Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 31 May 2016
Sec. Cultural Psychology

What's in the Chinese Babyface? Cultural Differences in Understanding the Babyface

\r\nWenwen ZhengWenwen Zheng1Qian YangQian Yang2Kaiping Peng*Kaiping Peng1*Feng YuFeng Yu3
  • 1Department of Psychology, Tsinghua University, Beijing, China
  • 2Department of Public Health, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
  • 3Department of Leadership and Organization Management, School of Economics and Management, Tsinghua University, Beijing, China

We investigated the cultural differences in understanding and reacting to the babyface in an effort to identify both cultural and gender biases in the universal hypothesis that the babyfaced individuals are perceived as naïve, cute, innocent, and more trustworthy. Sixty-six Chinese and Sixty-six American participants were required to evaluate Chinese faces selected from the Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS)—Pose, Expression, Accessories, and Lighting (PEAL) Large-Scale Chinese Face Database. In our study, we applied Active Shape Models, a modern technique of machine learning to measure facial features. We found some cultural similarities and also found that a Chinese babyface has bigger eyes, higher eyebrows, a smaller chin, and greater WHR (Facial width-to-height ratio), and looks more attractive and warmer. New findings demonstrate that Chinese babyfaces have a lower forehead and closer pupil distance (PD). We found that when evaluating the babyfacedness of a face, Chinese are more concerned with the combination of all facial features and American are more sensitive to specific highlighted babyfaced features. The Chinese babyface tended to be perceived as more babyfaced for American participants, but not less competent for Chinese participants.

Introduction

You can never tell a book by its cover, but we always automatically and unconsciously judge people by their faces. Indeed, in fewer than 50 ms, first impressions about a face can be generated (Todorov et al., 2009). The babyface, with its unique facial structures which differ from mature adult faces, evokes a series of stereotypes. Whether in humans or animals, the babyface is usually defined as a round face with big eyes, high raised eyebrows, a narrow chin, and a small nose. All these features give us the impression of child-like traits, such as being naïve, cute, innocent, etc. (Zebrowitz and Montepare, 1992, 2005; Zebrowitz et al., 1993, 2012). The babyface overgeneralization effect applies to both infants and adults, including youth and seniors (Zebrowitz and Franklin, 2014; Zebrowitz et al., 2015). Babyface stereotypes can bias social life outcomes, including elections, financial rewards, job applications, academic performances, prison sentences, altruism, and communication environments (Zebrowitz and McDonald, 1991; Zebrowitz et al., 1992, 1998; Collins and Zebrowitz, 1995; Zebrowitz and Montepare, 2008; Livingston and Pearce, 2009; Poutvaara et al., 2009).

There are a few cross-cultural investigations of babyface phenomena in different cultural contexts, but they are focused more on identifying similarities than differences (Zebrowitz et al., 1992, 1993). Zebrowitz et al. (2012) proposed a common mechanism among people's social perception of faces. Even individuals who are isolated from the industrial revolution and modernization can still develop the ability to perceive attractive faces and babyfaces.

However, we believe there is still room for cultural differences in the definition of the babyface and in inferences regarding the babyface in different cultural contexts. The babyface distributes in a given ratio among different ethnic groups, but the definition of the babyface in terms of facial structures and social perceptions varies across cultures. We make judgments about faces based on visual information, cognitive learning such as attention training, and the usefulness of information to observers (Zebrowitz et al., 2011). All of these cognitive strategies have been found to have systematic cultural differences by cultural psychologists for decades (Peng and Nisbett, 1999; Nisbett et al., 2001). Hence, we cannot conclude that babyface perceptions and inferences can easily escape cultural influence from a cultural psychology perspective. It is widely known that the Chinese holistic cognitive style is concerned more with integrated features, and the American analytical cognitive style is concerned more with prominent features. Evidence also suggests that Japanese use more configuration information in face perception than Caucasian (Miyamoto et al., 2011). Therefore, we assumed that Chinese and American participants would differ in babyface perceptions and inferences. Chinese should focus more on the combination of all facial features and Americans should be more sensitive to some highlighted babyfaced features. Forehead height and WHR should show cultural differences, because they are closely related to the integrated perception of a face. Eyebrow height and chin width should show cultural differences, because they are highlighted babyfaced features.

