Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 20 July 2017
Sec. Cognition

Longitudinal Assessment of Verbal Learning and Memory in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: Practice Effects and Meaningful Changes

  • Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain

Objectives: To identify learning effects and meaningful changes in amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) at a follow-up assessment.

Method: The Spanish version of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) was administered to a sample of 274 adults of age over 50 years with subjective memory complains (SMC), including single and multiple domain aMCI groups and participants with SMC but without cognitive impairment (SMC group). The Wilcoxon test was used to compare results at baseline and after 18 months in short and long recall, and standardized regression-based (SRB) methods were used to study meaningful changes.

Results: Scores were significantly higher at follow-up for short and long-delayed recall in all groups indicating generalized practice effect. SRB scores indicated a significant decline in recall in a higher proportion of participants with aMCI than in SMC group.

Discussion: Patients with multiple and single domain aMCI benefit from practice in a verbal learning memory test. The SRB approach revealed a higher incidence of meaningful decline in short and long-delay recall and recognition in the aMCI groups than in the SMC group. Specifically, compared to SMC participants, single-domain aMCI individuals declined in a higher proportion in all measures, and multiple-domain aMCI individuals in long delay free recall.

Introduction

The ability to learn and retain new information is one of the most age-sensitive cognitive domains (Kramer et al., 2003; Dixon et al., 2004; Josefsson et al., 2012) and tends to deteriorate more rapidly in patients with amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (aMCI) who subsequently develop Alzheimer Disease (AD; Albert et al., 2011). One of the tools most widely used to assess verbal learning and memory is the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT; Delis et al., 1987), based on word lists learning.

When longitudinal designs are implemented to study the temporal course of cognitive decline, the measurement and analysis of change must be carefully considered. Practice effects have to be taken into account to avoid underestimating decline when measuring instruments are used repeatedly (Rabbit et al., 2004; Salthouse et al., 2004; Knight et al., 2007). The cognitive decline might also be influenced by the time elapsed between assessments, which can also modulate the magnitude of the practice effect. The practice effect is readily observed, mainly when intervals are very short (i.e., 1 week or 1 month), as in studies carried out by Krenk et al. (2012) and Woods et al. (2006). Calamia et al. (2012) noted a negative effect of the length of test-retest interval, indicating that longer retest intervals are associated with lower estimated score gains at retesting.

There is evidence for the universality of the practice effect in verbal learning and memory, and its effects have been observed in healthy older adults (Woods et al., 2006; Machulda et al., 2013)—even in very old adults (Balasubramanian et al., 2012)—and in patients suffering from MCI and from dementia (Calamia et al., 2012; Machulda et al., 2013). In a recent longitudinal study, the Mayo Clinic research group found that cognitively normal participants who developed incident MCI or dementia showed an initial practice effect in memory and that those who developed incident MCI or dementia at visit 3 or later (or at least 30 months following baseline) showed the most notable practice effects, suggesting that even in this group practice effects persist a few years before the onset of MCI/dementia (Machulda et al., 2013). In a meta-analyses, Calamia et al. (2012) showed a similar practice effect in MCI patients and in the non-clinical population, but a less notable effect in AD patients. The improvements in cognitive performance can be linked with implicit memory, which tends to be maintained through normal ageing (Swick and Knight, 1997) and AD (Knopman and Nissen, 1987; Bozoki et al., 2006) or to decrease slightly with increasing age (Howard and Howard, 1998), while declarative memory tends to deteriorate progressively.

In this context, the study of the natural history of the cognitive decline must consider the significance of change over the lifespan. Numerous methods are available for evaluating the significance of change, including the simple Reliable Change Index (RCI) method (RCI; Jacobson and Truax, 1991), the RCI with correction for practice effects (Chelune et al., 1993), a simple regression model that predicts follow-up from baseline scores (Speer and Greenbaum, 1995), and the standardized-regression-based (SRB) methods (McSweeny et al., 1993). The SRB method involve deriving a predicted follow-up score based on initial test performance in a control sample and including the regression scores at base line and other potential predictors (e.g., retest interval, age, education, etc.). With the exception of the simple RCI method, in which the practice effect is not controlled for, the accuracy of prediction of the other models is comparable and overall higher than that of the simple RCI (Temkin et al., 1999; Heaton et al., 2001; Frerichs and Tuokko, 2005).

