Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 09 February 2017
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Commentary: Causal Effects in Mediation Modeling: An Introduction with Applications to Latent Variables

  • Health Disparities Institute, UConn Health, Farmington, CT, USA

A commentary on
Causal Effects in Mediation Modeling: An Introduction with Applications to Latent Variables

by Muthén, B., and Asparouhov, T. (2015). Struct. Equation Model. 22, 12–23. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.935843

Causal mediation1 is an increasingly popular analysis, as recently described by Muthén and Asparouhov (2015, M&A)2. We suggest a simplified notation for causal mediation effects, iT/iP = BK and dT/dP, provide a graphical view of potential outcomes (PO) and expand the M&A approach by using VanderWeele's (2014) mediation decomposition.

An intuitive way to label and see causal in/direct effects is to directly display POs, as in Figure 1 below. POs are values that could be observed, but have not been realized (yet). They reveal themselves partially once nature or researchers assign people to specific experimental conditions, or when people make choices. POs are useful in defining causal total effects (TE), as differences between the same individual's (i) two POs, Yi1 – Yi0, had the person been treated (subscript 1), and alternatively (but simultaneously) not treated (0); evidently, in our reality one of these has to be “contrary-to-fact” (CF).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Causal mediation represented with potential outcomes (POs). *Y01 is the PO of Y is X was set to 0 (subscript) but M took the value it would attain if X was instead set to 1 (superscript); * means that PO is unobservable/contrary-to-fact (CF). The upper level is the potential world if treated (X = 1), the bottom if not treated (X = 0); iP = BK/iT and dP/dT are pure/total indirect and direct effects (BK comes from the “classic” Baron-Kenny). The total effects (iT and dT, in bold) are shown as longer than their pure counterparts, iP = BK and dP (in italics), both by exactly INTMed, the mediated interaction. The arrows from lower to upper PO worlds are the two causal direct effects dP and dT, those from the left-side POs to the right-side POs capture the indirect effects iP = BK and iT, while the diagonal up and to right the total effect TE. TE can be decomposed then as dP + iT, or iP = BK + dT.

The indirect effect of X on Y through a mediator M is the part of the total effect that “flows through” M, or the contribution of the path X->M->Y to the observed association between X and Y, which is an open path because causal association flows through it (Elwert, 2013). The key problem in intuitively grasping causal in/direct effects is the “nesting” of the POs due to the double role of the mediator as a cause and an effect3: the PO “Y if X was set to x,” or Yx, can be combined with “Y if M was set to m,” or Ym (we suggest using a superscript for scenarios involving M). So *Y1M0, for example, labeled Y(1, M(0)) in M&A, is the PO of the outcome Y if a person was treated (1), but his/her mediator took on the value had s/he would belonged to the opposite (control) condition (M0). This PO is clearly contrary-to-fact (CF), never observable, a “cross-worlds” quantity (Lok, 2016), hence our * sign. Y00 and Y11 are in principle realizable, only one of them at a time for the same person, however.

The four key POs involved in understanding causal in/direct effects are shown in Figure 1. The total effect is decomposable into direct and indirect causal effects, possibly in two ways, through one of two fully contrary-to-fact POs: *Y10 or *Y01.

Both decompositions of TE can be obtained by adding and subtracting a fully CF intermediary term; e.g., through *Y10:

TE=Y11Y00=(Y11Y10)+(Y10Y00)=iT+dP(1)

Intuitively, one can see that the two vertical arrows are direct effects, because they capture the “change” in Y (in the PO world), marked by subscript/superscript changes: when “changing” only X, i.e., while (un-naturally) holding the mediator at a “constant” PO-value. The causal pure direct effect dP is often referred to as natural (or pure natural direct effect, PNDE, in M&A), because the mediator takes on the same value under the control condition, which would be the “natural” course of action without any change in nature.

Similarly, the two horizontal arrows are indirect effects, because they are the result of “changing” only M, while keeping X constant (at 0, or 1)4. The “upper” indirect effect is called also natural, but is in fact a total indirect effect (total natural indirect effect, TNIE, in M&A); it is total because it is a sum, of its pure kind, which we label iP = BK, and an interacted mediation component, see Equation (3) below; here X is kept “unchanged” at the treated level (1), yet the mediator “changes” its (potential) value, from its natural (control) value to the value “if treated.”

