Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 14 November 2017
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Getting Better Acquainted with Auditory Voice Hallucinations (AVHs): A Need for Clinical and Social Change

\r\nAntonio Iudici*Antonio Iudici1*Elena FaccioElena Faccio1Maria QuaratoMaria Quarato2Jessica NeriJessica Neri1Gianluca Castelnuovo,Gianluca Castelnuovo3,4
  • 1Department of Philosophy, Sociology, Education and Applied Psychology, University of Padova, Padua, Italy
  • 2Interactionist School of Padova, Istituto di Psicologia e Psicoterapia, Padua, Italy
  • 3Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy
  • 4Istituto Auxologico Italiano IRCCS, Psychology Research Laboratory, Ospedale San Giuseppe, Verbania, Italy

Introduction

The phenomenon of hearing voices (AVHs) is very much a subject of current scientific interest, both clinically1 and socially. For a long time, auditory hallucinations—perceiving sounds without external stimuli (David, 2004)—were considered an obvious sign of schizophrenic or psychotic psychopathology (Goodwin et al., 1971; Larøi et al., 2012), but these days such an association is no longer taken for granted. Various recent studies in the areas of psychology, psychiatry, and neuroscience have brought a renewal of interest in AVHs. First of all, the move beyond Kraepelinian logic (van Os, 2009; Fusar-Poli et al., 2014) has led us to see AVHs as a phenomenon in their own right, and not just a characteristic of schizophrenia (Fernyhough, 2004). Furthermore, a number of studies in imaging techniques have allowed us to study the phenomenon live, as it occurs, collecting various new data (Shergill et al., 2000). On the other hand, psychological studies with attempts at modeling, have boosted the idea that AVHs are linked to the linguistic and verbal qualities of the subject, thus reducing the association between voice hallucinations and signs of pathology (Johns and van Os, 2001; Pearson et al., 2001; Stanghellini and Cutting, 2003).

Other researchers have theorized that hearing voices is a different manifestation of self-awareness (Salvini and Bottini, 2011; Salvini and Quarato, 2011).

Even DSM-5 has modified the importance it attaches to hallucinations, in fact although the 4th edition diagnosed “schizophrenia” simply on the basis of the symptom “hallucinations,” in the new edition hallucinations on their own are not considered a sufficient symptom to diagnose the specter of schizophrenia” (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Many of those suffering from this condition are not under treatment and are not diagnosable in psychopathological terms, which asks ever more questions of health professionals (Iudici, 2015), and which brings with it the risk that the phenomenon of hearing voices may be considered pathological because of a lack of understanding of the problem.

One direct implication of this risk concerns non-psychotic and non-schizophrenic hearers of voices who are afraid of being considered mad or disturbed, who very often live in fear for years without talking about it with anyone, although realizing that hearing voices causes no general maladjustment in their lives (Andrew et al., 2008). In the long term this can lead to feelings of alarm in some of them, and when such situations result in a visit to a clinic or a psychiatrist, there are often “suffering and conflicted confessions” about such experiences, especially by people who have never had psychiatric experience (Iudici and Gagliardo Corsi, 2017). These people consequently do not have appropriate information to help them understand their experiences (Faccio et al., 2013). This fact raises further doubts about the direct juxtaposition of auditory hallucinations and diagnoses of mental disturbance, and consequently our interest is in sensitizing clinicians to a broader interpretation of the phenomenon than the traditional view, highlighting the importance of considering more perspectives.

Understanding AVHs: New Research Perspectives

There are various approaches which have recently offered a contribution to the understanding and management of AVHs and, in addition to the traditional psychological methods, we would point to psychological and phenomenological perspectives, cognitive neurobiology, and neuroimaging.

Psychological approaches presuppose that there is some continuity between AVHs and normal experience, and focus attention on the vocal content and on how it is interpreted and managed by people (van Os et al., 2000). The voices heard may take the form of a dialogue, may often be impersonated, or have a specific meaning for the individuals who experience them (David, 2004). They can be associated with psychological suffering, but may be a positive experience, for example when voices serve as a guide and reduce social isolation (Miller et al., 1993; Honig et al., 1998). Romme and Escher (1989) carried out research on 450 voice hearers found by the viewers of a popular German TV show, and a third of these appeared to be sane and have a positive relationship (coping) with their hallucinations. In research into hallucinatory experiences, Bentall (2000, p. 95) concluded that “the finding that a substantial minority of the population experiences frank hallucinations at some point in their lives must be considered very robust.” Many writers have observed that AVHs, in the absence of a diagnosis of mental disturbance, do not interfere with the activities of everyday life (Lawrence et al., 2010; Daalman et al., 2011; Badcock and Chhabra, 2013; Hill and Linden, 2013).

According to Liester (1996), psychotic and non-psychotic hallucinations are perceived in the same way, whereas they actually differ according to the type of personal experience that results from them, as also confirmed by Chadwick and Birchwood (1994). In particular, perceptions and beliefs regarding identity (hearing the voice of someone else distinct from oneself) and the interpersonal disposition (intentionality) toward the voices may appear to be highly relevant aspects (Mawson et al., 2010).

Saavedra et al. (2012) utilize Harre and van Langenhov's (1999) theories on the building of social interactions to understand the relationship between the voices and the subjects who hear them. In particular, the authors consider the characteristics of moral positioning, the conversational story and conversational ability in redefining interactions, within which hallucinatory experiences can be generated. Moreover, we should stress the practical characteristics connected with hearing AVHs as spoken phenomena (Leudar and Thomas, 2000).

