Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 25 April 2016
Sec. Developmental Psychology

The Complex Nature of Bilinguals' Language Usage Modulates Task-Switching Outcomes

  • 1School of Social Sciences, Singapore Management University, Singapore, Singapore
  • 2Department of Psychology, Ewha Womans University, Seoul, South Korea

In view of inconsistent findings regarding bilingual advantages in executive functions (EF), we reviewed the literature to determine whether bilinguals' different language usage causes measureable changes in the shifting aspects of EF. By drawing on the theoretical framework of the adaptive control hypothesis—which postulates a critical link between bilinguals' varying demands on language control and adaptive cognitive control (Green and Abutalebi, 2013), we examined three factors that characterize bilinguals' language-switching experience: (a) the interactional context of conversational exchanges, (b) frequency of language switching, and (c) typology of code-switching. We also examined whether methodological variations in previous task-switching studies modulate task-specific demands on control processing and lead to inconsistencies in the literature. Our review demonstrates that not only methodological rigor but also a more finely grained, theory-based approach will be required to understand the cognitive consequences of bilinguals' varied linguistic practices in shifting EF.

Introduction

Executive functions (EF) refer to a multifaceted construct of a general control process that consists of three postulated functions: inhibition, updating, and shifting (Miyake et al., 2000). Recent studies of the cognitive advantages of bilingualism in EF report inconsistent findings, and contend that bilingual advantages in EF do not exist—and, further, that there is no compelling evidence that any specific bilingual experience stimulates a certain aspect of EF (e.g., Paap et al., 2015, p. 272). This claim, however, requires a more rigorous, theory-driven approach to identify potential moderators or confounds that contribute to the presence (or absence) of bilingual advantages in EF (Yang et al., 2016).

In this review, we focus on the importance of bilinguals' varied language-switching experiences in modulating shifting aspects of EF, and suggest that specific bilingual experiences can potentially moderate bilingual advantages in EF. We examined three aspects of bilinguals' language-switching experiences: (a) the interactional context of conversational exchanges, (b) language-switching frequency, and (c) typology of code-switching, which refers to the alternation between two or more languages. Moreover, given that varying task demands modulate bilingual advantages in EF (Costa et al., 2009; Yang and Yang, 2016), we also examined whether divergent findings in the literature can be attributed to task-specific demands driven by methodological differences or other factors, such as age.

Inconsistencies in the Literature

Shifting EF has typically been assessed by the task-switching paradigm (e.g., color-shape and number-letter tasks), in which participants are prompted to alternate between two different tasks. The paradigm typically derives two task-switching costs that have been found to implicate different control mechanisms (Braver et al., 2003; Rubin and Meiran, 2005). Switch costs refer to slower responses on task-switch trials than on non-switch trials, and thereby reflect the actual cost of switching between different task sets. Switch costs are driven by local control mechanisms that involve (a) task-set reconfiguration, which is the ability to change from one task set to another (Rogers and Monsell, 1995; Meiran, 1996), and (b) inhibition of proactive interference, which is the ability to regulate interference from the previously formed task set (Wylie and Allport, 2000; Philipp et al., 2008). Mixing costs, on the other hand, refer to slower responses on task-repeat trials in mixed-task blocks than in pure blocks—which consist of a single task—and thereby reflect the cost of monitoring and coordinating multiple streams of incoming information. Mixing costs entail the activation of global control mechanisms that are necessary to monitor task cues, maintain two competing tasks/response sets, and make task-appropriate decisions (Braver et al., 2003; Rubin and Meiran, 2005).

Bilinguals' language switching requires the ability to reconfigure language sets while simultaneously inhibiting potential interference from a previously relevant, but now irrelevant, language. This has led some to argue that bilingual advantages likely reside in switch costs (Garbin et al., 2010; Prior and MacWhinney, 2010; Prior and Gollan, 2011; Houtzager et al., 2015). On the other hand, since bilinguals constantly monitor two languages that are activated simultaneously, others have argued that bilingual advantages should be evident in mixing costs (e.g., Barac and Bialystok, 2012; Gold et al., 2013; Wiseheart et al., 2016). While the two camps debate whether bilingualism benefits either switch costs or mixing costs, still other researchers have failed to replicate bilingual advantages in either context, and therefore argue that bilingualism does not benefit shifting-specific EF (Hernández et al., 2013; Paap and Greenberg, 2013; Paap and Sawi, 2014; Mor et al., 2015).