Some previous studies have measured practice effects using healthy samples or MCI samples, but not MCI subtypes (Dixon et al., 2004; Duff et al., 2007, 2008; Knight et al., 2007; Krenk et al., 2012). Study of practice effects in the CVLT in different subtypes of aMCI may provide clinically useful information about variations in progression of memory impairment (Duff et al., 2008). The present study aimed to identify patterns of change of verbal learning and memory in people with subjective memory complains and with two subtypes of aMCI (single and multiple domain) which represent different degrees of severity along the continuum between normal aging and dementia (Brambati et al., 2009; Han et al., 2012). We also aimed to provide standardized change scores for the CVLT by using the SRB method to identify meaningful change in both subtypes of aMCI patients relative to participants with subjective memory complaints (SMC) but without cognitive impairment (SMC group). As practice effects may be enhanced by short intervals between assessments (Rabbit et al., 2004; Salthouse et al., 2004; Knight et al., 2007) and as practice effects may mask cognitive changes, we aimed to identify the pattern of meaningful change in verbal learning and memory during a period of around 18 months, which is a commonly used interval for follow-up assessments in MCI studies (Machulda et al., 2013).

Methods

Participants

Two hundred seventy-four adults over 50 years old (range 50–87; mean 66.53) who completed the cognitive and neuropsychological assessment at baseline (T1) and at the first follow-up (T2) where selected for this study from the sample of a larger longitudinal study on cognitive decline (Juncos-Rabadán et al., 2012). All participants were recruited from primary care health centers in Spain and referred to us by general practitioners. The inclusion criteria for the study were: (1) the presence of SMC, and (2) no prior diagnosis of MCI or dementia, clinical stroke, traumatic brain injury, motor-sensory defects, alcohol or drug abuse/dependence, or diagnosis of any neurological or psychiatric disease. Participants were classified into two aMCI subtypes, a multiple domain aMCI group (mda-MCI; n = 21) and a single domain aMCI group (sda-MCI; n = 46), and a reference group with no MCI but with Subjective Memory Complaints (SMC group; n = 207). Patients with non-amnestic MCI were excluded. The aMCI participants met the general criteria outlined by the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association (Albert et al., 2011): (a) informant-corroborated memory complaints assessed by the Subjective Memory Complaints Questionnaire (SMCQ; Benedet and Seisdedos, 1996; short version); (b) performance of 1.5 standard deviations (SD) below age norms on the of the Verbal Paired Associates-Immediate and Delayed subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition (Wechsler, 1999, Spanish version); (c) no significant impact on activities of daily living assessed by the Lawton and Brody Index (Lawton and Brody, 1969); and (d) not demented according to the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and the Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association and Diagnostic and Statistica Manual of Mental Disorders-fourth edition criteria.

The patients with mda-MCI also scored 1.5 SDs below age- and education-related norms on at least two cognitive subscales of the Spanish version of the Cambridge Cognitive Examination, CAMCOG-R (López-Pousa, 2003; Pereiro et al., 2015), which assesses deterioration in specific domains such as language, attention-calculation, praxis, perception, and executive functioning (Huppert et al., 1996; Cullum et al., 2000) and detects prodromal AD in mild cognitive impairment (Gallagher et al., 2010). The general cognitive functioning of this group was around 1.5 SDs below age- and education-related norms in the Spanish version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (Lobo et al., 1999). Patients with sda-MCI scored 1.5 SDs below age norms in the memory tests and maintained normal general cognition, as measured by the Mini-Mental State Examination and the CAMCOG-R subtests. Activities of daily living assessed by the Lawton and Brody index (Lawton and Brody, 1969) were intact or minimally affected. SMC group scored higher than the cut-off in memory, general cognitive functioning, and specific cognitive domain tests. All participants of the SMC group performed as cognitively normal adults according norms by age and years of education. All diagnoses were reached by consensus, at a special meeting of the research team and taking into account only the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition scores for evaluating memory. The demographic and neuropsychological profiles of the participants are summarized in Table 1 that includes group comparisons (parametric, ANOVA, or not parametric, Kruskal–Wallis depending on the homogeneity of the variance tested with Levene procedure).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean scores (and standard deviations) for demographic and neuropsychological variables obtained at Time 1 by the different study groups: single domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment (sda-MCI), multiple domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment (mda-MCI), and a group with subjective memory complaints without cognitive impairment (SMC).

After the baseline assessment, the participants were informed that they would be called for a follow-up evaluation after about 18 months. For the purposes of this study, we selected all those participants who completed the Verbal Paired Associates-Immediate and Delayed subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition at baseline, and who completed the Spanish version of the CVLT at baseline and at the follow-up assessment. Reason for not continuing in the study, and consequently not completing follow-up assessments, included motivation, morbidity, physical health, conversion to dementia and mortality (see Facal et al., 2016 for further information about attrition).

Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the study, which met with the approval of the Research Ethics Committee of the Xunta de Galicia (Spain) and was conducted in accordance with the provisions of the Declaration of Helsinki, as revised in Seoul 2008.

