Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 16 May 2017
Sec. Emotion Science

Effects of Music on Agitation in Dementia: A Meta-Analysis

\r\nSiv K. A. PedersenSiv K. A. Pedersen1Per N. AndersenPer N. Andersen2Ricardo G. LugoRicardo G. Lugo1Marita AndreassenMarita Andreassen1Stefan Sütterlin,*Stefan Sütterlin1,3*
  • 1Department of Psychology, Inland Norway University of Applied Sciences, Lillehammer, Norway
  • 2Department of Education and Social Work, Inland Norway University of Applied Sciences, Lillehammer, Norway
  • 3Center for Clinical Neuroscience, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway

Agitation is a common problem in patients suffering from dementia and encompasses a variety of behaviors such as repetitive acts, restlessness, wandering, and aggressive behaviors. Agitation reduces the probability of positive social interaction and increases the psychological and organizational burden. While medical interventions are common, there is need for complementary or alternative methods. Music intervention has been brought forward as a promising method to reduce agitation in dementia. While interventions, target groups and research designs differ, there has so far not been a systematic overview assessing the effect of music intervention for agitation in patients with dementia. A meta-analysis was conducted in order to investigate possible effects of music interventions. Twelve studies met inclusion criteria. Music intervention had a medium overall effect on agitation in dementia, suggesting robust clinical relevance. While the moderate number of studies does not allow for further differentiation between sub-types of music intervention, the sub-group comparisons indicated promising pathways for future systematic reviews. This meta-analysis is the first systematic and quantitative overview supporting clinically and statistically robust effects of music intervention on agitation in dementia. The analysis provides further arguments for this non-pharmacological approach and highlights needs for future systematic research reviews for the investigation of intervention types.

Introduction

The American Music Therapy Association (AMTA) defines music therapy (MT) as “the clinical and evidence-based use of music interventions to accomplish individualized goals within a therapeutic relationship by a credentialed professional who has completed an approved music therapy program” (American Music Therapy Association, 2006). Other definitions underline MT's role more specifically as a therapeutic medium to address “developmental, adaptive, and rehabilitative goals in the areas of psychosocial, cognitive, and sensorimotor behavior of individuals with disabilities” (Hallam et al., 2009). Music therapy can be applied in a range of possible settings, such as “everyday environments with individuals, groups, families, or communities who seek to optimize their quality of life and improve their physical, social, communicative, emotional, intellectual, and spiritual health and well-being” (World Federation of Music Therapy, 2011). Clinical experiences and research results suggest MT to be an intervention with considerable positive health outcomes (MacDonald, 2013). The National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) guidelines for treating people with dementia who have comorbid agitation recommends a range of sensory stimulation interventions (National Institute for Clinical Excellence, 2006). These include aromatherapy, music intervention, animal-assisted therapy, massage, and multisensory stimulation (National Institute for Clinical Excellence, 2006). According to NICE, a range of health and social care staff and volunteers may deliver the interventions with appropriate training and supervision. Furthermore, the interventions should be tailored to the person's preferences, skills, abilities, and responsiveness of treatment.

For the purpose of this meta-analysis, music intervention was defined as the controlled use of music in a therapeutic setting to accomplish individualized goals within physiological, psychological, and emotional well-being during the treatment of an illness or disease.

Despite anecdotal evidence of positive effects, systematic research in controlled experimental settings aiming to quantify health outcomes following MI is still scarce. The heterogeneity of intervention types, target groups, and clinical context requires a systematic overview before firm conclusions on the specific effects of MI can be made.