Interactional Context of Conversational Exchanges

Recently, Green and Abutalebi (2013) proposed the adaptive control hypothesis, which holds that the different interactional contexts of bilinguals' conversational exchanges place varying demands on language control, which in turn adaptively alter their cognitive-control capacities. Specifically, Green and Abutalebi distinguish three different interactional contexts: (a) the dual-language context, in which bilinguals use two languages (L1 and L2) within the same context (e.g., at home and work); (b) the single-language context, in which bilinguals speak only one language in one environment, and therefore rarely switch languages (e.g., L1 at home and L2 at work); and (c) the dense code-switching context, in which bilinguals routinely mix the linguistic elements (e.g., words) of two languages within a single utterance (i.e., intrasentential code-switching). For instance, an English-Tagalog bilingual might say, “Wala akong cash pang grocery ngayon”; I do not have cash for grocery today (Green and Abutalebi, 2013, p. 518).

The adaptive control hypothesis suggests that bilinguals' interactional context is a key factor that modulates cognitive advantages in EF. Specifically, bilinguals' dual-language context engages a more complex and taxing level of control processes of goal maintenance, conflict monitoring, and interference suppression, and therefore facilitates more adaptive cognitive control than either the single-language or dense code-switching context. Given that bilinguals in a dual-language context should experience not only more frequent but also more qualitatively challenging language switching, it is conceivable that the dual-language context will promote shifting EF—and therefore enhance task-switching performance—while the single-language and dense code-switching contexts will not. Consistently, Hartanto and Yang (2016) found that bilinguals who mainly engage in a dual-language context showed smaller switch costs than those who mainly engage in a single-language context. Moreover, Guerrero et al. (2015) found that bilingual children who spoke a more balanced mixture of languages at home (e.g., 50% English and 50% another language) displayed lower switching costs in a trail-making task than those who spoke only one language at home. Verhagen et al. (2015) also reported that bilingual children whose parents spoke different languages performed significantly better on the Stroop and delay-of-gratification tasks than bilinguals whose parents spoke the same language as the child. It is clear, therefore, that the interactional context of bilinguals' conversational exchanges is a critical factor that renders bilingual experience more advantageous in shifting EF and, in turn, enhances task-switching performance.

Frequency of Language Switching

Because bilinguals' frequent language switching likely requires greater control of each language, it is reasonable to expect that bilinguals who frequently switch languages will have enhanced task-switching skills. Prior and Gollan (2011) first examined this issue by comparing monolinguals to Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals who differed in their self-reported frequency of language switching. Not surprisingly, they found that only Spanish-English bilinguals—who were regarded as frequent language switchers—had smaller switch costs than monolinguals, suggesting that there may be a minimum threshold for language-switching frequency that confers advantages on task switching.

Language-switching frequency has also been found to modulate inhibitory control, which is closely related to task switching (Friedman et al., 2006; Miyake and Friedman, 2012). Specifically, Verreyt et al. (2016) compared three groups of bilinguals—unbalanced bilinguals, balanced non-switching bilinguals, and balanced switching bilinguals—on flanker and Simon tasks. They found that of the three groups, balanced switching bilinguals had the significantly lowest flanker and Simon effects. More importantly, frequency of switching was negatively correlated with both flanker and Simon effects, which suggests that language-switching frequency is important for efficient inhibitory control. In the same vein, Woumans et al. (2015) found that higher language-switching proficiency is associated with lower congruency effects in Simon and attention-network tasks.

In contrast, Paap and Greenberg (2013) did not find that frequent language switching confers benefits on either task-switching costs or inhibitory control. Given this discrepancy, it is notable that they employed the percentage of daily English usage (L1) as a proxy measure of language-switching frequency; they reasoned that the smaller percentage of L1 usage reflects considerable usage of the other language (L2), and therefore can approximate the frequency of language switching between L1 and L2. Albeit plausible, the validity of this approximation may be questionable, since the percent usage of L1 does not always indicate the frequency of language switching. That is, 50% of L1 use can be achieved without language switching by speaking L1 exclusively at home and L2 at work (i.e., a single-language context).