Child-like traits from the babyface have been reported by previous studies. We summarized 17 traits which can be inferred from the babyface, including naïveté, attractiveness, likeability, caring, friendliness, kindness, honesty, trustworthiness, health, openness, extroversion, emotional stability, confidence, intelligence, leadership ability, aggressiveness, and threat (Berry and McArthur, 1985; McArthur and Berry, 1987; Zebrowitz et al., 1992, 1993, 2007b, 2015; Albright et al., 1997; Zebrowitz and Montepare, 2005; Zebrowitz, 2006). These 17 traits were evaluated in our study in researching cultural differences in inferences made about the babyface. A stereotype content model (Caprariello et al., 2009; Cuddy et al., 2009) applies a method to refine these traits. They could be divided into two categories—warmth and competence. We anticipated that the babyface overgeneralization hypothesis and the halo effect of attractiveness should also work on Chinese faces. There should be a positive correlation between attractiveness, health, warm traits and babyfacedness. However, cultural differences may appear when perceiving the trait of competence. There may be no negative correlation between these traits and babyfacedness, because there exists an evolutionary mechanism by which the babyface is a wise strategy to help Chinese people gain more resources. A round face shape, especially a babyface, will help them to get limited resources in limited time, which may increase, not decrease their competence inferences (Buss, 1989; Cunningham et al., 1995). Unlike previous studies, we hypothesized that the Chinese babyface may lead to differences in competence inferences.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Seventy-two undergraduate students from Tsinghua University, 38 males, and 34 females, participated in the experiment. Data from six participants were excluded from further analyses because they are not Chinese. The valid data included 66 people (32 females, age: M ± SD, 21.5 ± 3.17 years old; 34 males, 21.56 ± 3.60 years old, range: 18–31 years old). Seventy-eight undergraduate students from the University of California Berkeley, 26 males, 52 females, participated in the experiment. Data from twelve participants who are not Caucasian were excluded from further analyses. The valid data included 66 people (44 females, 21.07 ± 4.89 years old; 22 males, 22.91 ± 4.36 years old, range: 18–34 years old). The study was approved by both Tsinghua University and University of California Berkeley Institutional Review Board, and all participants gave informed consent.

Stimuli

In this study, Chinese faces were researched as experiment material after being filtered and measured. Chinese faces came from the Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS)—Pose, Expression, Accessories, and Lighting (PEAL) Large-Scale Chinese Face Database, including 1040 adult volunteers (445 women) (Gao et al., 2008). In the pre-experiment, the black-white photo group with the unified background, light, focal length, neutral expression, and no ornaments was chosen.

We applied Active Shape Models to value the level of babyfacedness and ranked all faces in Stasm Software (Milborrow and Nicolls, 2014). All the 1040 faces were first marked with 38 fixed points (Zebrowitz-McArthur and Montepare, 1989; Zebrowitz et al., 2003, 2007b, 2010). The result of photo pointing is to get the coordinate value of 38 fixed points by setting up a rectangular coordinate system with the bottom left point of the screen as the origin (Figure 1). Referring to previous research, 18 feature vectors were described by the coordinates of facial standard points (Table 1). All the values were standardized by pupil distance (PD).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. An example of a pointing picture.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Facial Structure Feature Vectors and Fixed Points.

Following Berry and McArthur (1985) and Zebrowitz-McArthur and Montepare (1989), we found that the feature vectors significantly correlated with the babyfacedness and the correlation coefficients between them (Table 2). The standardized vectors, which were selected, were eye size, eyebrow height, forehead height (from highest point of forehead and highest point of nose), eye shape, chin width, PD, and cheek smoothness degree.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Selected vectors and correlation coefficients with babyfacedness.

Initial filter formula as follows, DA, DB, and DC represent the value of babyfacedness.

DA = Eye size + Eyebrow height + Forehead height1           + Chin width + PD + Cheek smoothness degree    (1)
DB = r1 × Eye size + r2 × Eyebrow height +r3           ×  Forehead height2 + r4 × Eye shape + r5           × Chin width + r6 × PD + r7            × Cheek smoothness degree    (2)
DC = r1 × Eye size + r2 × Eyebrow height +r3           ×  Forehead height2 + r4 × Eye shape + r5           × Chin width + r6 × PD + r7            × Cheek smoothness degree

Comparing the three kinds of babyfacedness value, significant positive correlations were found between DA and DB, r(46) = 0.96, p < 0.01; DA and DC, r(46) = 0.96, p < 0.01; DB and DC, r(46) = 1.00, p < 0.01. It can be inferred that the difference of correlation coefficients had little influence on babyfacedness value. Faces were ranked by the average value of DA, DB, and DC.