One area in which MI has been repeatedly reported to promote positive outcomes is agitation in demented patients. Agitation per se is no diagnosis, but consists of a transdiagnostic group of symptoms that may reflect an underlying disorder and has been defined as “inappropriate verbal, vocal, or motor activity that is not judged by an outside observer to result directly from the needs or confusion of the agitated individual (p. 712)” (Cohen-Mansfield and Billing, 1986). The prevalence rates for agitation in dementia range from 20 to over 80% depending on the definition used and the means of assessment (Sourial et al., 2001). Agitation encompasses a variety of behaviors such as repetitive acts, restlessness, wandering, and aggressive behaviors toward oneself or others (Cohen-Mansfield, 2008). It is considered one of the core features of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) besides others such as anxiety, depression, irritability, hallucinations, delusions, and eating problems (Cerejeira et al., 2012). BPSD and particularly agitation pose an increased burden for both patients and caregivers and are common causes of hospitalization and institutionalization of people with dementia (Schulz and Williamson, 1991). Agitation has detrimental psychosocial consequences for the patient and reduces the probability of positive social interaction and thus cognitive stimulation. Typical treatment in severe depression often includes antidepressants and anxiolytic pharmaceuticals to control emotional symptoms, and antipsychotic drugs to control hallucinations, delusions, and are aimed to also reduce agitation (Caltagirone et al., 2005). Because of an increased risk of medication misuse, increased health care costs (Cerejeira et al., 2012), and a number of adverse side effects like nausea, insomnia, anorexia, gastrointestinal discomfort, and fatigue (Caltagirone et al., 2005), it has been recommended that non-pharmacological treatment is given first priority if the patient shows distressing behavioral symptoms (Azermai et al., 2011). The reason why pharmacological intervention is widely in use to reduce agitation in dementia is most likely due to a current lack of other proven effective treatments (Hansen et al., 2007).

Musical Interventions to Reduce Agitation

Musical interventions to reduce agitation can be administered as different interventions across several settings. Active MI involves the participants actively by means of singing, dancing or instrument playing. In passive MI, the patients listen to live or recorded music without being actively engaged. The music is either prescribed by the therapist without any prior knowledge of the patient's preferences, or it is selected in accordance with the patient's preferences. In individual MI, the patient experiences music alone or together with the therapist, whereas group MI refers to shared music experience between two or more patients.

Numerous studies indicated beneficial effects of MI on reducing agitation in people with dementia (Clark et al., 1998; Remington, 2002; Sung et al., 2006a; Raglio et al., 2008, 2010; Lin et al., 2011; Janata, 2012; Ridder et al., 2013; Sakamoto et al., 2013). One factor contributing to the occurrence and maintenance of agitation lies in impaired capabilities to communicate, such as to express one's emotions or desires verbally (Sung et al., 2006b). It has been argued that MI provides a channel for a more appropriate expression of emotions (Sung et al., 2006a) while other studies did not replicate positive effects of MI on agitation (Sung et al., 2006b, 2012; Raglio et al., 2015). A number of different therapy forms co-exists and have been investigated. Group MI provides those with dementia an opportunity for communication and social interaction which can divert attention away from environmental and emotional cues that can provoke agitation (Sung et al., 2006a, 2012; Raglio et al., 2010; Lin et al., 2011). Active MI act as a powerful stimulus that promotes socialization, involvement with the environment, and awareness (Raglio et al., 2008, 2010; Sakamoto et al., 2013). In addition to this, active MI is shown to reduce stress and apathy, and negative behaviors such as aggressiveness and agitation (Sakamoto et al., 2013) by helping the patients to create meaningful activities (Svansdottir and Snaedal, 2006). Passive music listening is reported to have a beneficial effect on agitated behavior by eliciting repressed feelings (Lin et al., 2011). Music based on the patient's preferences has been argued to have a good effect on agitation in dementia (Sung et al., 2006a,b; Raglio et al., 2008; Sakamoto et al., 2013). Familiar music with pleasing sound can possibly remind the listening patients of their lives before the outbreak of the disease and a life beyond the care facility (Lin et al., 2011). The use of individualized chosen has been argued to provide more arousing and positive emotional memories (El Haj et al., 2012), helping the patients to attain a state of calmness and greater relaxation (Lin et al., 2011), and consequentially alleviates agitated behavior. In a first systematic overview of 10 randomized controlled studies, Vink et al. (2004) investigated the effects of MI in treatment of behavioral, social, cognitive, and emotional problems of elderly people with dementia. The authors concluded that there was a high probability of positive effects on cognitive abilities, quality of life and agitation, but conceded that methodological shortcomings and inconsistencies in the published work available by 2004 reduced the robustness and comparability of the reported findings and limited any conclusions that could be drawn in regards to robust effects of MI intervention (Vink et al., 2004, 2011). Recent additional research with higher methodological standards has been published corroborating the assumption of clinically significant positive outcomes in BPSD, particularly for managing agitation and aggressiveness (e.g., Raglio et al., 2008).