Procedure and Materials

We used the Spanish version of the CVLT (Delis et al., 1987; Test de Aprendizaje Verbal de España-Complutense; Benedet and Alejandre, 1998), since the more recent revision of the test (CVLT-II) was not available in Spanish. This test has proven to have adequate reliability (odd pair correlation, 0.94; split-half correlation, 0.82) and validity (factorial structure explains 67% of the variance; Benedet and Alejandre, 1998, pp. 27–31). A list of 16 words (four from each of four semantic groups) was presented orally to the participants, who were required to recall the words immediately, after short and long delays and also with and without semantic cues. A recognition task was then administered. The test included the following measures to reflect learning and recall, also called primary variables: Total Trial 1–5, Short Delay Free Recall (SDFR), Short Delay Cued Recall (SDCR), Long Delay Free Recall (LDFR), and Long Delay Cued Recall (LDCR). It also included other variables that reflect processing of information and other cognitive processes such as Semantic Clustering and Serial Clustering, Region effects, Intrusions, and Perserverations (see Campos-Magdaleno et al., 2014, for a more detailed explanation of the test). We considered only the primary variables for the purposes of this work.

The CVLT was administered to all participants at T1 and T2 after a previously established interval of about 1 year and half (M = 19.21; SD = 5.33; Facal et al., 2014). No parallel version of the CVLT was used due to unavailability in Spanish during assessments.

Statistical Analysis

The first phase of analysis examined the extent of practice effects on each of the CVLT primary variables (raw scores). The skewness parameters revealed the homogeneity of distribution of the variables Total Trial 1–5, Short Delay Free Recall (SDFR) and Long Delay Free Recall (LDFR) and the no-homogeneity of the variables Short Delay Cued Recall (SDCR), and Long Delay Cued Recall (LDCR) for the SMC group. For the sda-MCI and mda-MCI all variables showed homogeneous distribution. Taking into account the non-homogeneity of two variables in the SMC groups and the small size of the two aMCI groups we decided the application of non-parametric analysis. The Wilcoxon signed-rank test (z) was performed to determine which variables changed significantly between T1 and T2, and Spearman's rho (ρ) was used to obtain correlation coefficients for assessing test-retest reliability. The magnitude of changes between assessments was measured with Cohen's d (Fritz et al., 2012).

In a second phase, the regression-based prediction of follow-up test scores was calculated using the SRB approach (McSweeny et al., 1993). This involves deriving a predicted follow-up score based on initial performance by a control sample. We took the SMC group as control or reference sample. The basic strategy of the regression analysis consists of determining the variables that might affect performance at follow-up in the SMC group. The variables entered in the regression were the baseline score in Total Trial 1–5, SDFR and LDFR (the CVLT variables that showed homogeneous distribution), age, education, and test-retest interval. The regression-based predicted scores of follow-up were then calculated according to the equation Yp = β1X1 + β2X2 + β3X3 + β4X4 + C, where Yp is the predicted follow-up score, β is the regression coefficients (slopes) for each of the entered variables, X1 is the observed baseline score, X2 is the age, X3 is the education, X4 is the test-retest interval, and C is a constant (intercept). The equation was developed by considering the SMC group as control population used to develop the change norms.

Once the predicted score was established, the standardized change score for each individual was calculated as follows: z change score = (Y2 – Yp)/S.E.E, where Y2 is the particular score in Tfor one individual, Yp is the predicted follow-score for the SMC group, and S.E.E is the standard error of estimate in the regression equation. On obtaining the z change score, further calculation can provide individual levels of determination of change. Statistically reliable change may be based on change scores that exceeded a z score value of ±1.64 change score units (90% Confidence Interval, CI; McSweeny et al., 1993). Individuals for whom the z change score fell within the 90% CI were classified as cognitively “stable,” for that variable, whereas scores outside the CI were designated as having significantly “declined” or “improved” as appropriate.

Finally, we performed cross-tabs for all the CVLT measures to compare proportion of individuals in the three groups whose performance remained stable, declined or improved, using chi-square analyses to assess global significance, the Cramer's V to assess the intensity of the association and corrected standardized residuals (CSR, Haberman, 1973) to determine the specific associated categories. All analyses were implemented using IBM SPSS 20.

Results

The mean values and standard deviations, skewness scores, and standard errors for the skewness of the CVLT variables at each assessment time, Wilcoxon's z score, Spearman's ρ, size effects and means and standard deviation of the difference T2 – T1 are shown in Table 2, for SMC, sda-MCI and mda-MCI groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Test-retest data for the Spanish version of CVLT in the group with subjective memory complaints but without cognitive impairment (SMC), single-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment group (sda-MCI), and multi-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment group (mda-MCI).

