Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 08 November 2016
Sec. Psychology of Language

Setting the Balance between the Lexical and Sublexical Pathways of Dual-Route Models of Reading: Insight from Atypical Dyslexia in Surgical Glioma Patients

  • 1Neurosurgery, Lariboisière Hospital, Paris, France
  • 2Université Paris 7 Diderot, Paris, France
  • 3IMNC UMR 8165, Orsay, France
  • 4Neurosurgery, Hôpital Gui de Chauliac, CHU Montpellier, Montpellier, France
  • 5Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale U1051, Montpellier, France
  • 6Université Montpellier 2, Montpellier, France

Reading is a complex skill that has been acquired only recently in the evolution of humans. This fascinating ability has motivated many neuropsychological models. In particular, it has been proposed that reading is sustained by two parallel and complementary systems (see for example Epelbaum et al., 2008 for a plausible anatomical implementation of this model): a lexico-semantic one and a phonological one (that further requires attentional resources for processing serially the different syllables of a given word). In parallel to advances in neuropsychological models of reading, computational models have been intensively studied. Such models attempt to go further than the usual “boxology”, by providing a simulation on a computer of the different cognitive processes that are involved in reading. One of the very first version in 1989 (Seidenberg and McClelland, 1989) was a connectionist approach, known as the “triangle model”, that already implemented the two routes (a semantic one and a phonological one). Since then, two categories have been competing in order to achieve the best fit between simulations and human datas in healthy and brain-damaged individuals: the dual-route cascade (DRC) (Coltheart et al., 2001) and the connectionist dual-process (CDP) (Zorzi et al., 1998) and its updated versions CDP+ (Perry et al., 2007), CDP++ (Perry et al., 2010), and CDP++-parser (Perry et al., 2013). Both kind of models share the same architecture regarding the lexical route, while differing in the sublexical route. In both cases, the balance between the lexical and sub-lexical route is a critical issue: whenever the lexical route is too strong, pseudowords will be read as a lexical neighbor, and whenever the sub-lexical route is too strong, error rates rise for irregular words (which are regularized by the sublexical route). As clearly explained in the paragraph “Searching parameter space” of Coltheart et al. (2001), the optimal balance is found in the DRC model by trial and error, testing the ability of the system to read a specific pair of irregular word and pseudoword (respectively “chef” and “starn”). Similarly, it is stated in Perry et al. (2007): “The first step was to determine the appropriate balance between lexical and sublexical phonology, which in turn largely depends on the speed at which the serial process of grapheme parsing occurs. These parameters need to be chosen together, because slower grapheme parsing speeds reduce the amount of sublexical phonology in the model, and faster speeds increase it. Performance on irregular words provides a particularly important benchmark for parameter setting.” Importantly, in both models, there is no explicit computational system that resets the balance between the two routes depending on the characteristics of each trial (regular, irregular, and pseudowords).

In a recent paper, Zemmoura et al. focused on reading abilities in a small series of patients operated on in awake conditions for a diffuse low-grade glioma of the posterior temporo-basal region (Zemmoura et al., 2015). Most importantly, it was reported that some patients (# 3, 4, & 5) had a long term impairment for reading irregular words and pseudo-words, while reading regular words was spared. These three patients had a cavity extending in the short vertical indirect portion of the arcuate fasciculus (see Catani et al., 2005 for a description of this tract).

This new kind of dissociation differs from the commonly reported surface dyslexia (specific deficit in irregular words reading) and phonological dyslexia (selective difficulty for reading pseudo-words). Those two acquired dyslexia are easily interpreted in the dual-route models: the lexical pathway is damaged in the surface dyslexia, while the sublexical pathway is injured in the phonological dyslexia. Our atypical dyslexia is more difficult to understand within the framework of dual-route processes. Zemmoura et al. (2015) thus interpreted these datas by assigning a feedback role to this short vertical indirect portion of the arcuate fasciculus: “Thus, to explain that reading out loud irregular and pseudowords can be impaired by an unique lesion, we propose that reading these words do not rely on a simple serial, feed-forward neural system, but rather on feedback connections linking visual to nonvisual information to create an interactive system for visual words recognition (Twomey et al., 2011). According to our observations, these feedback connections might therefore be involved in both the semantic and the phonological pathways. More particularly, this interactive system should be mostly recruited when spelling-sound incoherence (the case for irregular words) or when absence of meaning (the case of pseudowords) is detected;…”.

The simplest way to implement such feedback is the following (see Figure 1): let us assume that the two routes operate independently (at a subconscious level), each leading to a preliminar response in a (subliminal) phonological buffer. Then, some part of the brain analyzes the difference between the phonological responses generated by the lexical route and the sublexical route, and this signal is used as a feedback to re-balance the contribution of each route. In case of regular words, this difference is null, and no feedback is needed that would favor either routes. In case of pseudo-words, the difference is said negative, as there is no response from the lexical pathway. The feedback reinforces the sublexical pathway and inhibits the re-entry in the lexical pathway. In case of irregular words, the difference is said positive, as there is a difference and the phonological output coming from the lexical pathway is not zero. This “positive signal” will feedback in order to inhibit the sublexical route and amplify the lexical one. Within this “boxology” model, one can easily understand that an absence of this feeback signal will impact irregular and pseudo-words reading, while leaving unaffected regular words reading.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. “Boxology” model of a feedback allowing to tune the balance between the lexical and sub-lexical routes. LIMA, lateral inferotemporal multimodal area; VWFA, visual word form area; VBGA, visual bigram area. For regular words, the preliminar phonological outputs from the lexical and sublexical routes are the same, feedback signal is unnecessary. For irregular words, the difference between the two routes is said positive, generating a feedback signal boosting the lexical route while stopping the sublexical one. For pseudowords, the difference is said negative (as there is no entry from the lexical route), resulting in a feedback signal inhibiting any re-entry in the lexical route.