In the list of faces, 23 male faces and 23 female faces were chosen as the stimuli where the step size equaled to 20, from the least babyfaced to the most babyfaced. For each level, the babyfacedness values of faces are close. We controlled three confounding variables in our study: First, age. The faces are between the ages of 22 and 45 as inferred from their appearance (23 females, 29.14 ± 4.87 years old; 23 males, 30.46 ± 5.44 years old). Second, attractiveness. The face which was chosen is the face with average attractiveness among faces of each level. Third, front bangs. We chose the faces in which the forehead can be seen, instead of those in which the forehead is totally covered by front bangs. By controlling these confounding variables, we can not only control the unexpected influence of attractiveness, perceived age and hair style, but also avoid the confound effects of the attractiveness of faces on levels of babyfacedness.

Procedure

The experiment was conducted in a quiet and bright lab. Participants were asked to fill in the questionnaire individually on a computer with 17 inch LCD monitor (1280 × 1024, 60 HZ). All face images were displayed in 360 × 480 pixel size, 96-dpi. After studying the concept of the babyface: “Babyface, referring to the facial features of those with newborns face,” participants were asked to practice choosing a more babyfaced face from two female and two male faces. If the choice was not correct, a second trial was conducted. If the choice was still wrong at the second trial, then participants were sent back to study the concept of the babyface. They would not enter the next trial until the choice was right.

The gender of faces was randomly presented in the formal trial to ensure the same presentation times of female and male faces. Everyone should react to only one gender. There was no right or wrong in the formal experiment.

The first stage was to practice a forced choice task. 23 faces were presented randomly in pairs, totaling 253 pairs. Participants were asked to judge the two faces with the same gender (two male faces or two female faces), and then give a reaction as soon and as correctly as possible. They pressed S, if the left face was more babyfaced. And L, if the right one was more babyfaced. There was no time limit. The purpose of forced choice task is to make participants practice their definition of babyface and decide what makes a face look babyfaced. The results were not considered in our final analysis or evaluation.

Second, participants were asked to grade the babyfacedness of 23 faces (male or female) presented randomly from 0 to 100, with 0 = the least babyfaced, and 100 = the most babyfaced.

In the rest stage, participants could rest for 10 minutes.

Third, according to the grade of every face given by the participant in the second stage, the most babyfaced and the least babyfaced faces were presented randomly. On a 7-point Likert-scale, participants evaluated 17 traits, including naïveté, attractiveness, likeability, caring, friendliness, kindness, honesty, trustworthiness, health, openness, extroversion, emotional stability, confidence, intelligence, leadership ability, aggressiveness, and threat. And last, participants graded the babyfacedness of these two faces again from 0 to 100 and answered how certain they were of their judgment.

When participants finished all three stages, the experiment was over. American participants used the same program as the Chinese, while the language was English. The introduction was translated by a bilingual speaker and checked by a native speaker.

Results

Facial Structures of the Babyface

With the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, we found that the grades of babyfacedness given by Chinese and American participants showed no systematic difference. For female faces, D = 0.44, p = 0.99. For male faces, D = 0.30, p = 0.24.

Hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) techniques were used to investigate the effect of facial structures and culture on the perception of babyfacedness. The outcome variable is babyfacedness, which was rated by the evaluation of individual faces on a 100-point scale. The face-level, which is level 1, consists of face gender (a dummy variable, 0 = Female, 1 = Male) and eight facial features (cheek smoothness degree; eye size; forehead height; eyebrow height; chin width; eye separation; PD; Facial width-to-height ratio, WHR). The culture-level, which is level 2, consists of two cultural backgrounds, both Chinese and American. Culture is a dummy variable (0 = Chinese; 1 = American).

Table 3 presents descriptive statistics and correlations among the variables. Table 4 shows the HLM results. As shown in Model 1 of Table 4, face gender (γ = 4.69, p = 0.02), eye size (γ = 227.41, p < 0.01), eyebrow height (γ = 240.98, p < 0.01) and WHR (γ = 160.51, p < 0.01) are positively related to babyfacedness, but forehead height (γ = −26.69, p < 0.01), chin width (γ = −31.03, p < 0.01), and PD (γ = −0.50, p < 0.01) are negatively related to babyfacedness.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Means, standard deviations, and correlations between the variables.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Hierarchical Linear Modeling Result: Effect of Facial Structures and Culture on the Babyface.

Examining the effects of culture, Model 2 of Table 4 shows that culture has interaction effects with forehead height (γ = 22.77, p = 0.03) (Figure 2A), eyebrow height (γ = 129.46, p < 0.01) (Figure 2B), chin width (γ = −21.59, p = 0.04) (Figure 2C), and WHR (γ = 48.66, p = 0.01) (Figure 2D) on babyfacedness.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Interaction Effects among the facial features and culture. (A) Interaction effects between forehead height and culture on babyfacedness. (B) Interaction effects between eyebrow height and culture on babyfacedness. (C) Interaction effects between chin width and culture on babyfacedness. (D) Interaction effects between WHR and culture on babyfacedness.