A systematic overview and quantification of the reported effects and analysis of potential publication bias, however, is currently lacking. It remains so far unclear whether systematically controlled intervention studies support the notion of robust and clinically significant reduction of agitation in dementia, and whether different types of intervention differ in regards of their achieved clinical effects. This meta-analysis attempts to provide the first systematic review over controlled intervention studies to suggest a conclusion on the magnitude of effects. In addition, an evaluation of intervention characteristics such as group- vs. personal therapy, prescribed vs. individually chosen music, active vs. passive participation and the degree of dementia has been conducted to shed a light on which type of intervention might be most beneficial.

More precisely, this meta-analysis will (1) investigate whether music therapy is an effective intervention for reducing agitated behaviors in dementia, will (2) compare personalized and group interventions, will (3) assess the effect of music preference by comparing music based on the patient's preference with music without prior consulting of patients or caregivers, and will (4) compare benefits of active vs. passive music intervention.

Methods

Data Collection

The studies included in the meta-analysis used to evaluate the effect of music intervention on agitation in demented people were identified via databases PsycINFO, PsycArticles, Medline/PubMed, CINAHL, and Academic Search Premier. Each database was searched by using the key terms Music AND dementia, Music AND Alzheimer's combined with music therapy OR music intervention AND agitation limited to English language. Because the use of music therapy as intervention for people with dementia is a relatively new field of study, no limitation for the year of publication was applied. All searches were made during October 2015 throughout February 2016. The search engine Google Scholar was also applied to increase the chance of research findings in gray literature (repositories, dissertations, etc.). In addition, a review of the references from previous overview articles was conducted. Figure 1 shows the selection of articles included in the later meta-analysis.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart process of study selection and categorization.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

This meta-analysis included original research fulfilling the following inclusion criteria: (1) Used randomized controlled study as design. (2) The study population consisted exclusively of elderly individuals who were formally diagnosed in accordance with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders or International Classification of Diseases V. In addition, other accepted clinical criteria like Global Deterioration Scale (GDS) used to examine the progression of Alzheimer's disease (Reisberg et al., 1982), and the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) or Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) were accepted as inclusion criteria. (3) The study used a music intervention aiming at reducing agitation. (4) Agitation was assessed by validated and reliable scales. (5) The study provided effect sizes or reported quantitative data sufficient for calculation of effect sizes. (6) Data were available from both pre- and post-intervention time points. (7) The presence of a trained music therapist as required for the MT definition was not required in this meta-analysis.

Exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) Studies with <5 participants. (2) Uncontrolled studies.

Intervention Outcome Measures

The instruments used in the studies to measure agitation include the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (CMAI; Cohen-Mansfield et al., 1989) assessing agitated behaviors and their frequencies, Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI; Cummings et al., 1994), the Behavioral Pathology in Alzheimer's Disease Rating Scale (BEHAVE-AD; Reisberg et al., 1987) and an agitation checklist. The CMAI is administered by trained research staff or caregivers. Interrater reliability (Cronbach's alpha) of the CMAI has been reported between 0.82 and 0.93 across different ethnic populations (Finkel et al., 1992). The NPI is an inventory used in the studies in order to obtain information about the patient's psychopathology, in particular agitation, for the purpose of the included studies. The NPI is based on responses from an informed observer or caregiver. Internal consistency reliability for the NPI is reported with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.88 (Cummings et al., 1994). BEHAVE-AD is based on an interview of an informant who knows the patient. Interrater reliability (Cronbach's alpha) of the BEHAVE-AD has been reported to be 0.96 (Reisberg et al., 1996). The study of Clark et al. used an agitation checklist developed for the purpose of this particular study (Clark et al., 1998). The target behavior included hitting, biting, screaming, crying, abusive language, wandering, spitting, refusals to cooperate, pinching, scratching, kicking, throwing of objects, and grabbing. The authors reported an internal consistency of Cronbach's alpha = 0.90 after documenting their reporting's at the beginning of their study and at approximately 2-week intervals during data collection (Clark et al., 1998).