We suggest to label the pure indirect effect iP = BK, because its estimate for continuous M and Y matches the classic no interaction and no confounder Baron and Kenny (1986) indirect effect “a · b” (see Equation 8 in M&A, when an interaction X-by-M is specified).

The relation between the key causal effects dT and dP and iT and iP = BK has been revealed by VanderWeele's decomposition (VanderWeele, 2014), hence the total labels we proposed:

dT=dP+INTMedandiT=iP=BK+INTMed,(2)

where INTMed is the mediated interaction component5, which is the product of the interaction estimate and the X->M linear effect, βX*M · a, labeled γ1· β3 in M&A, see their Equations (5) and (9); INTMed is non-zero when X impacts M, and X and M interact in how they impact Y.

Because the Mplus software code in M&A for computing causal in/direct effects did not estimate the effects proposed by VanderWeele's “decomposition” (mediated interaction, controlled direct effect, proportion attributable to interaction, and portion eliminated), we expand the Mplus code for continuous M and Y to estimate them (see the online appendix at https://bit.ly/pos_frontiers); we present an expanded VanderWeele SAS code too, which estimates the Mplus additional effects: pure direct, total indirect and total direct.

To illustrate, we estimated effects from a weight-loss randomized intervention data (SisterTalk Hartford, Burleson et al., 2008; de-identified data for replication available in appendix), which was meant to improve food habits and consequently reduce BMI in African-American women; effects are shown in Equation (3) (following VanderWeele's Figure 4, 2013, which is an expanded online version of the published (VanderWeele, 2014); * signals statistically significant at p < 0.05, NS signifies non-significant):

TE0.663=mCDE+mINTRefdP=0.495(75%)+INTMed+BKiT=0.168(25%)0.507(76%)0.012NS(2%)0.021NS(3%)dT=0.474(71%)0.189(29%)iP=BK=0.189(29%)(3)

where INTMed is the mediated interaction, BK is the “Baron and Kenny” causal indirect effect, mCDE is the controlled direct effect, mINTRef reference interaction, with superscript m signaling that those effects depend on what value m the analyst decided to estimate them at.

The total effect TE was −0.66 BMI units (approx. −3.9 lbs. for an average 64 inch woman). The mediated interaction effect INTMed is about 3% of the TE, and statistically non-significant, hence statistically iT =stat.  iP = BK and dT =stat. dP (“stat.” signals statistical, not mathematical, equality), so one can report the classic iBK6: the weight loss achieved through improving one's food habits is about 25% of the total effect, while the residual direct effect is about 75% of it.

While POs are central to “causal” mediation, visually “seeing” them is challenging, yet, when achieved, it helps uncover the mechanics behind causal direct and indirect effect estimation. Intuitive graphical displays could aid in visualizing some assumptions, many of which refer to relations between POs, and not their observed cousins (e.g., ignorability, or unconfoundedness, Imai et al., 2010); such assumptions ensure identifiability of in/direct causal effects.

We hope that the simplified notation and a visual display of how causal in/direct effects emerge from a mix of the POs of the mediator and the final outcome can contribute to a more intuitive understanding and reporting of causal mediation, as presented in the seminal paper we commented on. The notational bridge and cross-pollination of software syntaxes we suggested should facilitate such an improved understanding.

Author Contributions

ENC has developed the idea, FT has verified the claims, expanded, and revised the manuscript extensively, JF has worked on the theoretical and design portion of the original study and has revised and edited the manuscript.

Funding

The Sistertalk Hartford project was funded by the Patrick and Catherine Weldon Donaghue Medical Research Foundation.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Judea Pearl for his useful suggestions that were instrumental in clarifying and simplifying our language, and to Ali Ünlü, who provided us with comprehensive yet friendly constructive criticism and suggestions.

Footnotes

1. ^The label “causal” mediation reflects more than the expansion of the original Baron and Kenny model to allow for X-by-M interaction, and does not suddenly make any three-variable model causal in the profound sense. Causal mediation relies on meeting other assumptions, like the no-confounder assumption of M and Y, and would require causal investigations like those afforded by Direct Acyclic Graphs (DAGs, Greenland et al., 1999).