From previous neurospychological models and Zemmoura's work, one can propose the following anatomical substrates for this model: the visual bigram area (VBGA) (Dehaene et al., 2005) directly feeds the sublexical route, allowing the serial process of phonological reading, thanks to the participation of parietal areas sustaining the required attentional resources. Through connections corresponding anatomically to the U-fibers of the occipito-temporal projection system (Catani et al., 2003), this VBGA also feeds the visual word form area (VWFA) (Dehaene et al., 2005), which is itself linked to the lateral inferotemporal multimodal area (LIMA) (Cohen et al., 2004). This latter LIMA can be considered as the system making the association between the orthographic and phonological lexicons within the lexical route. The phonological buffer that performs the analysis of the difference between the outputs of the two routes is not well localized anatomically, but likely involves the superior temporal gyrus, the supramarginal gyrus, and the posterior part of inferior frontal gyrus (Epelbaum et al., 2008), all areas known to be involved in phonological processing (Vigneau et al., 2006). Finally, the feedback signal is sent through the short vertical portion of the arcuate fasciculus, amplifying VWFA, and its input to the LIMA [i.e., the interactive activation lexical network of Perry et al. (2007)] for irregular words, and inhibiting this same network in case of pseudowords reading (hence favoring the sublexical route). Finally, the lexical route is also probably in close relationship with the semantic system (as in the triangle model), but this interaction is beyond the scope of this paper.

Furthermore, this model could provide a new basis for analyzing functional MRI studies of reading. We would expect that areas computing the phonological difference and sending the feedback signal would show up in the contrast condition of irregular words or pseudowords versus regular words, and wash out in the contrast condition of irregular words versus pseudowords. Reinterpreting in this way data from a previous study (Binder et al., 2005), one can identify those areas as a network comprising Broca's area on both sides, pre-SMA on both sides, and part of the left supramarginal and angular gyri (see Figures 4B,C in the aforementioned paper). Otherwise, the LIMA should show an activity increasing in the order pseudowords < regular words < irregular words, an hypothesis that could be tested in future studies.

Although we found this “boxology” model of feedback very convenient to explain the atypical pattern of selective dyslexia for irregular and pseudo-words as reported by Zemmoura et al. (2015), it remains to be demonstrated whether these results could play any role in refining computational models of reading. This leads us to ask to the community of computational modelers the following questions:

- is it possible to simulate the atypical dyslexia within the current framework of DRC and CDP++-parser?

- if yes, which parameters should be changed and how?

- if not, does it mean that an explicit feedback (as the one we qualitatively described) should be incorporated in the models?

These questions should motivate further work in the exciting field of computational models of reading.

Author Contributions

All authors listed, have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

EM thanks Michel Thiebaut de Schotten and Marc Teichmann for fruitful discussions on this topic.

References

Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A., Desai, R., Conant, L. L., and Liebenthal, E. (2005). Some neurophysiological constraints on models of word naming. Neuroimage 27, 677–693. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catani, M., Jones, D. K., Donato, R., and Ffytche, D. H. (2003). Occipito-temporal connections in the human brain. Brain J. Neurol. 126(Pt 9), 2093–2107. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catani, M., Jones, D. K., and Ffytche, D. H. (2005). Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Ann. Neurol. 57, 8–16. doi: 10.1002/ana.20319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, L., Jobert, A., Le Bihan, D., and Dehaene, S. (2004). Distinct unimodal and multimodal regions for word processing in the left temporal cortex. NeuroImage 23, 1256–1270. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., and Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychol. Rev. 108, 204–256. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Sigman, M., and Vinckier, F. (2005). The neural code for written words: a proposal. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 335–341. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epelbaum, S., Pinel, P., Gaillard, R., Delmaire, C., Perrin, M., Dupont, S., et al. (2008). Pure alexia as a disconnection syndrome: new diffusion imaging evidence for an old concept. Cortex 44, 962–974. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C, and Zorzi, M. (2010). Beyond single syllables: large-scale modeling of reading aloud with the connectionist dual process (CDP++) Model. Cogn. Psychol. 61, 106–151. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., and Zorzi, M. (2013). A computational and empirical investigation of graphemes in reading. Cogn. Sci. 37, 800–828. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., and Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychol. Rev. 114, 273–315. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidenberg, M. S., and McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychol. Rev. 96, 523–568. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigneau, M., Beaucousin, V., Hervé, P. Y., Duffau, H., Crivello, F., Houdé, O., et al. (2006). Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. Neuroimage 30, 1414–1432. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zemmoura, I., Herbet, G., Moritz-Gasser, S., and Duffau, H. (2015). New insights into the neural network mediating reading processes provided by cortico-subcortical electrical mapping. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 2215–2230. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zorzi, M., Houhgton, G., and Butterworth, B. (1998). Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 1131–1161. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.4.1131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: computational models, reading, dual-route cascade, connectionist dual-process, glioma, surgery, fMRI

Citation: Mandonnet E and Duffau H (2016) Setting the Balance between the Lexical and Sublexical Pathways of Dual-Route Models of Reading: Insight from Atypical Dyslexia in Surgical Glioma Patients. Front. Psychol. 7:1730. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01730

Received: 04 September 2016; Accepted: 19 October 2016;
Published: 08 November 2016.

Edited by:

Carlo Semenza, University of Padua, Italy

Reviewed by:

Elena Salillas, Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, Spain

Copyright © 2016 Mandonnet and Duffau. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Emmanuel Mandonnet, mandonnet@mac.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.