The mixed model is:

Babyfacedness=γ00+γ01Culture+γ10Face gender+γ11CultureFace gender +γ20Cheek smoothness degree                               + γ21Culture Cheek smoothness degree+γ30Eye size+γ31Culture Eye size+γ40Forehead height                               + γ41CultureForehead height+γ50Eyebrow height+γ51CultureEyebrow height+γ60Chin width                               + γ61CultureChin width+γ70Eye separation+γ71CultureEye separation+γ80Pupil distance                               + γ81CulturePupil distance+γ90WHR+γ91CultureWHR

Table 5 shows results of multiple regression analysis predicting babyfacedness from facial structures. The dependent variable is the average of babyfacedness rating of each face. We applied a backward elimination method in our analysis. Results showed facial structures contributed differently to babyface perception in the two cultures. For Chinese judges, WHR, PD, and forehead height contribute more to babyface perception than other cues. For American judges, WHR, PD, and eyebrow height made greater contributions.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of multiple regression analysis predicting babyfacedness from facial structures.

Trait Impressions of the Babyface

Attractiveness and health are kinds of evolutionary traits and related more directly to evolutionary tendencies, so we analyzed them individually. We conducted a factor analysis on the other 15 traits. Bartlett's test of sphericity was significant, χ(66)2 = 2028.45, p < 0.01, showing a factor structure, KMO = 0.896; these traits can be analyzed by factors. Principal component analysis and Promax rotation (Kappa = 4) were adopted. Items with the loading of < 0.60 were gradually deleted. One more analysis will be conducted with every change. Twelve remaining traits are presented in Table 6. Following the stereotype content model (Caprariello et al., 2009; Cuddy et al., 2009), these traits were divided into two categories—warmth and competence. Commonalities of all the items were more than 0.50; 67.44% of the total variance was explained by warmth and competence.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Component matrix: trait impressions of babyface.

Kindness, caring, trustworthy, honesty, threatening, friendliness, likability, aggressiveness, and naïve are labeled as warmth traits; leadership, confidence, and intelligence are labeled as competence traits.

We conducted MANOVA to analyze attractiveness with the perceived age as covariates, culture (Two cultures:0 = Chinese, 1 = American), face gender (Two face genders:0 = Female, 1 = Male), and babyfacedness (Two babyfacedness levels:1 = Low, the least babyfaced face chosen in the second stage; 2 = High, the most babyfaced) as factors. There is no significant influence from perceived age [F(1, 255) = 0.61, p = 0.44, ηp2 = 0.00]. Babyfacedness has a significant main effect [F(1, 255) = 74.98, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.23].

We analyzed the simple effect of significant interaction effects among the variables of babyfacedness, face gender, and culture. We found that when perceiving female [F(1, 255) = 97.33, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.28] and male faces [F(1, 255) = 10.03, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.04], regardless of whether Chinese [F(1, 255) = 82.40, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.24] or American [F(1, 255) = 18.78, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.07], the attractiveness of the high-level babyface is significantly higher than the low-level. But a high-level babyface seems to be more attractive [F(1, 255) = 7.62, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.03] and a low-level babyface seems to be less attractive [F(1, 255) = 4.59, p = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.02] for Chinese participants than Americans. A male mature face [F(1, 255) = 9.95, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.04] and a female babyface [F(1, 255) = 22.83, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.08] look more attractive (Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Interaction effects among the variables of the babyface, face gender, and culture. (A) Interaction effects between the babyface and culture, babyface and face gender on attractiveness. Covariate: perceived age = 31.54. (B) Interaction effects between the babyface and culture for two face genders on competence. Covariates: attractiveness = −0.96, perceived age = 31.54.

We conducted MANOVA to analyze health, warmth and competence traits with attractiveness and perceived age as covariates, because there is positive correlation between attractiveness, health, warm traits (Table 7), and babyfacedness and age effect on the babyface (Boshyan et al., 2014; Zebrowitz and Franklin, 2014). Attractiveness [Wilk's lambda = 0.74, F(3, 252) = 30.19, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.26], perceived age [Wilk's lambda = 0.91, F(3, 252) = 8.34, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.09] and babyfacedness [Wilk's lambda = 0.66, F(3, 252) = 44.24, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.35] have significant influence when considered jointly on the variables health, warmth, and competence.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Means, standard deviations, and correlations between the variables: age, attractiveness, health, warmth, and competence.

A separate ANCOVA was conducted for each dependent variable with attractiveness and perceived age as covariates. We analyzed simple effect of significant interaction effects among the variables of babyfacedness, face gender, and culture.