Statistical Analysis

Effect Size

Excel 2013 and the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software program version 2.0 were used to conduct the meta-analysis. Weighted average Cohen's d was used as overall effect size. For the individual studies, we computed d according to Morris (2008) as d = (MT_PRE − MT_POST) − (MC_PRE − MC_POST)/SDPRE where MT_PRE and MTPOST are the pre and post mean scores for the treatment group, MC_PRE and MC_POST the mean scores for the control group, and SDPRE is the pooled standard deviation for the pre-scores of both groups. Individual studies Significance level was set to alpha = 0.05. According to Cohen's criteria, an effect size of 0.2–0.4 reflects a low effect size, 0.5–0.7 moderate effect size, and >0.8 is considered a large effect size (Cohen, 1992). These criteria should only be applied in the absence of better scientific criteria based on the research literature and are therefore applied here in the context of this new field of research where systematic studies are scarce. The random effects model was used in the data analysis.

Heterogeneity

Methodical differences in design, participants, interventions, or exposures increase the heterogeneity between studies and can partially account for observed differences in the results of included studies and thus limit the interpretational value of summarized findings. To ensure sufficient consistency of combined studies and conclude on the generalizability of the summarized findings, the I2 index of heterogeneity was calculated (Higgins et al., 2003). I2values of 0–25% were interpreted as no heterogeneity, 25–50% as representing low heterogeneity, 50–75% as moderate heterogeneity, and 75–100% as representing high heterogeneity between studies.

Publication Bias

Publication bias reflects the fact that studies supporting the hypothesis are more likely to be published than null results (Easterbrook et al., 1991; Rothstein et al., 2006). A funnel plot and calculation of the Fail-Safe-N (the number of unpublished studies with mean effect of zero necessary to equalize the statistically significant effect in published studies) was completed (Egger et al., 1997).

Results

Study Identification

An initial literature search using the chosen key terms identified 57 articles out of which 45 did not meet the inclusion criteria (see Figure 1). This included lack of quantitative data required for the calculation of effect sizes, inclusion of other diagnoses than dementia, review articles, and discussion papers. The remaining 12 articles were composed of randomized controlled trials (RCT) fulfilling minimum standards and were included in the meta-analysis. Figure 1 illustrates the process of study selection and categorization.

Characteristics of the Studies

Methodological main characteristics of included studies are summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Description of included studies.

Participants

A total of N = 658 participants were involved in the included 12 studies (M = 55, min = 9, max = 51). Five of the studies included only people with Alzheimer's disease; four studies included people with Alzheimer's disease, vascular dementia, mixed type dementia, Lewy body dementia, and frontotemporal dementia. The remaining three studies did not specify type of dementia. The degree of severity ranged from mild to severe.

Intervention

All but one of the studies used participants' preferred music in the interventions. The studies included in the meta-analysis varied in the use of practitioners carrying out interventions. Six of the studies used authorized music therapists. Five of the included studies used researchers or nurses who had completed music therapy courses. Another study used care workers that administered the music, while a trained music therapist analyzed the videotapes after the sessions. Only one study did not specify the type of practitioner used. The interventions took many different forms. Eight studies used a combination of methods, including singing, stretching, and clapping (active music intervention). Six studies used passive intervention therapy as intervention context. The music interventions in the different studies were administered through different forms, including music listening through headphones, CD players or live performances.

Overall Effects of Music Intervention on Agitation

The first research question was whether music intervention is an effective intervention for reducing agitated behaviors in dementia. The effect sizes of music intervention in agitation are presented in Figure 2. The obtained mean effect size of the 12 studies included in this meta-analysis for exploring the effectiveness of music intervention on agitation was d = 0.61 with a 95% confidence interval (CI) of 0.38–0.84. The results indicate that music intervention significantly reduces agitated behaviors in demented people. According to common classification standards, a low-to-moderate heterogeneity was found between the 12 included studies (Q = 20.35; p = 0.001; I2 = 46%; Higgins et al., 2003). The calculation of Rosenthal's “Fail-Safe N” (Rosenthal, 1979) indicates that it would take 149 studies with insignificant findings before the cumulative effect in the analysis would no longer be statistically significant (p > 0.05). Similarly, “Orwin's Fail-Safe N” (Orwin, 1983) suggests that in order to bring the criterion down to a trivial level, which represents an effect value other than zero (in this study defined as 0.05), 120 non-significant studies would be required. Given the total number of studies in this field (Figure 1), it appears unlikely that 149 or 120 studies, respectively that would yield non-significant or negative results, have not reached publication. The positive 95%-CI around the mean treatment effect (d) corroborates this conclusion further.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Forest plot on the effect of music intervention in demented people. The center point reflects the mean effect size between a confidence interval of 95%.