Not surprisingly, other studies have found no correspondence between the percent usage of L1 relative to L2 and self-reported language-switching frequency. For example, Prior and Gollan's (2011) Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals used English at comparable percentages (i.e., MSE = 84.6%, MME = 86.6%), but their language-switching frequency differed significantly (MSE = 3.2, MME = 2.4 on a 5-point Likert scale, with 1 = almost never and 5 = constantly). Likewise, Paap and Sawi (2014) tested bilinguals who reported speaking L1 about 70% of the time, but their report of actual switching frequency was limited to only a couple of times a day. Given this, findings based on the percent usage of L1 (or L2) as a proxy measure of language-switching frequency should be interpreted with caution.

The Typology of Code-Switching

The adaptive control hypothesis proposes that the challenging quality of bilinguals' linguistic practices plays a key role in triggering more adaptive cognitive control (Green and Abutalebi, 2013). In support of this hypothesis, Macnamara and Conway (2013) found that bilinguals' cognitive control and working memory were positively influenced by the degree of bilingual management demands—the extent to which bilinguals engage cognitive control to appropriately use the two languages—and the amount of experience managing the bilingual demands. Therefore, we examined the influence of different types of code-switching on shifting EF.

Broadly, bilinguals' language switching is classified as either intrasentential code-switching (i.e., mixing linguistic units from two languages within a sentence) or intersentential code-switching (i.e., interchanging two or more languages between sentences; e.g., Brice, 2000). Intrasentential code-switching is characterized by loan words that are integrated into the other language's syntactic context (i.e., grammar). For instance, a Singapore bilingual might say, “I like to makan some good seafood”; makan means “eat” in Malay. Even when the bilingual does not understand Malay grammar, the speaker is still able to switch languages easily, because intrasentential code-switching does not necessarily entail a substantial shift from one linguistic set to another. Intrasentential code-switching, therefore, facilitates language production by permitting the speaker to use whatever comes most readily (e.g., Gollan and Ferreira, 2009; Green and Abutalebi, 2013). In line with this, Soveri et al. (2011) found that frequency of intrasentential code-switching showed a negative correlation with (i.e., attenuated) mixing costs, but a trend of positive correlation with (i.e., increased) switch costs, suggesting that intrasentential code switching may not benefit switch costs.

On the other hand, intersentential code-switching is believed to be more demanding than intrasentential code-switching, as the former requires language-set reconfiguration and proactive inhibition. Therefore, more frequent intersentential code-switching likely facilitates bilingual advantages in task switching. In support of this view, recent studies suggest that the demands of language control modulate task-switching performance. For instance, Hartanto and Yang (2016) identified a trend in which, when bilinguals' SES and intelligence were controlled for, intersentential code-switching negatively predicted switch costs (i.e., reduced switch costs), while intrasentential code-switching positively predicted switch costs (e.g., increased switch costs) in the same regression model.

Relatedly, it seems that cognitive demand for intersentential code-switching is further influenced by who initiates the discourse—i.e., self vs. other, which is sometimes referred to as voluntary vs. involuntary (e.g., Gollan et al., 2014). Self-initiated intersentential code-switching seems to be less costly and relatively less taxing than other-initiated intersentential code-switching, because the former permits sufficient preparation time to reconfigure language sets, while other-initiated intersentential code-switching is triggered unexpectedly and therefore imposes greater demands on language-set reconfiguration and proactive inhibition. As demonstrated by Gollan et al. (2014), self-initiated voluntary switching is associated with significantly lower language-switching costs than is the case with other-initiated switching. This notion is consistent with the adaptive control hypothesis, which proposes that language switching in response to an interchange with another speaker incurs greater costs than voluntary language switching (Green and Abutalebi, 2013). Accordingly, more frequent exercise of other-initiated intersentential code-switching is likely to be more beneficial for switch costs than self-initiated intersentential code-switching. We suggest, therefore, that various factors may modulate the impact of complex intersentential code-switching on task-switching performance. To date, however, this issue has received little attention, and warrants more in-depth research. In sum, the typology of code-switching is an important factor that sheds light on the demands of various bilingual experiences and their impact on task-switching performance.