Attractiveness has a significant main effect on health rating [F(1, 254) = 21.08, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.08], warmth [F(1, 254) = 46.82, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.16], and competence [F(1, 254) = 44.52, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.15]. Perceived age has a significant main effect on warmth [F(1, 254) = 11.77, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.04] and competence [F(1, 254) = 6.71, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.03], but not health [F(1, 254) = 0.14, p = 0.71, ηp2 = 0.00]. Babyfacedness level also has a significant main effect on warmth [F(1, 254) = 108.22, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.30] and competence [F(1, 254) = 25.43, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.09], but not health [F(1, 254) = 2.32, p = 0.13, ηp2 = 0.01].

The interaction effect of culture, face gender, and babyfacedness was significant, F(1, 254) = 6.17, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.02. Figure 3B shows that the male babyface shows less competence than mature faces for both Chinese [F(1, 254) = 22.10, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.08] and Americans [F(1, 254) = 18.64, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.07]. However, cultural differences are found for female faces. The female babyface looks less competent for Americans [F(1, 254) = 13.71, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.05], but no significant differences are found between babyface and mature faces for Chinese [F(1, 254) = 0.00, p = 0.96, ηp2 = 0.00]. Compared with the Chinese, Americans are convinced that female mature faces are significantly more competent [F(1, 254) = 8.75, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.03]. Gender differences were also found in that for Chinese judges, male mature faces showed more competence than female mature faces.

Discussion

The babyface phenomenon seems to be easily found among male Chinese faces, which is similar to white faces (McArthur and Apatow, 1984; Berry and McArthur, 1985). However, we also found the babyface effect on female faces, which may be due to the earlier cessation of growth which causes female faces to retain more neotenous traits (Jones et al., 1995; Tanikawa et al., 2015). But because of the halo effect of attractiveness, further analysis is needed. In the second stage of our experiment, participants graded the babyfacedness at all babyface levels; we did not find significant gender differences when considering the effect of culture. Since we did not ask participants to evaluate the attractiveness of all faces, we may not be able to control attractiveness as a covariate when analyzing the relationship between facial features and babyfacedness. But, with the data of trait evaluation tasks in the third stage, we conducted an ANOVA, with attractiveness, culture and face gender as independent variables and babyfacedness as dependent variables. The main effect of attractiveness was significant, F(14, 208) = 6.11, p < 0.01, ηp2=0.29. The interaction effect between attractiveness and face gender was not statistically significant, F(14, 208) = 1.73, p = 0.052, ηp2 = 0.10. According to the result, we may not indicate that babyfacedness ratings of female faces co-vary by attractiveness more, because these data did not include all levels of babyfacedness. One possible explanation is that either male or female faces co-vary by attractiveness. More evidence is needed in the future.

We found that a lower forehead and closer PD were indices of the Chinese babyface. A different definition may be needed for the Chinese babyface. According to previous research about Caucasian faces, the babyface is usually defined as a round face with big eyes, wide PD, high raised eyebrows, a small nose, and low vertical placement of features, which yields a large forehead and a small chin. In our study, a Chinese babyface, a face with a high level of babyfacedness, has bigger eyes, a lower forehead, higher eyebrows, a smaller chin, a narrow PD, and greater WHR. The finding that faces with bigger eyes, higher eyebrows, a narrower chin and greater WHR were more babyfaced replicates many previous studies by Zebrowitz et al. (2015), including studies examining East Asian faces. But the facial features inferring the Chinese babyface, a lower forehead, and a narrower pupil, are new findings. Berry and McArthur (1985) and McArthur and Berry (1987) did not find that forehead height was related to American babyface. We can find that participants use different facial structures to determine the Chinese babyface and Caucasian babyface. Cunningham et al. (1995) demonstrated an evolutionary mechanism which can explain this difference. Cultural groups will modify their reproductive strategies. Comparing Whites and Blacks, greater sexual restraint is produced among Asians because of the harsh climate in North Asia. To get limited resources in limited time, evolutionary changes occurred in their appearance and life style. A round face shape, especially a babyface, will help them to delay and reduce their sexual activities. Asians may be faster on the evolutionary trend of babyface. Hence, people use different definitions to judge the Chinese babyface and Caucasian babyface.

What's more, we find interaction effects between culture and forehead height, eyebrow height, chin width, and WHR on the babyfacedness of Chinese faces. Chinese participants consider a face as more babyfaced with a lower forehead, higher raised eyebrow and greater WHR than American participants. But for Americans, a narrower chin contributes more for the babyfacedness of a face.