Analysis of publication bias showed a fairly symmetrical funnel plot (Figure 3). Taken together, the calculated overall effect size seems to be rather robust and not to be strongly affected by a publication bias.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Funnel plot illustrating proneness to publication bias for the included studies. Mean effect size is d = 0.61 (CI 0.38, 0.84).)

Subgroup Analysis

Overall effect sizes were separately analyzed for personalized vs. group intervention, individualized vs. prescribed music and active vs. passive intervention (Table 2). Both personalized and group intervention showed comparable substantial effect sizes (ddiff = 0.14). Results of personalized intervention showed larger heterogeneity indicating lower comparability of results.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Subgroup differentiation.

Both individualized and prescribed music showed a medium effect size (ddiff = 0.05). However, because only two studies used prescribed music compared to individualized music with nine studies it is difficult to draw a conclusion from these results that can be generalized to the population.

Active and passive interventions yielded nearly identical effect sizes (ddiff = 0.04). This comparison has to be interpreted with caution as passive MI intervention seem to be nearly too heterogeneous to be analyzed as a category. The category of active MI interventions, however, consisted of a highly homogeneous group of studies.

Discussion

The systematic evaluation of effects of music intervention is a prerequisite for the systematic improvement of these interventional approaches (Vink et al., 2011). This study evaluated the effectiveness of music intervention on agitation in demented people. Results suggest a stable medium positive effect. Effects of controlled studies varied between d = 0.18, 95% CI, −0.23–0.60 (min) and d = 1.11, 95% CI, 0.29–1.94 (max) (mean d = 0.61, 95% CI 0.38–0.84). The effect size will be considered clinically significant by most practitioners and the therapy is furthermore appealing because of its low risk profile and moderate cost. Individual MI showed tendentially higher effects than MI applied in groups, but much larger variations of these effects. This might indicate the particular importance of so far underreported influencing factors contributing to the success of individual MI settings, but can also be related to the larger heterogeneity of included studies, their settings, designs and target groups. The subgroup analysis of individualized and prescribed MI showed both a medium effect. With the two included studies on prescribed music, the results turned out to be highly homogeneous. Individualized MI consisted of a larger group of studies, and showed a small, but yet heterogeneous result. While active and passive MI yielded very similar mean effects, passive interventions showed remarkably higher heterogeneity between studies and consequentially larger variations of effects. Further research is needed to investigate the predictors of successful MI in regards to the particular settings before conclusions on the underlying causes for these differences can be drawn.

Limitations

While the overall effect of MI can be considered to be robust with effect sizes ranging from 0.18 to 1.11, the subgroup analysis suffers from the low number of studies providing sufficient statistical information to be included in this meta-analysis, and from high heterogeneity within the group of personalized MI and passive MI. Furthermore, this meta-analysis did not differentiate between various types of dementia or degrees of severeness. This information is usually not provided and could account for varying effect sizes between studies. Diagnosis of dementia includes a wide range of symptoms, stages of progression, comorbidities and effects of pharmaceutical treatment. The majority of research investigated MI effects in patients with moderate to severe dementia. Patients with severe dementia have reduced abilities of emotional expression (Sung et al., 2006b; Sakamoto et al., 2013). Which type of MI is more or less suitable for different types of dementia and degrees of severeness can't yet be answered due to the relative early stage of systematic research in this field.

Further Research

For further studies evaluating the effectiveness of MI we recommend to differentiate between diagnoses, clinical samples and various degrees of severity, where this is possible due to the limited amount of research in this field. Future studies should also be careful in describing the actual setting of intervention (e.g., group size) so that future reviews and meta-analyses can investigate additional contributing and differentiating factors, conclude on underlying causes of the MI effect and propose modifications to maximize intervention effects.

Agitation is a frequent problem among demented patients increasing the caregivers' burden, impairing the quality of social interaction with other persons, it deteriorates with age (Cerejeira et al., 2012) and can therefore be of particular relevance. However, other aspects of BPSD might be affected as well and deserve additional attention in future research, once a sufficiently large body of original research can be provided.