2. ^Other dominant causal mediation “schools” are led by the Imai (Imai et al., 2010) and Pearl (Pearl, 2001) teams, first centered on R and Stata implementations, the latter more theoretical and non-parametrical. They differ also in terms of formulating the assumptions for identification of the causal in/direct effects.

3. ^Pearl (2013) calls them nested counterfactuals; the key insight Sewall Wright foresaw when proposing the path analytic method may have been that the change in Y in relation to the change in X (the slope δY/δX), traced on the path through an intermediary M, is linked to the slopes δM/δX and δY/δM following the composite function chain rule of derivatives: δy/δx = δy/δm · δm/δx, which mirrors the Baron and Kenny i = a · b. Adding the contributions of all such X-to-Y open paths yields the model predicted association between X and Y (see the “tracing rule,” Loehlin, 2004).

4. ^The fact that there is possibly more than one indirect (and hence direct) effect to estimate follows from the interaction of X and M in causing Y, which makes the effect of M on Y vary with X (or the effect of X on Y vary with M).

5. ^The INTMed key component is defined in VanderWeele (2013) as (Y11 − *Y10 − *Y01 + Y00) (M1 – M0) which for continuous M and Y becomes either iT-iP = BK or dT-dP.

6. ^While we label the pure indirect effect iP = BK, as being the Baron and Kenny classic indirect effect, a · b, its estimate in the “causal” specification, with the X-by-M interaction term included, will not coincide of course with the estimate from the simpler model without interaction; in our case the classic BK estimate was iBK = −0.179 (SE = 0.054), p < 0.001, while iP = BK was −0.189 (SE = 0.069), p = 0.006.

References

Baron, R. M., and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1173. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burleson, J., Curry, L., Dauser-Forrest, D., Henderson, C., McKinney, M., Peele, M., et al. (2008). “Testing the Social Action Theory framework of a faith-based weight control program collaboratively translated with African-American and Black Women: SisterTalk Hartford,” in Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of North American Primary Care Research Group (Rio Grande).

Elwert, F. (2013). “Graphical causal models,” in Handbook of Causal Analysis for Social Research, ed S. L. Morgan (New York, NY: Springer), 245–273.

Google Scholar

Greenland, S., Robins, J. M., and Pearl, J. (1999). Confounding and collapsibility in causal inference. Stat. Sci. 14, 29–46.

Google Scholar

Imai, K., Keele, L., and Tingley, D. (2010). A general approach to causal mediation analysis. Psychol. Methods 15, 309–334. doi: 10.1037/a0020761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loehlin, J. C. (2004). Latent Variable Models: An introduction to Factor, Path, and Structural Equation Analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Lok, J. J. (2016). Defining and estimating causal direct and indirect effects when setting the mediator to specific values is not feasible. Stat. Med. 35, 4008–4020. doi: 10.1002/sim.6990

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, B., and Asparouhov, T. (2015). Causal effects in mediation modeling: an introduction with applications to latent variables. Struct. Equation Model. 22, 12–23. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.935843

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearl, J. (2001). “Direct and indirect effects” in Proceedings of the Seventeenth Conference on Uncertainty in Artificial Intelligence. Available online at: ftp://ftp.cs.ucla.edu/pub/stat_ser/R273-U.pdf

Google Scholar

Pearl, J. (2013). Structural counterfactuals: a brief introduction. Cogn. Sci. 37, 977–985. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

VanderWeele, T. J (2013). A Unification of Mediation and Interaction. Harvard University Biostatistics. Working Paper Series. Available online at: http://biostats.bepress.com/harvardbiostat/paper164

VanderWeele, T. J. (2014). A unification of mediation and interaction: a 4-way decomposition. Epidemiology 25, 749–761. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mediation, causal mediation, potential outcomes, causal inference, counterfactuals

Citation: Coman EN, Thoemmes F and Fifield J (2017) Commentary: Causal Effects in Mediation Modeling: An Introduction with Applications to Latent Variables. Front. Psychol. 8:151. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00151

Received: 08 October 2016; Accepted: 23 January 2017;
Published: 09 February 2017.

Edited by:

Pietro Cipresso, Istituto Auxologico Italiano (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Ali Ünlü, Technische Universität München, Germany

Copyright © 2017 Coman, Thoemmes and Fifield. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Emil N. Coman, comanus@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.