Reliability coefficients for tests T1 and T2 (Spearman's ρ) were significant for all groups and ranged from 0.46 (e.g., LDCR in mda-MCI) to 0.73 (SDFR in mda-MCI).

Practice Effects

As indicated by Wilcoxon's z score, the SMC group obtained significant higher scores at T2 than at T1 for all variables, indicating practice effects. By contrast, both amnestic MCI groups only obtained higher scores at T2 for SDFR, SDCR and LDCR. Neither of the two groups had significant higher scores at T2 than at T1 for Total Trials 1–5 and for Long Delay Free Recall (LDFR). According to the three levels of Cohen's d size effect proposed by Fritz et al. (2012) (0.2 = small; 0.5 = medium; 0.8 = large), Cohen's d for SDFR was large in mda-MCI (0.71), medium in sda-MCI (0.50), and small in SMC group (0.14). For SDCR, the size effect was moderate in mda-MCI (0.47), less than moderate in sda-MCI (0.37), and small in SMC group (0.17). For LDCR, the size effect was moderate in mda-MCI (0.48) and small in sda-MCI (0.29) and SMC group (0.22).

The Confidence Intervals for differences in means, corrected by bootstrapping procedure because the different size of groups, are included in Table 2, and are graphically represented in Figure 1 for the main variables. The CIs for all variables were largest in the mda-MCI group and in the SMC group. In general, the size of CIs followed the order mda-MCI > sda-MCI > SMC, where the variability in the data increases with the CI, and having the mda-MCI group the greater variability.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Confidence intervals (95%) for Means Difference Time 2 and Time 1 in the California Verbal Learning Test primary measures, Short Delay Free Recall (SDFR), Short Delay Cued Recall (SDCR), Long Delay Free Recall (LDFR), and Long Delay Cued Recall (LDCR) and Total Trial 1–5, for the three groups: multiple domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment (mda-MCI), single domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment (sda-MCI), and controls with subjective memory complaints (SMC).

Meaningful Changes

The SRB parameters from the equations predicting follow-up performance in the SMC group are shown in Table 3. Baseline scores were the strongest predictors of follow-up performance for the three CVLT variables, accounting for 46% (Total trial 1–5), 35% (SDFR), and 36% (LDFR) of the statistical variance. Age was included in the regression equations for the three measures, but only accounted for <5% of the statistical variance. Years of education was also included in Total Trial 1–5 but accounted for a very low amount of variance (<2%). The test-retest interval variable only met statistical criteria for inclusion in regression equations for Total Trial 1–5 accounting for <2% of the variance. The inclusion of age in the regression equations predicting follow-up performance explains that the percentages of stable, declined, and improved do not present the typical pattern of 90, 5, and 5% that would correspond if the baseline score was only considered.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regression coefficients and indices of significance for equations predicting follow-up scores for each of the three CVLT primary variables that showed homogeneous distribution in the group with subjective memory complaints but without cognitive impairment.

The cross-table results comparing the meaningful change for the three groups in the three measures are shown in Table 4. There was significant (chi-square) and weak association (Cramer's V < 0.3) between the groups and the categories of change (stable, decline, and improvement) for the measures Total Trial 1–5 and LDFR. There were no significant differences between groups in SDFR. For Total Trial 1–5, the sda-MCI group included a lower proportion of individuals whose performance remained stable (73.9%, CSR = −2.5) and a higher proportion of individuals whose performance declined (17.4%, CSR = 2.9) than in the SMC group (87.9%, CRS = 1.8; and 5.8%, CRS = −1.7, respectively). In SDFR, the only significant difference was in decline, which was greater for the sda-MCI group (37%, CSR = 2.4) than for SMC group (19.8%, CSR = −2.4). In LDFR, the percentage of individuals whose performance declined was also higher in the mda-MCI (38%, CSR = 4.1) and sda-MCI groups (38.1%, CSR = 1.8) than in the SMC group (15.9%, CSR = −4.6). The percentage of individuals whose performance remained stable was lower in sda-MCI (54.3%, CSR = −3.5) than in SMC group (80.7%, CSR = 3.9). No differences between groups were found in the percentage of individuals who improve in any of the measures.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Comparison of the meaningful change for the multiple domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment (mda-MCI), single domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment (sda-MCI), and group with subjective memory complaints but without cognitive impairment (SMC) in the three studied primary measures.