The included studies also have implications for the time at which music intervention may be most effectively administered. Janata (2012) reported lower agitated behaviors and the frequency of this behavior for the morning observations than for the afternoon observations using the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory. The fact that the frequency of agitated behavior appears to be partially dependent on the time of day should be considered when measuring agitation in dementia and planning musical interventions. Another research question pending further systematic investigation is in how far MI effects persons in contact with the patients which are not subject to the intervention themselves (health care staff and relatives, other patients). A recent study reported that participants in the MI intervention group were less agitated and anxious after the sessions (Sung et al., 2012), and were thus less likely to evoke these emotions in other residents, including control group participants, in the care facility (Sung et al., 2012). While this situation was previously considered a limitation in a particular study, it can be hypothesized that MI facilitates positive emotions and well-being for those with indirect contact with music interventions. These secondary effects should be considered in more future study designs. The experimental group in Raglio et al. (2010) study received three cycles of treatment followed by 1 month of washout period after each cycle. The results from this intervention demonstrated that music intervention is an efficacious intervention even though the treatment was interrupted for 1 month. In the overall literature of music intervention and dementia, there is lacking evidence of long-term effects and its effectiveness of therapy. The Raglio et al. (2010) study provides interesting contributions to the reduction of agitation, indicating that the length of treatment may not matter significantly, as long as the treatment is available and sustained. However, for future research, longer follow-up assessments to determine possible beneficial effects of music intervention are recommended.

This meta-analysis focused on agitation as a major aspect of problematic behavior in dementia. Other outcome measures such as quality of life, depression and cognitive functions might also be of interest but are currently underrepresented or do not yet allow for comparison due to insufficient comparability of methods and designs. Previous studies documented positive effects of MI on depression (Guétin et al., 2009; Janata, 2012; Raglio et al., 2015), anxiety (Sung et al., 2012), cognitive function, and quality of life (Cooke et al., 2010; Raglio et al., 2015). However, more quantitative data is needed in randomized controlled studies for computing more accurate effect sizes in depression, cognition, anxiety, and quality of life. For a review of the effectiveness music intervention has on these outcomes it is recommended to consult the meta-analyses of Vasionyte and Madison (2013), and Ueda et al. (2013). Vasionyte and Madison (2013) did not have any particular inclusion criteria in the study design of their meta-analysis. The results of Vasionyte and Madison's (2013) indicated that music therapy significantly improved cognitive functioning in demented people, reflecting a high effect size. Ueda et al. (2013) included RCTs, controlled clinical trial and controlled trials in their meta-analysis. Their results revealed that music interventions reduced depression to a small extent, and the results of anxiety reflected a moderate effect size.

Conclusion

This meta-analysis of music interventions for demented people showing agitation provides evidence for the effectiveness of music intervention in treatment of agitation in dementia. The analysis validates a non-pharmacological approach in treatment of agitation, a particular detrimental symptom of dementia. The overall medium effect size of this meta-analysis suggests that music intervention can reduce agitation in persons with dementia.

Author Contributions

SP collected the data material and was in charge of drafting the manuscript. PA, RL, MA, and SS wrote parts of the manuscript, proofread and revised, and provided guidance through all stages.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

American Music Therapy Association (2006). Music Therapy and Alzheimer's Disease. Available online at: http://www.musictherapy.org/assets/1/7/MT_Alzheimers_2006.pdf (Accessed Dec 19, 2016).

Azermai, M., Elseviers, M., Petrovic, M., Van Bortel, L., and Stichele, R. V. (2011). Geriatric drug utilisation of psychotropics in Belgian nursing homes. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 26, 12–20. doi: 10.1002/hup.1160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caltagirone, C., Bianchetti, A., Di Luca, M., Mecocci, P., Padovani, A., Pirfo, E., et al. (2005). Guidelines for the treatment of Alzheimer's disease from the Italian Association of Psychogeriatrics. Drugs Aging 22, 1–26. doi: 10.2165/00002512-200522001-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cerejeira, J., Lagarto, L., and Mukaetova-Ladinska, E. B. (2012). Behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. Front. Neurol. 3:73. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Clark, M. E., Lipe, A. W., and Bilbrey, M. (1998). Use of music to decrease aggressive behaviors in people with dementia. J. Gerontol. Nurs. 24, 10–17. doi: 10.3928/0098-9134-19980701-05