When evaluating the babyfacedness of a face, WHR and PD contribute the same to babyface perception for Chinese and American judges. But, cultural differences show that Chinese judges care more about forehead height, because the Chinese are concerned more with the combined result of all facial features. Forehead height is related to the entire facial shape, accounting for a high proportion of a face. Peng and Nisbett (1999) and Nisbett et al. (2001) indicated that it is the result of a holistic cognitive style. In contrast, Americans are accustomed to analytical thinking. One or a few prominent characteristics, such as eyebrow height, will lead to a conclusion of babyface. This finding verified our hypothesis. Different cultural groups with different cognitive styles will take different facial features into account.

Based on the coefficients of the mixed model, the influence of culture has a significant tendency (p = 0.08). When judging on a same Chinese face, especially a babyface, American tend to rate it higher in babyfacedness. A Chinese babyface (Figure 4) is usually perceived to be more babyfaced for the American judges than for the Chinese judges. This result is inconsistent with evidence that Korean faces are judged more babyfaced than White faces by both American and Korean judges, and Korean judges rate both Korean and White as more babyfaced than do American judges (Zebrowitz et al., 2007a). Both Chinese faces and Korean faces are kinds of Asian faces. But these two kinds of faces are not totally the same. Previous studies show DNA evidence of Koreans and we have reasons to infer that the shape of Korean's eyes is more narrow and elongated (Jin et al., 2003, 2009). But as we know, bigger eyes are the key index of a babyface. From this perspective, Chinese faces seem to be more babyfaced than Korean faces and also American faces. Zebrowitz et al. (2007a) didn't indicate if the rates of Korean and American differ systematically. Their explanation is that it comes from face race effect. We found that a typical Chinese babyface is judged more babyfaced by Americans than Chinese. One possible reason is that Chinese faces are more babyfaced than Korean faces and American faces. Even with the effect of face race, Americans still judge Chinese babyfaces as more babyfaced. More research is needed.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. A pair of typical Chinese babyfaces, female babyface, (A) and male babyface (B).

We argue that a babyface is more attractive in general, which is consistent with the babyface overgeneralization hypothesis and the halo effect of attractiveness. There is a remarkable agreement in the warmth inference of the babyface all over the world (Zebrowitz et al., 2012), which is also true in our study. However, an interesting cultural difference is that Chinese participants seem to be more extreme. They consider a high-level babyface as more attractive and a low-level babyface as less attractive than Americans. Apparently, Chinese people like the female babyfaces and male mature faces more, which can be easily explained by evolutionary tendencies (Buss, 1989; Zebrowitz, 2003; Zebrowitz and Montepare, 2008). This phenomenon may also reflect a preference for own-race faces, which is consisted with previous research (Zebrowitz et al., 2007a).

It is usually believed that the babyface can lead to impressions of weakness, obedience, naïve characteristics (McArthur and Apatow, 1984; Berry and McArthur, 1985) and more femininity (Buss, 1989; Zebrowitz, 2006). Zebrowitz et al. (2012) only examined male faces and found a negative effect of babyface on health. Zebrowitz and Franklin (2014) tested both male and female faces and found a stronger influence of babyface on older adults than young adults in the impressions of health. However, we found no effect of babyface on health rating among young adults. Because the halo effect of attractiveness makes faces look healthier, we only found that attractiveness has a significant effect on health.

In our study, we found that for male Chinese faces, both Chinese and Americans believed that the babyface shows less competence than mature faces. But for the female Chinese faces, the Chinese do not consider the female babyface as less competent, but it is less competent for American judges. The American inference about Chinese female babyfaces may simply be a natural extension of the babyface in general. However, given that the babyface is an evolutionary tendency and survival strategy, a Chinese female babyface implies more fertility and attractiveness, but no less competence. Because it is an evolutionary result, there is no doubt that a female babyface shows the same competence with mature faces for Chinese participants. Furthermore, Chinese may have often encountered numerous competent Chinese women who happen to have babyfaces; naturalistic realism may be the core source of the cultural differences. Additional research is needed to evaluate if babyfaced Chinese women also have the same social status and power as their peers. Gender differences suggesting that mature male faces are more competent than mature female faces for the Chinese can be explained by evolutionary tendencies (Buss, 1989; Zebrowitz, 2003; Zebrowitz and Montepare, 2008). A man with a mature face may possess more resources, more wealth, and higher social status.

The current study suggests that the facial structures and first impressions of the babyface are not necessarily universal. A Chinese babyface may be more babyfaced in the eyes of Americans, but it is not perceived to be less competent, and may be seen as even more attractive in the eyes of the Chinese. Recognizing the cultural differences in an evolution-based natural phenomenon may enrich our understanding of human commonality and diversity.