Discussion

The Spanish version of the CVLT was used to measure changes in verbal learning and memory after 1 year and half interval in three different groups of participants, with the aim of identifying variables that might affect performance at follow-up, patterns of change related to practice effects, and the proportion of individuals whose performance improves, declines, or remains stable. The study supports the mid-term test-retest reliability of the CVLT in two samples of patients with aMCI and in a SMC group. Test-retest stability coefficients were generally adequate. These findings were consistent with those of studies in which the CVLT was administered with shorter test-retest intervals to healthy adults (Woods et al., 2006) and to other clinical samples (see Tröster et al., 2007, for 17-month retest interval used with Parkinson Disease patients). The data provide new information regarding the applicability of the CVLT in a follow-up evaluation of memory for clinical and research purposes.

The results show that performance on the CVLT primary measures SDFR, SDCR, and LDCR improved, at a group level, in both types of aMCI patients and in the SMC group over the 18-month interval. However, improvement on Total Trial 1–5 (total number of words recalled on the five trials) and Long Delay Free Recall (total number of words recalled after a long delay and without semantic cues) only was significant in the control group (SMC). These results may indicate the existence of practice effects in some recalling variables for all participants due to repeated measures using the same instruments according to Knight et al. (2007), Rabbit et al. (2004), and Salthouse et al. (2004), and the lack of that practice effect on the most memory-demanding variables for the two amnestic MCI groups. In this regard, our data seem to indicate that the ability to benefit from previous exposure to the same instrument in a year and half interval may be present in both cognitively healthy individuals and in aMCI patients (regardless of the number of domains affected) except for the most memory-demanding variables such as Long Delay Free Recall in which only the control individuals take advantage. These results indicate that implicit memory involved in practice effect is preserved in aMCI patients as in normal aging (Swick and Knight, 1997) whereas explicit memory more involved in Long Delay Free Recall seems to be deteriorate in aMCI patients (Brown et al., 2009). Age, years of education and test-retest interval only accounted for a small amount of the variance (between 2 and 5%) when entered as predictors in the SRB procedure. These results provide evidence to understand the practice effect as a universal phenomenon, which occurs in normal aging, MCI and dementia diagnoses, even in very old ages (Dodge et al., 2011; Balasubramanian et al., 2012; Machulda et al., 2013), independently of the number of cognitive domains affected, and the type of instrument used (Calamia et al., 2012).

Although, the size effect of practice effect for some variables was greater in the aMCI groups than in the SMC group at follow-up, to achieve the same confidence level, aMCI groups need larger range due to bigger variability. The increased variability in the change scores for the aMCI groups, especially for the mda-MCI group, is consistent with the notion of instability of the MCI as a diagnostic and clinical entity (Han et al., 2012; Facal et al., 2014). The CIs for all variables followed the order mda-MCI > sda-MCI > SMC, with the variability in the data increasing with the CI. The mda-MCI group shows the greater variability and, accordingly, the highest proportion of change. In this respect, the data on the mda-MCI group was the most variable (large CIs), which may explain the high mean value of practice effects at follow-up but also the large proportion of participants whose performance in relation to learning variables declined.

Although, practice effects are evident in both aMCI groups, it is not clear whether this increase represents meaningful change. The SRB approach showed that the improved performance occurred in a very low proportion of participants, with no significant differences between the aMCI group and the SMC group. However, the proportion of individuals whose performance declined was higher for the single-domain aMCI group in all the measures and for the multiple-domain in LDFR than for SMC group, which remained the most stable. A higher proportion of participants whose performance on these variables declined over time indicate a higher proportion of participants whose learning capacities are impaired and will become more forgetful over time. A similar trend has already been observed in a previous cross-sectional study carried out by Greenaway et al. (2006), in which MCI participants displayed a lower learning capacity and more rapid forgetting (among other indices) than SMC group, but performed better than individuals with AD. The data do not support a previously reported pattern of impairment starting on immediate recall and subsequently in delayed recall (Bilgel et al., 2014). Our findings show that impairments seem to appear simultaneously in short and delayed recall in both subtypes of aMCI within a period of 1 year and a half.

The RBS was calculated to obtain a predicted follow-up score in the three groups based on initial performance of SMC group and to determine the proportion of individuals in each group whose performance remained stable, declined, or improved. This approach allows other variables to be used as predictors of retest scores and to control for practice effects that can alter the final results (Calamia et al., 2012). In our study, baseline scoring was the best predictive variable for the follow-up scores; age also predicted CVLT primary measures, explaining only <5% of the variance; and years of education and time interval between retest explained only <2% of the variance. The RBS approach allows more reliable follow-up results to be obtained to study the progress of each group, since not all statistically significant effects are clinically meaningful (Peters and Katz, 2015), using relatively short retest intervals that are frequent in neuropsychological assessments.