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen-Mansfield, J. (2008). Agitated behavior in persons with dementia: the relationship between type of behavior, its frequency, and its disruptiveness. J. Psychiatr. Res. 43, 64–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen-Mansfield, J., and Billing, N. (1986). Agitated behaviors in the elderly: a conceptual review. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 34, 711–721. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1986.tb04302.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen-Mansfield, J., Marx, M., and Rosenthal, A. S. (1989). A description of agitation in a nursing home. J. Gerontol. 44, 77–84. doi: 10.1093/geronj/44.3.M77

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooke, M. L., Moyle, W., Shum, D. H. K., Harrison, S. D., and Murfiled, J. E. (2010). A randomized control trial exploring the effect of music on quality of life and depression in older people with dementia. J. Health Psychol. 15, 765–766. doi: 10.1177/1359105310368188

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummings, J., Mega, M., Gray, K., Rosenberg-Thompson, S., Carusi, D. A., and Gornbein, J. (1994). The neuropsychiatric inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 44, 2308–2314. doi: 10.1212/WNL.44.12.2308

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Easterbrook, P. J., Gopalan, R., Berlin, J. A., and Matthews, D. R. (1991). Publication bias in clinical research. Lancet 337, 867–872. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)90201-Y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., and Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Br. Med. J. 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El Haj, M., Postal, V., and Allain, P. (2012). Music enhances autobiographical memory in mild Alzheimer's disease. Educ. Gerontol. 38, 30–41. doi: 10.1080/03601277.2010.515897

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finkel, S. I., Lyons, J. S., and Anderson, R. L. (1992). Reliability and validity of the Cohen-mansfield agitation inventory in institutionalized elderly. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 7, 487–490. doi: 10.1002/gps.930070706

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guétin, S., Portet, F., Picot, M. C., Pommié, C., Messaoudi, M., Djabelkir, L., et al. (2009). Effect of music therapy on anxiety and depression in patients with Alzheimer's type dementia: randomised, controlled study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 28, 36–46. doi: 10.1159/000229024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallam, S., Cross, I., and Thaut, M. (2009). The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Hansen, R. A., Gartlehner, G., Lohr, K. N., and Kaufer, D. I. (2007). Functional outcomes of drug treatment in Alzheimer's disease. Drugs Aging 24, 155–167. doi: 10.2165/00002512-200724020-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., and Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Br. Med. J. 327, 557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Janata, P. (2012). Effects of widespread and frequent personalized music programming on agitation and depression in assisted living facility residents with Alzheimer-type dementia. Music Med. 4, 8–15. doi: 10.1177/1943862111430509

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Lin, Y., Chu, H., Yang, C. Y., Chen, C. H., Chen, S. G., Chang, H. J., et al. (2011). Effectiveness of group music intervention against agitated behavior in elderly persons with dementia. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 26, 670–678. doi: 10.1002/gps.2580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, R. A. R. (2013). Music, health, and well-being: a review. Int. J. Qual. Stud. Health Well Being 8:20635. doi: 10.3402/qhw.v8i0.20635

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, S. B. (2008). Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs. Organ. Res. Methods 11, 364–386. doi: 10.1177/1094428106291059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Institute for Clinical Excellence (2006). Guidance for Dementia: Supporting People with Dementia and Their Carers in Health and Social Care. Clinical Guideline. National Institute for Clinical Excellence. 42.

Orwin, R. G. (1983). A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis. J. Educ. Stat. 8, 157–159. doi: 10.3102/10769986008002157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Raglio, A., Bellelli, G., Traficante, D., Gianotti, M., Ubezio, M. C., Villani, D., et al. (2008). Efficacy of music therapy in the treatment of behavioral and psychiatric symptoms of dementia. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 22, 158–162. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181630b6f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raglio, A., Bellelli, G., Traficante, D., Gianotti, M., Ubezio, M. C., Villani, D., et al. (2010). Efficacy of music therapy treatment based on cycles of sessions: a randomised controlled trial. Aging Ment. Health 14, 900–904. doi: 10.1080/13607861003713158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Raglio, A., Bellelli, G., Traficante, D., Gianotti, M., Ubezio, M. C., Villani, D., et al. (2015). Effect of active music therapy and individualized listening to music on dementia: a multicenter randomized controlled trial. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 63, 1534–1539. doi: 10.1111/jgs.13558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reisberg, B., Auer, S. R., and Monteiro, I. M. (1996). Behavioral pathology in Alzheimer's disease (BEHAVE-AD) rating scale. Int. Psychogeriatric 8, 301–308.