Author Contributions

WZ developed the study concept with KP. WZ developed the experimental paradigm with QY and FY. WZ and QY conducted the experiment and collected the data. WZ performed the data analysis and interpretation under the supervision of KP. WZ and KP drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the discussion of the manuscript. QY and FY provided critical revisions. All authors approved the work for publication.

Notes

1. According to Berry and McArthur (1985), forehead height1 is the distance between highest point of forehead and the connection of the two highest eyebrow points.

2. According to Zebrowitz-McArthur and Montepare (1989), forehead height2 is the distance between highest point of forehead and the connection of two pupil center points.

Funding

This work was supported by National Nature Science Foundations of China (No. 31170973 and No. 31471001) and Tsinghua University Top Genius Training Program (Spark Plan).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Celia Hsueh for conducting the study in the University of California Berkeley, Pengxiang Cheng, and Cheng Li for valuable contributions on dealing with photos with Stasm software, Ling Ma, and Bo Ning for helping us design the experiment program, Timothy Beneke for helping us edit the manuscript.

References

Albright, L., Malloy, T. E., Dong, Q., Kenny, D. A., Fang, X., Winquist, L., et al. (1997). Cross-cultural consensus in personality judgments. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72:558. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.3.558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, D. S., and McArthur, L. Z. (1985). Some components and consequences of a babyface. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48:312. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.48.2.312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boshyan, J., Zebrowitz, L. A., Franklin, R. G., McCormick, C. M., and Carré, J. M. (2014). Age similarities in recognizing threat from faces and diagnostic cues. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 69, 710–718. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behav. Brain Sci. 12, 1–14. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00023992

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caprariello, P. A., Cuddy, A. J., and Fiske, S. T. (2009). Social structure shapes cultural stereotypes and emotions: a causal test of the stereotype content model. Group Processes Intergroup Relat. 12, 147–155. doi: 10.1177/1368430208101053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, M. A., and Zebrowitz, L. A. (1995). The contributions of appearance to occupational outcomes in civilian and military settings1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 25, 129–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1995.tb01588.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuddy, A. J., Fiske, S. T., Kwan, V. S., Glick, P., Demoulin, S., Leyens, J. P., et al. (2009). Stereotype content model across cultures: towards universal similarities and some differences. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 48, 1–33. doi: 10.1348/014466608X314935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, M. R., Roberts, A. R., Barbee, A. P., Druen, P. B., and Wu, C.-H. (1995). “Their ideas of beauty are, on the whole, the same as ours”: consistency and variability in the cross-cultural perception of female physical attractiveness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68:261. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.2.261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, W., Cao, B., Shan, S., Chen, X., Zhou, D., Zhang, X., et al. (2008). The CAS-PEAL large-scale Chinese face database and baseline evaluations. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern. A Syst. Hum. 38, 149–161. doi: 10.1109/TSMCA.2007.909557

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, H.-J., Kwak, K.-D., Hammer, M. F., Nakahori, Y., Shinka, T., Lee, J.-W., et al. (2003). Y-chromosomal DNA haplogroups and their implications for the dual origins of the Koreans. Hum. Genet. 114, 27–35. doi: 10.1007/s00439-003-1019-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, H.-J., Tyler-Smith, C., and Kim, W. (2009). The peopling of Korea revealed by analyses of mitochondrial DNA and Y-chromosomal markers. PLoS ONE 4:e4210. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, D., Brace, C. L., Jankowiak, W., Laland, K. N., Musselman, L. E., Langlois, J. H., et al. (1995). Sexual selection, physical attractiveness, and facial neoteny: cross-cultural evidence and implications [and comments and reply]. Curr. Anthropol. 36, 723–748. doi: 10.1086/204427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingston, R. W., and Pearce, N. A. (2009). The teddy-bear effect does having a baby face benefit black chief executive officers? Psychol. Sci. 20, 1229–1236. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02431.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McArthur, L. Z., and Apatow, K. (1984). Impressions of baby-faced adults. Soc. Cogn. 2:315. doi: 10.1521/soco.1984.2.4.315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McArthur, L. Z., and Berry, D. S. (1987). Cross-cultural agreement in perceptions of babyfaced adults. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 18, 165–192. doi: 10.1177/0022002187018002003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milborrow, S., and Nicolls, F. (2014). “Active Shape Models with SIFT Descriptors and MARS,” in VISAPP, Vol. 2, (Lisbon: IEEE), 380–387.