In conclusion, this study provides evidence that patients with multiple or single domain aMCI benefit from practice when their memory is reassessed at follow-up. However, when the RBS approach is used to control for practice effects, both types of aMCI displayed a high proportion of decline in short and long delay recall, suggesting that memory impairment appears simultaneously in both recall conditions. These results support the idea previously started by Brown et al. (2009) that, whereas explicit memory tends to deteriorate in higher proportion in MCI participants, implicit memory can remain stable in both MCI and in participants with subjective memory complaints without cognitive impairment, favouring the appearance of practice effects due to implicit learning. Some limitations must be indicated. First, sizes of the sample groups are unequal, having a great difference in number of participants between MCI groups and SMC group. However, previous research in MCI and practice effects used similar diagnostic sample sizes (Schrijnemaekers et al., 2007; Brambati et al., 2009) and certain statistical procedures have been adopted to maximize the accuracy of comparisons (non-parametric tests, bootstrap to compute confidence intervals). Second, no alternative versions of the Spanish version of the CVLT were used at follow-up. In this case, examinee would learn the specific words and remember them in consecutive evaluations, and consequently larger practice effects are expected. Even if retest period are quite large, analysis with parallel versions should be used in future research. For the reference group, we have taken the group of patients who attended primary health care centres with memory complaints. Even when they do not presented cognitive impairment, this group cannot be considered a fully healthy group in their cognitive aging process (Jessen et al., 2014). Finally, lack of biomarkers is a limitation according to current research criteria for MCI diagnosis, due to the possibility of having undetected preclinical AD subjects. Replications of these studies are currently being conducted including alternative version of the Spanish test, study of biomarkers and healthy participants without memory complaints. Further longitudinal studies with more than one follow-up assessment are required to confirm the pattern of meaningful changes in learning and memory in aMCI.

Author Contributions

MC, DF, CL, AP, and OJ designed the study, conducted research, analysed data, contributed to the wrote of the manuscript and revised and approved the final version.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was financially supported by the Spanish Directorate General of Scientific and Technical Research (Project PSI2014-55316-C3-1-R) and by the Galician Government (Consellería de Cultura, Educación e Ordenación Universitaria; axudas para a consolidación e Estruturación de unidades de investigación competitivas do Sistema universitario de Galicia; GPC2014/047) through FEDER founds.

References

Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association work groups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balasubramanian, A. B., Kawas, C. H., Peltz, C. B., Brookmeyer, R., and Corrada, M. M. (2012). Alzheimer disease pathology and longitudinal cognitive performance in the oldest-old with no dementia. Neurology 79, 915–921. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318266fc77

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedet, M. J., and Alejandre, M. A. (1998). TAVEC: Test de Aprendizaje Verbal Espa-a-Complutense. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

Benedet, M. J., and Seisdedos, N. (1996). Evaluación Clínica de las Quejas de Memoria en la Vida Cotidiana. Madrid: Editorial Médica Panamericana.

Bilgel, M., An, Y., Lang, A., Prince, J., Ferrucci, L. B., and Resnick, S. M. (2014). Trajectories of Alzheimer disease-related cognitive measures in a longitudinal sample. Alzheimers Dement. 10, 735–742. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.04.520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bozoki, A., Grossman, M., and Smith, E. E. (2006). Can patients with Alzheimer's disease learn a category implicitly? Neuropsychologia 44, 816–827. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brambati, S. M., Belleville, S., Kergoat, M. J., Chayer, C., Gauthier, S., and Joubert, S. (2009). Single- and multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment: two sides of the same coin? Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 28, 541–549. doi: 10.1159/000255240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. M., Robertson, E. M., and Press, D. Z. (2009). Sequence skill acquisition and off-line learning in normal aging. PLoS ONE 19:e6683. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calamia, M., Markon, K., and Tranel, D. (2012). Scoring higher the second time around: meta-analyses of practice effects in neuropsychological assessment. Clin. Neuropsychol. 26, 543–570. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2012.680913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campos-Magdaleno, M., Facal, D., Juncos-Rabadán, O., Bra-a, T., and Pereiro, A. X. (2014). Cluster subtypes of the Spanish version of the California Verbal Learning Test in a sample of adults with subjective memory complaints. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 36, 317–333. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.890698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chelune, G. J., Naugle, R. I., Luders, H., Sedlak, J., and Awad, I. A. (1993). Individual change after epilepsy surgery: practice effects and base-rate information. Neuropsychology 7, 41–52. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.7.1.41

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cullum, S., Huppert, F. A., McGee, M., Dning, T., Ahmed, A., Paykel, E. S. et al. (2000). Decline across different domains of cognitive function in normal ageing: results of a longitudinal population-based study using CAMCOG. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 15, 853–862. doi: 10.1002/1099-1166(200009)15:9<853::AID-GPS211>3.0.CO;2-T