Google Scholar

Reisberg, B., Borenstein, J., Salob, S. P., Ferris, S. H., Franssen, E. H., and Georgotas, A. (1987). Behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer's disease: phenomenology and treatment. J. Clin. Psychiatry 48, 9–15.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Reisberg, B., Ferris, S. H., de Leon, M. J., and Crook, T. (1982). The global deterioration scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. Am. J. Psychiatry 139, 1136–1139. doi: 10.1176/ajp.139.9.1136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Remington, R. (2002). Calming music and hand massage with agitated elderly. Nurs. Res. 51, 317–325. doi: 10.1097/00006199-200209000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridder, H. M., Stige, B., Qvale, L. G., and Gold, C. (2013). Individual music therapy for agitation in dementia: an exploratory randomized controlled trial. Aging Ment. Health 17, 667–678. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2013.790926

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenthal, R. (1979). The “file drawer problem” and tolerance for null results. Psychol. Bull. 86, 638–641. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothstein, H. R., Sutton, A. J., and Borenstein, M. (eds.). (2006). Publication Bias in Meta-Analysis: Prevention, Assessment and Adjustments. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

*Sakamoto, M., Ando, H., and Tsutou, A. (2013). Comparing the effects of different individualized music interventions for elderly individuals with severe dementia. Int. Psychogeriatr. 25, 775–784. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212002256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, R., and Williamson, G. M. (1991). A two-year longitudinal study of depression among Alzheimer's caregivers. Psychol. Aging 6, 569–578. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.6.4.569

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sourial, R., McCusker, J., Cole, M., and Abrahamowicz, M. (2001). Agitation in demented patients in an acute care hospital: prevalence, disruptiveness, and staff burden. Int. Psychogeriatr. 13, 183–197. doi: 10.1017/S1041610201007578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Sung, H. C., Chang, A. M., and Abbey, J. (2006b). The effects of preferred music on agitation of older people with dementia in Taiwan. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 21, 999–1000. doi: 10.1002/gps.1585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Sung, H. C., Chang, S. M., Lee, W. L., and Lee, M. S. (2006a). The effects of group music with movement intervention on agitated behaviours of institutionalized elders with dementia in Taiwan. Complement. Ther. Med. 14, 113–119. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2006.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Sung, H. C., Lee, W. L., Li, T., and Watson, R. (2012). A group music intervention using percussion instruments with familiar music to reduce anxiety and agitation of institutionalized older adults with dementia. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychol. 27, 621–627. doi: 10.1002/gps.2761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svansdottir, H. B., and Snaedal, J. (2006). Music therapy in moderate and severe dementia of Alzheimer's type: a case–control study. Int. Psychogeriatr. 18, 613–621. doi: 10.1017/S1041610206003206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ueda, T., Suzukamo, Y., Sato, M., and Izumi, S. (2013). Effects of music therapy on behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res. Rev. 12, 628–641. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2013.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasionyte, L., and Madison, G. (2013). Musical intervention for patients with dementia: a meta-analysis. J. Clin. Nurs. 22, 1203–1216. doi: 10.1111/jocn.12166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vink, A. C., Bruinsma, M. S., and Scholten, R. J. P. M. (2004). Music therapy for people with dementia. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 4:CD003477. doi: 10.1002/14651858.cd003477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vink, A. C., Bruinsma, M. S., and Scholten, R. J. P. M. (2011). Music therapy for people ewith dementia (review). Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 9, 1031–1038.

World Federation of Music Therapy (WMFT) (2011). What is Music Therapy? Available online at: http://www.musictherapyworld.net/WFMT/About_WFMT.html

*^References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

Keywords: music intervention, agitation, dementia, meta-analysis, therapy

Citation: Pedersen SKA, Andersen PN, Lugo RG, Andreassen M and Sütterlin S (2017) Effects of Music on Agitation in Dementia: A Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 8:742. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00742

Received: 19 December 2016; Accepted: 24 April 2017;
Published: 16 May 2017.

Edited by:

Michael Noll-Hussong, University of Ulm, Germany

Reviewed by:

Daniel Rossignol, Rossignol Medical Center, United States
Floris Tijmen Van Vugt, McGill University, Canada

Copyright © 2017 Pedersen, Andersen, Lugo, Andreassen and Sütterlin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stefan Sütterlin, stefan.sutterlin@inn.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.