Miyamoto, Y., Yoshikawa, S., and Kitayama, S. (2011). Feature and configuration in face processing: Japanese are more configural than Americans. Cogn. Sci. 35, 563–574. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01163.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychol. Rev. 108:291. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, K., and Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. Am. Psychol. 54:741. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.9.741

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poutvaara, P., Jordahl, H., and Berggren, N. (2009). Faces of politicians: babyfacedness predicts inferred competence but not electoral success. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 1132–1135. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanikawa, C., Zere, E., and Takada, K. (2015). Sexual dimorphism in the facial morphology of adult humans: a three-dimensional analysis. HOMO J. Comp. Hum. Biol. 67, 23–49. doi: 10.1016/j.jchb.2015.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, A., Pakrashi, M., and Oosterhof, N. N. (2009). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness after minimal time exposure. Soc. Cogn. 27, 813–833. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.813

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A. (2003). Commentary: overgeneralization effects in perceiving nonverbal behavior: evolutionary and ecological origins. J. Nonverbal Behav. 27, 133–138. doi: 10.1023/A:1023966526601

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A. (2006). Finally, faces find favor. Soc. Cogn. 24:657. doi: 10.1521/soco.2006.24.5.657

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Andreoletti, C., Collins, M. A., Lee, S. Y., and Blumenthal, J. (1998). Bright, bad, babyfaced boys: appearance stereotypes do not always yield self-fulfilling prophecy effects. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75:1300. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1300

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Bronstad, P. M., and Lee, H. K. (2007a). The contribution of face familiarity to ingroup favoritism and stereotyping. Soc. Cogn. 25:306. doi: 10.1521/soco.2007.25.2.306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Bronstad, P. M., and Montepare, J. M. (2011). “An ecological theory of face perception,” in The Science of Social Vision, eds N. Ambady, R. Adams, K. Nakayama, and S. Shimojo (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 3–30.

Zebrowitz, L. A., Brownlow, S., and Olson, K. (1992). Baby talk to the babyfaced. J. Nonverbal Behav. 16, 143–158. doi: 10.1007/BF00988031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Fellous, J.-M., Mignault, A., and Andreoletti, C. (2003). Trait impressions as overgeneralized responses to adaptively significant facial qualities: evidence from connectionist modeling. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 7, 194–215. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0703_01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., and Franklin, R. G. Jr. (2014). The attractiveness halo effect and the babyface stereotype in older and younger adults: similarities, own-age accentuation, and older adult positivity effects. Exp. Aging Res. 40, 375–393. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2014.897151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Franklin, R. G., and Boshyan, J. (2015). Face shape and behavior: implications of similarities in infants and adults. Pers. Individ. Dif. 86, 312–317. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Kikuchi, M., and Fellous, J.-M. (2007b). Are effects of emotion expression on trait impressions mediated by babyfaceness? Evidence from connectionist modeling. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 33, 648–662. doi: 10.1177/0146167206297399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Kikuchi, M., and Fellous, J.-M. (2010). Facial resemblance to emotions: group differences, impression effects, and race stereotypes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 175. doi: 10.1037/a0017990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., and McDonald, S. M. (1991). The impact of litigants' baby-facedness and attractiveness on adjudications in small claims courts. Law Hum. Behav. 15, 603. doi: 10.1007/BF01065855

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., and Montepare, J. M. (1992). Impressions of babyfaced individuals across the life span. Dev. Psychol. 28:1143. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1143

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., and Montepare, J. M. (2005). Appearance DOES matter. Science 308, 1565–1566. doi: 10.1126/science.1114170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., and Montepare, J. M. (2008). Social psychological face perception: why appearance matters. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 2, 1497–1517. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00109.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Montepare, J. M., and Lee, H. K. (1993). They don't all look alike: individual impressions of other racial groups. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65:85. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.85

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Wang, R., Bronstad, P. M., Eisenberg, D., Undurraga, E., Reyes-García, V., et al. (2012). First impressions from faces among US and culturally isolated Tsimane'people in the Bolivian rainforest. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 43, 119–134. doi: 10.1177/0022022111411386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz-McArthur, L., and Montepare, J. M. (1989). Contributions of a babyface and a childlink voice to impressions of moving and talking faces. J. Nonverbal Behav. 13, 189–203. doi: 10.1007/BF00987049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: babyface, cultural differences, facial structure, face perception, trait impressions

Citation: Zheng W, Yang Q, Peng K and Yu F (2016) What's in the Chinese Babyface? Cultural Differences in Understanding the Babyface. Front. Psychol. 7:819. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00819

Received: 04 March 2016; Accepted: 17 May 2016;
Published: 31 May 2016.

Edited by:

Keiko Ishii, Kobe University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Matia Okubo, Senshu University, Japan
Leslie Zebrowitz, Brandeis University, USA

Copyright © 2016 Zheng, Yang, Peng and Yu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kaiping Peng, pengkp@mail.tsinghua.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.