Variations in the Task-Switching Paradigm

In view of the variety of EF tasks being used to test bilingual effects, inconsistent conclusions regarding bilingual effects on EF can be attributed to task-specific demands driven by methodological differences. Not surprisingly, findings in the literature demonstrate that even minor parametric variations can modulate bilingual advantages in EF through diverse levels of task-specific demands (Costa et al., 2009; Yang and Yang, 2016). Little is known, however, about whether this is the case in relation to shifting EF, which is assessed by a task-switching paradigm. We reviewed the literature, therefore, to examine whether the discrepant findings of bilingual effects on shifting EF can be attributed to varying task demands in task switching. To maintain a reasonable scope, we narrowed our focus within the task-switching paradigm (see Table 1; Monsell, 2003) and excluded variants that have been known to implicate other aspects of EF, despite their ability to derive task-switching costs (e.g., flanker and Simon tasks; e.g., Paap and Sawi, 2014).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of studies examining bilingual benefit using a task-switching paradigm (task-cuing paradigm) in adults.

Varying Task Demands

It has been documented that procedural changes in color-shape switching tasks substantially affect task-switching performance in bilinguals due to varying task demand (i.e., difficulty; Kiesel et al., 2010; Vandierendonck et al., 2010). Notably, the type of response mapping—i.e., the arrangement of response keys for two different (color and shape) tasks—has yielded mixed results. Studies with non-overlapping response-stimulus sets—i.e., separate response keys are mapped to different answers for each of the two task sets—failed to find bilingual advantages in mixing costs (e.g., Prior and Gollan, 2011; Hernández et al., 2013; Paap and Greenberg, 2013), while those with overlapping response-stimulus sets—i.e., the same response keys are (re)mapped to each of the two tasks—reported bilingual advantages in mixing costs (e.g., Soveri et al., 2011; Gold et al., 2013; Qu et al., 2015; Wiseheart et al., 2016). One possible reason for this discrepancy is that tasks with overlapping response mapping are more demanding, and therefore more sensitive to detect bilingual advantages in mixing costs (Mayr, 2001). According to the task-switching literature, overlapping response mapping increases task interference by triggering greater response competition associated with tasks (Gade and Koch, 2007). Therefore, heightened demand for task competition—which is apparent in overlapping response mapping—likely enhances task sensitivity to capture participants' variations in mixing costs (Rubin and Meiran, 2005). Hence, the overlapping response-stimulus set seems to be a critical boundary condition that leads to observable differences in mixing costs between bilinguals and monolinguals (e.g., Mayr, 2001).

Importantly, the type of response mapping may still affect switch costs via changes in the demands of task-set reconfiguration (Meiran, 2000; Kiesel et al., 2010). For instance, Meiran (2008) demonstrates that the adverse effect of overlapping response mapping on switch costs is sensitive to the manipulation of time variables (e.g., the cue-to-stimulus interval), which directly influences task-set reconfiguration. Meiran argues that when the task cue switches from color to shape (or vice versa), the same response key in tasks with response overlapping should be recoded to match different aspects of the stimulus in a fraction of a second (i.e., response recoding), and this demands greater task-set reconfiguration. In contrast, response recoding is unnecessary in tasks with non-overlapping response mapping, in which each response is assigned to a separate key and therefore imposes smaller demands on task-set reconfiguration. Given that different types of response mapping may trigger divergent outcomes, future studies should closely examine the potential impact of task demands, which may modulate bilingual effects on task switching (e.g., Qu et al., 2015).

Participant Age

In view of the importance of varying task demand, it is plausible that an interplay between the participant's age and task demand moderates bilingual advantages in task switching. Since behavioral variability typically has a U-shaped function over the lifespan, with larger variability in children and older adults (Grady, 2012), task demand should be sensitive to capture performance differences in children and older adults, who are markedly different from young adults in terms of (a) cognitive maturation and deterioration and (b) extent of involvement in challenging cognitive activities other than language switching (Valian, 2015). Therefore, it is plausible that even a mildly difficult task should be sensitive to capture larger variability associated with bilingualism in the child or aging population. Given this, it is not surprising that the majority of studies that tested young adults—who have likely reached the peak of their cognitive functioning—have reported either null or inconsistent findings, primarily because testing young adults on tasks with mild or moderate difficulty appears to constrain potential variability between monolinguals and bilinguals (Bialystok, 2006; Costa et al., 2008).

