to the types of play in which they engage, from the manual dexterity required for dressing up or making models out of toy bricks, to the physical play and team games seen in the playground. If the skills required for these activities are impaired, as in DCD, it is likely to have implications for social functioning and peer relations. Indeed, research into DCD investigating social activities, peer relations and social problems has reported significant correlations between motor abilities and parent-reported peer difficulties (Cummins et al., 2005; Green et al., 2006; Wagner et al., 2012). In playground observations, children with DCD were more likely to be alone and to be onlookers, rather than the center of a social group (Smyth and Anderson, 2000), and the relationship between motor coordination and self-reported loneliness by children with DCD was mediated by their participation in team sports (Poulsen et al., 2007). Poor motor development, and consequent low self-concept, could explain this reduced participation in motor activities and physical play in those with DCD (e.g., Wrotniak et al., 2006), which may result in social exclusion.

It is also possible that the close connections between brain areas related to motor and executive control could affect social functioning. Research in typically-developing children has reported that executive functions, such as cognitive flexibility, are highly correlated with social understanding and performance on theory of mind tasks in early and middle childhood (e.g., Hughes and Ensor, 2007; Bock et al., 2015). Further support for this view arises from research with children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which is characterized by both executive functioning and social difficulties (Wehmeier et al., 2010). It is suggested that problems with flexibility could interfere with typical social interaction in those with ADHD, while difficulties inhibiting unwanted responses and regulating behavior could lead to peer rejection (e.g., Wheeler Maedgen and Carlson, 2000; Marton et al., 2009). As ADHD often co-occurs with DCD (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), and given that children with DCD often present problems in inhibition and self-regulation (e.g., Rahimi-Golkhandan et al., 2014; Leonard et al., 2015), it could be that social difficulties in DCD are related to these issues in executive control. Poor motor development could thus affect social functioning through at least two routes: first by limiting the potential to participate in physical activities and play, and second by affecting executive control and social understanding. As low motor competence is not only associated with DCD, but to a range of other neurodevelopmental disorders and environmental factors, it is of great importance that the identification of motor difficulties is improved, allowing appropriate early intervention in order to reduce the potential adverse effects on perceptual, cognitive and social development.

Moving Forward: An Embodied Approach

From the research outlined throughout this article, it is clear that motor development not only provides opportunities for the development of a range of perceptual, social, and cognitive skills, but is influenced in turn by these abilities in an interactive process. Using an embodied approach, it is possible to consider the relationships and constant interactions between domains as part of a dynamic system with shared underlying mechanisms, such as predictive control. Improving our understanding of these shared mechanisms will be important for developing more integrated interventions for those with a range of difficulties. Furthermore, raising awareness of the impact of poor motor skills on perception, cognition and social development amongst psychologists, teachers, parents, and practitioners will be vital in improving outcomes for those with motor difficulties. DCD provides a model case for further investigation, due to the known motor impairments in the disorder. However, research into this area will have implications for a wide range of individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders, as well as for those with low motor competence associated with a number of different factors.

Author Contributions

The paper was conceived, drafted, and revised by HL as the sole author.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adolph, K. E., and Joh, A. S. (2007). “Motor development: how infants get into the act,” in Introduction to Infant Development, 2nd Edn., eds A. Slater and M. Lewis (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 63–80.

American Psychiatric Association (2013). DSM-5: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Bock, A. M., Gallaway, K. C., and Hynd, A. M. (2015). Specifying links between executive functioning and theory of mind during middle childhood: cognitive flexibility predicts social understanding. J. Cogn. Dev. 16, 509–521. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2014.888350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., and Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy 1, 149–219. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0102_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummins, A., Piek, J. P., and Dyck, M. J. (2005). Motor coordination, empathy, and social behaviour in school-aged children. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 47, 437–442. doi: 10.1017/S001216220500085X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Debrabant, J., Gheysen, F., Caeyenberghs, K., Van Waelvelde, H., and Vingerhoets, G. (2013). Neural underpinnings of impaired predictive motor timing in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 1478–1487. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 71, 44–56. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, D., Baird, G., and Sugden, D. (2006). A pilot study of psychopathology in developmental coordination disorder. Child Care Health Dev. 32, 741–750. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2006.00684.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, E. L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: a review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 36, 149–171. doi: 10.1080/13682820010019874

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, C., and Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Dev. Psychol. 43, 1447–1459. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyde, C., and Wilson, P. H. (2011). Dissecting online control in developmental coordination disorder: a kinematic analysis of double-step reaching. Brain Cogn. 75, 232–241. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson, J. M., and Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth and brain. The dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. J. Conscious. Stud. 6, 19–40.

Google Scholar

Kashiwagi, M., Iwaki, S., Narumi, Y., Tamai, H., and Suzuki, S. (2009). Parietal dysfunction in developmental coordination disorder: a functional MRI study. Neuroreport 20, 1319–1324. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832f4d87

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koziol, L. F., Budding, D. E., and Chidekel, D. (2012). From movement to thought: executive function, embodied cognition, and the cerebellum. Cerebellum 11, 505–525. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0321-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, H. C., Bernardi, M., Hill, E. L., and Henry, L. A. (2015). Executive functioning, motor difficulties, and developmental coordination disorder. Dev. Neuropsychol. 40, 201–215. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2014.997933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, H. C., and Hill, E. L. (2015). Executive difficulties in developmental coordination disorder: methodological issues and future directions. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2, 141–149. doi: 10.1007/s40474-015-0044-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marton, I., Wiener, J., Rogers, M., Moore, C., and Tannock, R. (2009). Empathy and social perspective taking in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 107–118. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9262-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., and Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends Neurosci. 24, 508–511. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01896-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piaget, J. (1953). The Origin of Intelligence in the Child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Google Scholar