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., and Ober, B. (1987). California Verbal Learning Test. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Dixon, R. A., Wahlin, A., Maitland, S. B., Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., and Bäckman, L. (2004). Episodic memory change in late adulthood: generalizability across samples and performance indices. Mem. Cognit. 32, 768–778. doi: 10.3758/BF03195867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodge, H. H., Wang, C. N., Chang, C. C., and Ganguli, M. (2011). Terminal decline and practice effects in older adults without dementia: the MoVIES project. Neurology 77, 722–730. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31822b0068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duff, K., Beglinger, L. J., Van Der Heiden, S., Moser, D. J., Arndt, S., Schultz, S. K., et al. (2008). Short-term practice effects in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: implications for diagnosis and treatment. Int. Psychogeriatr. 20, 986–999. doi: 10.1017/S1041610208007254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duff, K., Beglinger, L., Schultz, S., Moser, D., McCaffrey, R., Haase, R., et al. (2007). Practice effects in the prediction of long-term cognitive outcome in three patient samples. A novel prognostic index. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 22, 15–24 doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Facal, D., Guàrdia-Olmos, J., and Juncos-Rabadán, O. (2014). Diagnostic transitions in mild cognitive impairment by use of simple Markov models. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 30, 669–676. doi: 10.1002/gps.4197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Facal, D., Juncos-Rabadán, O., Guària-Olmos, J., Pereiro, A. X., and Lojo-Seoane, C. (2016). Characterizing magnitude and Selectivity of attrition in a study of mild cognitive impairment. J. Nutr. Health Aging 20, 722–728. doi: 10.1007/s12603-015-0635-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frerichs, R. J., and Tuokko, H. A. (2005). A comparison of methods for measuring cognitive change in older adults. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 20, 321–333. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., and Richler, J. J. (2012). Effect size estimates: current use, calculation, and interpretation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 2–18. doi: 10.1037/a0024338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, D., Mhaolain, A. N., Coen, R., Walsh, C., Kilroy, D., Belinski, K., et al. (2010). Detecting prodromal Alzheimer's disease in mild cognitive impairment: utility of the CAMCOG and other neuropsychological predictors. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 25, 1280–1287. doi: 10.1002/gps.2480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenaway, M. C., Lacritz, L. H., Binegar, D., Weiner, M. F., Lipton, A., and Cullum, M. C. (2006). Patterns of verbal memory performance in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer disease and normal aging. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 19, 79–84. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000208290.57370.a3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haberman, S. J. (1973). The analysis of residuals in cross-classified tables. Biometrics 29, 205–220. doi: 10.2307/2529686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, J. W., Kim, T. H., Lee, S. B., Park, J. H., Lee, J. J., Huh, Y., et al. (2012). Predictive validity and diagnostic stability of mild cognitive impairment subtypes. Alzheimers Dement. 8, 553–559. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heaton, R. K., Temkin, N., Dikmen, S., Avitable, N., Taylor, M. J., Marcotte, T. D., et al. (2001). Detecting change: a comparison of three neuropsychological methods, using normal and clinical samples. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 16, 75–91. doi: 10.1093/arclin/16.1.75

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, J. H., and Howard, D. V. (1998). Age differences in implicit learning of higher order dependencies in serial patterns. Psychol. Aging 12, 634–656. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.4.634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, F., Jorm, A. F., Brayne, C., Girling, D. M., Barkley, C., Beardsall, L., et al. (1996). Psychometric properties of the CAMCOG and its efficacy in the diagnosis of dementia. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 3, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/13825589608256624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, N. S., and Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: a statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 59, 12–19. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.59.1.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., van Boxtel, M., Breteler, M., Ceccaldi, M., Chételat, G., et al. (2014). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 10, 844–852. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Josefsson, M., de Luna, X., Pudas, S., Nilsson, L. G., and Nyberg. (2012). Genetic and lifestyle predictors of 15-year longitudinal change in episodic memory. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 2197–2388. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juncos-Rabadán, O., Pereiro, A. X., Facal, D., Rodriguez, N., Lojo, C., Caamaño, J. A., et al. (2012). Prevalence and correlates of cognitive impairment in adults with subjective cognitive complaints in primary care centres. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 33, 226–232. doi: 10.1159/000338607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, R. G., McMahon, J., Skeaff, C. M., and Green, T. J. (2007). Reliable Change Index scores for persons over the age of 65 tested on alternate forms of the Rey AVLT. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 22, 513–518. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knopman, D. S., and Nissen, M. J. (1987). Implicit learning in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 37, 784–788. doi: 10.1212/WNL.37.5.784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, J. H., Yaffe, K., Lengenfelder, J., and Delis, D. C. (2003). Age and gender interactions on verbal memory performance. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 9, 97–102. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703910113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krenk, L., Rasmussen, L. S., Siersma, V. D., and Kehlet, H. (2012). Short-term practice effects and variability in cognitive testing in a healthy elderly population. Exp. Gerontol. 47, 432–436. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2012.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawton, M. P., and Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist 9, 179–186. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lobo, A., Saz, P., Marcos, G., Díaz, J. L., de la Camara, C., Ventura, T., et al. (1999). Revalidación y normalización del Mini-Examen Cognoscitivo (primera versión en castellano del Mini- Mental State Examination) en la población general geriátrica. Med. Clin. 112, 767–774.