In contrast, studies that tested either young children or older adults have reported relatively consistent bilingual advantages (Soveri et al., 2011; Gold et al., 2013; Houtzager et al., 2015). For instance, older bilinguals have shown smaller mixing costs than their monolingual counterparts (e.g., Gold et al., 2013). Likewise, bilingual children produced significantly lower post-switch errors–which are an equivalent index of task-switching performance–on a simple DCCS task (see Table 2; Bialystok, 1999; Bialystok and Martin, 2004; Carlson and Meltzoff, 2008). Taken together, these studies demonstrate that the bilingual's age warrants further consideration as a key factor in the developmental influence of bilingualism on task switching (i.e., shifting function).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of studies examining bilingual advantages in task-switching among young children.

Conclusions

Given discrepancies in the literature, we propose that bilinguals' language-switching practices should be examined in ways that reflect the challenging quality of language control. Our review suggests that bilinguals' complex language-switching experiences (e.g., a dual-language context or frequent use of more demanding code-switching), task-specific demands on control processing, and potential moderators (e.g., age) will yield valuable insights on the link between bilingual experience and specific aspects of EF.

Author Contributions

HY, AH conceptualized the paper and HY, AH, and SY wrote the main paper and equally contributed to critical revision of this paper.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a research grant from Singapore Management University through a research grant (C242/MSS13S011) from the Ministry of Education Academic Research Fund Tier 1.

References

Barac, R., and Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Dev. 83, 413–422. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01707.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Dev. 70, 636–644. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E. (2006). Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 60, 68–79. doi: 10.1037/cjep2006008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E., and Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Dev. Sci. 7, 325–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00351.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braver, T. S., Reynolds, J. R., and Donaldson, D. I. (2003). Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching. Neuron 39, 713–726. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00466-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brice, A. (2000). Code switching and code mixing in the ESL classroom: a study of pragmatic and syntactic features. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2, 19–28. doi: 10.3109/14417040008996783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, S. M., and Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Dev. Sci. 11, 282–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., and Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: now you see it, now you don't. Cognition 113, 135–149. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, A., Hernández, M., and Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: evidence from the ANT task. Cognition 106, 59–86. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., and Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychol. Sci. 17, 172–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gade, M., and Koch, I. (2007). The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition. Mem. Cognit. 35, 603–609. doi: 10.3758/bf03193298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J. C., Rodriguez-Pujadas, A., and Belloch, V. E. (2010). Bridging language and attention: brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control. Neuroimage 53, 1272–1278. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kryscio, R. J., and Smith, C. D. (2013). Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in Aging. J. Neurosci. 33, 387–396. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3837-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollan, T. H., and Ferreira, V. S. (2009). Should I stay or should I switch? A cost–benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 35, 640–665. doi: 10.1037/a0014981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollan, T. H., Kleinman, D., and Wierenga, C. E. (2014). What's easier: doing what you want, or being told what to do? Cued versus voluntary language and task switching. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 2167–2195. doi: 10.1037/a0038006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 491–505. doi: 10.1038/nrn3256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, D. W., and Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: the adaptive control hypothesis. J. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 515–530. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.796377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerrero, S. L., Mesite, L., Surrain, S., and Luk, G. (2015). “Mixed language use and cognitive flexibility in young bilinguals,” in Poster presented at the CUNY workshop on Bilingualism and Executive Function: An Interdisciplinary Approach (New York, NY).