Poulsen, A. A., Ziviani, J. M., Cuskelly, M., and Smith, R. (2007). Boys with developmental coordination disorder: Loneliness and team sports participation. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 61, 451–462. doi: 10.5014/ajot.61.4.451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratt, M. L., Leonard, H. C., Adeyinka, H., and Hill, E. L. (2014). The effect of motor load on planning and inhibition in developmental coordination disorder. Res. Dev. Disabil. 35, 1579–1587. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Provost, B., Lopez, B. R., and Heimerl, S. (2007). A comparison of motor delays in young children: autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and developmental concerns. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 321–328. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0170-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahimi-Golkhandan, S., Steenbergen, B., Piek, J. P., and Wilson, P. H. (2014). Deficits of hot executive function in developmental coordination disorder: sensitivity to positive social cues. Hum. Mov. Sci. 38, 209–224. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2014.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramnani, N. (2012). Frontal lobe and posterior parietal contributions to the cortico-cerebellar system. Cerebellum 11, 366–383. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0272-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robert, M. P., Ingster-Moati, I., Albuisson, E., Cabrol, D., Golse, B., and Vaivre-Douret, L. (2014). Vertical and horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in children with developmental coordination disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 56, 595–600. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenbaum, D. A. (2005). The Cinderella of psychology: the neglect of motor control in the science of mental life and behavior. Am. Psychol. 60:308. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.4.308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruddock, S., Piek, J., Sugden, D., Morris, S., Hyde, C., Caeyenberghs, K., et al. (2015). Coupling online control and inhibitory systems in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: goal-directed reaching. Res. Dev. Disabil. 36, 244–255. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.10.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, L. B. (2005). Cognition as a dynamic system: Principles from embodiment. Dev. Rev. 25, 278–298. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smyth, M. M., and Anderson, H. I. (2000). Coping with clumsiness in the school playground: social and physical play in children with coordination impairments. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 18, 389–413. doi: 10.1348/026151000165760

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sumner, E., Leonard, H. C., and Hill, E. L. (2015). Oculomotor function in children with and without developmental coordination disorder [Abstract]. J. Comorb. 5(Supp.), 101. doi: 10.15256/joc.2015.5.52

CrossRef Full Text

Thelen, E. (1992). Development as a dynamic system. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 1, 189–193. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Swieten, L. M., van Bergen, E., Williams, J. H., Wilson, A. D., Plumb, M. S., Kent, S. W., et al. (2010). A test of motor (not executive) planning in developmental coordination disorder and autism. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 36, 493–499. doi: 10.1037/a0017177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Hofsten, C. (2004). An action perspective on motor development. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 266–272. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, M. O., Bös, K., Jascenoka, J., Jekauc, D., and Petermann, F. (2012). Peer problems mediate the relationship between developmental coordination disorder and behavioral problems in school-aged children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 33, 2072–2079. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wehmeier, P. M., Schacht, A., and Barkley, R. A. (2010). Social and emotional impairment in children and adolescents with ADHD and the impact on quality of life. J. Adolesc. Health 46, 209–217. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheeler Maedgen, J., and Carlson, C. L. (2000). Social functioning and emotional regulation in the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder subtypes. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 29, 30–42. doi: 10.1207/S15374424jccp2901_4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, P. H. (2015). “Neurocognitive processing deficits in children with developmental coordination disorder,” in Developmental Coordination Disorder and its Consequences, ed J. Cairney (Toronto, ON: University of Toronto Press), 138–168.

Wilson, P. H., Ruddock, S., Smits-Engelsman, B., Polatajko, H., and Blank, R. (2013). Understanding performance deficits in developmental coordination disorder: a meta-analysis of recent research. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 55, 217–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04436.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M. (1997). Computational approaches to motor control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1, 209–216. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01070-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., and Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Perspectives and problems in motor learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 487–494. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01773-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wrotniak, B. H., Epstein, L. H., Dorn, J. M., Jones, K. E., and Kondilis, V. A. (2006). The relationship between motor proficiency and physical activity in children. Pediatrics 118, e1758–e1765. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwicker, J. G., Missiuna, C., and Boyd, L. A. (2009). Neural correlates of developmental coordination disorder: a review of hypotheses. J. Child Neurol. 24, 1273–1281. doi: 10.1177/0883073809333537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwicker, J. G., Missiuna, C., Harris, S. R., and Boyd, L. A. (2011). Brain activation associated with motor skill practice in children with developmental coordination disorder: an fMRI study. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 29, 145–152. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2010.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: developmental coordination disorder, motor skill, embodiment, dynamic systems, physical activity, predictive control

Citation: Leonard HC (2016) The Impact of Poor Motor Skills on Perceptual, Social and Cognitive Development: The Case of Developmental Coordination Disorder. Front. Psychol. 7:311. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00311

Received: 02 December 2015; Accepted: 18 February 2016;
Published: 07 March 2016.

Edited by:

Klaus Libertus, University of Pittsburgh, USA

Reviewed by:

Peter Henry Wilson, Australian Catholic University, Australia

Copyright © 2016 Leonard. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hayley C. Leonard, h.leonard@surrey.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.