Google Scholar

López-Pousa, S. (2003). CAMDEX-R: Prueba de Exploración Cambridge Revisada Para la Valoración de los Trastornos Mentales en la Vejez. Adaptación Espa-ola. Madrid: TEA ediciones.

Machulda, M. M., Pankratz, V. S., Christianson, T. J., Ivnik, R. J., Mielke, M., and Roberts, R. O., et al. (2013). Practice effects and longitudinal cognitive change in normal aging vs. incident mild cognitive impairment and dementia in the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Clin. Neuropsychol. 27, 1247–1267. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2013.836567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McSweeny, A. J., Naugle, R. I., Chelune, G. J., and Lüders, H. (1993). “T scores for change”: an illustration of a regression approach to depicting change in clinical neuropsychology. Clin. Neuropsychol. 7, 300–312. doi: 10.1080/13854049308401901

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pereiro, A. X., Ramos-Lema, S., Juncos-Rabadán, O., Facal, D., and Lojo-Seoane, C. (2015). Normative scores of the Cambridge Cognitive Examination-Revised in healthy Spanish population. Psicothema 27, 132–139. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2014.169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, K. R., and Katz, S. (2015). Voices from the field: expert reflections on mild cognitive impairment. Dementia 14, 285–297. doi: 10.1177/1471301214562135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabbit, P., Diggle, P., Holland, F., and McInnes, L. (2004). Practice and drop-out effects during a 17-year longitudinal study of cognitive aging. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 59, 84–97. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.2.P84

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A., Schroeder, D. H., and Ferrer, E. (2004). Estimating retest effects in longitudinal assessments of cognitive functioning in adults between 18 and 60 years of age. Dev. Psychol. 40, 813–822. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schrijnemaekers, A. M. C., de Jager, C. A., Hogervorst, E., and Budge, M. M. (2007). Cases with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's Disease fail to benefit from repeated exposure to episodic memory test as compared with controls. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 28, 438–455. doi: 10.1080/13803390590935462

CrossRef Full Text

Speer, D. C., and Greenbaum, P. E. (1995). Five methods for computing significant individual client change and improvement rates: support for an individual growth curve approach. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 63, 1044–1048. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.63.6.1044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swick, D., and Knight, R. T. (1997). Event-related potentials differentiate the effects of aging on word and nonword repetition in explicit and implicit memory tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 23, 123–142. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.23.1.123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Temkin, N. R., Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., and Dikmen, S. S. (1999). Detecting significant change in neuropsychological test performance: a comparison of models. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 5, 357–369. doi: 10.1017/S1355617799544068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tröster, A. I., Woods, S. P., and Morgan, E. E. (2007). Assessing cognitive change in Parkinson's disease: development of practice effect-corrected reliable change indices. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 22, 711–718. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1999). WMS-III: Wechsler Memory Scale Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Woods, S. P., Delis, D. C., Scott, J. C., Kramer, J. H., and Holdnack, J. A. (2006). The California Verbal Learning Test- second edition: the test-retest reliability, practice effects, and reliable change indices for the standard and alternate forms. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 21, 413–420. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: California verbal learning test, subjective memory complaints, longitudinal design, standardized regression based methods, repeated assessments

Citation: Campos-Magdaleno M, Facal D, Lojo-Seoane C, Pereiro AX and Juncos-Rabadán O (2017) Longitudinal Assessment of Verbal Learning and Memory in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: Practice Effects and Meaningful Changes. Front. Psychol. 8:1231. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01231

Received: 21 April 2017; Accepted: 05 July 2017;
Published: 20 July 2017.

Edited by:

Kathrin Finke, University of Jena, Germany

Reviewed by:

Yang Jiang, University of Kentucky, United States
Sandra Düzel, Max Planck Institute for Human Development (MPG), Germany

Copyright © 2017 Campos-Magdaleno, Facal, Lojo-Seoane, Pereiro and Juncos-Rabadán. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: María Campos-Magdaleno, maria.campos@usc.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.