Hartanto, A., and Yang, H. (2016). Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition 150, 10–19. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández, M., Martin, C. D., Barceló, F., and Costa, A. (2013). Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching? J. Mem. Lang. 69, 257–276. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houtzager, N., Lowie, W., Sprenger, S., and De Bot, K. (2015). A bilingual advantage in task switching? Age-related differences between German monolinguals and Dutch-Frisian bilinguals. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 1–11. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., et al. (2010). Control and interference in task switching–A review. Psychol. Bull. 136, 849–874. doi: 10.1037/a0019842

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macnamara, B. N., and Conway, A. R. A. (2013). Novel evidence in support of the bilingual advantage: influences of task demands and experience on cognitive control and working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21, 520–525. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0524-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayr, U. (2001). Age differences in the selection of mental sets: the role of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap. Psychol. Aging 16, 96–109. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.1.96

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 22, 1423–1442. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1423

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meiran, N. (2000). “Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching,” in Attention and Performance XVIII: Control of Cognitive Processes, eds S. Monsell and J. Driver (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 377–400.

Meiran, N. (2008). The dual implication of dual affordance. Exp. Psychol. 55, 251–259. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.55.4.251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., and Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 134–140. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00028-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mor, B., Yitzhaki-Amsalem, S., and Prior, A. (2015). The joint effect of bilingualism and ADHD on executive functions. J. Atten. Disord. 19, 527–541. doi: 10.1177/1087054714527790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paap, K. R., and Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cogn. Psychol. 66, 232–258. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., and Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex 69, 265–278. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paap, K. R., and Sawi, O. (2014). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Front. Psychol. 5:962. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Philipp, A. M., Kalinich, C., Koch, I., and Schubotz, R. I. (2008). Mixing costs and switch costs when switching stimulus dimensions in serial predictions. Psychol. Res. 72, 405–414. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0150-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prior, A., and Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 682–691. doi: 10.1017/s1355617711000580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prior, A., and MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 13, 253–262. doi: 10.1017/s1366728909990526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qu, L., Low, J. J. W., Zhang, T., Li, H., and Zelazo, P. D. (2015). Bilingual advantage in executive control when task demands are considered. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 19, 277–293. doi: 10.1017/S1366728914000376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, R. D., and Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 124, 207–231. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, O., and Meiran, N. (2005). On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 31, 1477–1491. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., and Laine, M. (2011). Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within group analysis approach. Front. Psychol. 2:183. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 18, 3–24. doi: 10.1017/s1366728914000522

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandierendonck, A., Liefooghe, B., and Verbruggen, F. (2010). Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychol. Bull. 136, 601–626. doi: 10.1037/a0019791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhagen, J., Mulder, H., and Leseman, P. P. M. (2015). Effects of home language environment on inhibitory control in bilingual three-year-old children. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 1–14. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000590

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verreyt, N., Woumans, E., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A., and Duyck, W. (2016). The influence of language-switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 19, 181–190. doi: 10.1017/s1366728914000352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiseheart, M., Viswanathan, M., and Bialystok, E. (2016). Flexibility in task switching by monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 19, 141–146. doi: 10.1017/s1366728914000273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woumans, E., Ceuleers, E., Van der Linden, L., Szmalec, A., and Duyck, W. (2015). Verbal and nonverbal cognitive control in bilinguals and interpreters. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 41, 1579–1586. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wylie, G., and Allport, A. (2000). Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs.” Psychol. Res. 63, 212–233. doi: 10.1007/s004269900003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, H., Hartanto, A., and Yang, S. (2016). The importance of bilingual experience in assessing bilingual advantages in executive functions. Cortex 75, 237–240. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, H., and Yang, S. (2016). Are all interferences bad? Bilingual advantages in working memory are modulated by varying demands for controlled processing. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 1–13. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000632

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: bilingualism, task switching, the adaptive control hypothesis, shifting EF, switch costs, mixing costs

Citation: Yang H, Hartanto A and Yang S (2016) The Complex Nature of Bilinguals' Language Usage Modulates Task-Switching Outcomes. Front. Psychol. 7:560. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00560

Received: 31 October 2015; Accepted: 05 April 2016;
Published: 25 April 2016.

Edited by:

Yusuke Moriguchi, Joetsu University of Education, Japan

Reviewed by:

Roberto Filippi, Anglia Ruskin University, UK
Evy Adèle Woumans, Ghent University, Belgium

Copyright © 2016 Yang, Hartanto and Yang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hwajin Yang, hjyang@smu.edu.sg

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.