Other research (Stanghellini and Cutting, 2003; Seal et al., 2004) has highlighted how it is possible to talk to ourselves in subvocal language. This research considers the possibility that in the formation of certain auditory hallucinations there is an inner subvocal language which subjects see as separate from themselves. The subvocalizations, made possible by the activation of the muscles regulating the articulation of words, occur during normal verbal thought. This activity may be accentuated when voices are heard (Faccio et al., 2016).

Fernyhough (2004) developed a descriptive model of AVHs. He proposed that inner speech, as a product of ontogenetic development, retains the dialogical qualities of socially mediated exchanges. Thus, when a dialogue is internalized, the different perspectives of the reality that are manifested in this dialogue are also internalized. The key to understanding the differences between these two forms of inner speech is in the transformations that accompany the internalization. The voices could be caused by a difficulty in the internalization of the socially mediated outer dialogue (Iudici and Verdecchia, 2015).

Then there is a tradition of phenomenological studies attempting to observe how hearers of voices live with their experience (Longden et al., 2012; McCarthy-Jones et al., 2013), which means getting involved with the subjects themselves, with whom it is possible to describe a journey of understanding the voices. Nayani and David (1996) carried out a phenomenological study on AVHs in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia, observing a model of AVHs increasing in complexity over time, with the addition of extra voices, extended dialogues, and greater intimacy between subjects and voices.

Over the years, neuropsychological studies have defined a number of models to explain AVHs, including misattribution of inner speech, language production, aberrant memory, and early automatic sensory processing errors. Studies of the misattribution of inner speech maintain that the patients have poor monitoring of how thoughts are self-generated because of compromised cognition, meaning that they attribute the thoughts to outside agents (Frith and Done, 1998). Others maintain that this process is due to a neuronal corollary discharge which does not allow the person to recognize their own thoughts (McGuire et al., 1995; de Weijer et al., 2013). Smith et al. (2013) have examined the relationship between linguistic production, voice production and perception, appreciating especially that AVHs are associated with cortical areas linked to linguistic functions. Such studies have in fact opened up the need to examine the functional aspects of this topic rather than the anatomical ones. In the aberrant memory model, the AVHs are due to a failure of recall inhibition which produces an involuntary activation of the memory, which provokes intrusive memories which emerge “out of context”, and a final perception of “otherness” (Jardri et al., 2011). Then, there is the model of early automatic sensory processing errors, in which the AVHs would be activated following impaired auditory processing and pre-attentive operation, which would lead to incorrect interpretation of the voices (Rosenfeld et al., 2011).

As for neuroimaging studies, looking at matters structurally some writers have noted a correlation between the presence of AVHs and certain structural changes in the gray matter volume (GMV) (Upthegrove et al., 2015). Some authors have found white matter (WM) deficits in the frontal and temporal areas associated with AVHs, suggesting that disconnection in the left frontotemporal area can contribute to the physiopathology of AVHs in schizophrenia (Seok et al., 2007). A lower connectivity in the left frontotemporal areas can be associated with AVHs as crucial areas implied in the auditory information processing. The mechanism underlying this functional disconnectivity involves significant WM changes in those regions consisting of a long association fiber tract. Furthermore, to evaluate the integrity of fiber tracts and myelination of neural axons in the WM, a specific index is used, that is the FA (fractional anisotropy) and a reduction of FA, in the frontal part of one of these fiber tracts (SLF) was detected, as well as increases of WM density in the temporal part of one another of these fiber tracts (ILF).

Looking at matters functionally, functional MRI scans (fMRI) of patients with schizophrenia and AVHs have discovered that when the brain is in a resting state—not occupied with direct activity—there is reduced connectivity in the primary and secondary auditory cortex and in the areas of linguistic production (Mechelli et al., 2007). Based on evidence of a resting state, various fMRI studies have recently highlighted raised activity in the default mode network (DMN2) (Vercammen et al., 2010). Mechelli et al. (2007) and Vercammen et al. (2010) highlight different aspects related to the phenomenology and functional dimension of AVHs. They focus attention on the process of appraisal and (mis) attribution to an external source rather than an inner thinking or a patient's own speech with changes and abnormal patterns in the particular regions described. Although, the neuropsychological models and neuroimaging studies have led to a more precise evaluation of AVHs, so far no model seems to exhaust the great complexity of the phenomena found in the various studies (Upthegrove et al., 2016). It does, however, seem increasingly clear that what is needed is a dimensional and functional approach rather than one based on categories (Iudici et al., 2015), showing AVHs in terms of a variable experiential continuum, and not necessarily pathological.

Conclusions and Operational Implications

In the last 20 years the subject of AVHs has aroused lively interest in the scientific community. Apart from the nosographic approach, other perspectives—psychological, phenomenological, and neuropsychological—are contributing to a clearer understanding of the experience of hearing voices. AVHs, with the exception of certain medical conditions, can increasingly be seen as a normal experience for human beings, which takes different forms through subjective and interactive language-building processes (Saavedra et al., 2012). This is a phenomenon which interests and involves large numbers of people, and its sound management is a social necessity. It involves all those people, whether patients or not, successful professionals or ordinary people, who hear voices and, seeing them as a psychiatric symptom, do not talk to anyone about them, fearing that they will be judged. It should be added, though, that according to some studies many of these people are able to deal with them effectively, turning them into an enriching experience (Dillon and Hornstein, 2013; McCarthy-Jones and Longden, 2013).

In the field of mental health, for the healthcare professionals who may come into contact with patients who hear voices, what we have discussed here may turn out to be of vital importance, both to arrive at a better understanding of AVHs and to become proficient at managing them.

Author Contributions

AI has been involved in the Study Conception and Design. EF, JN, and MQ have been involved in data acquisition. EF, AI, and GC have been involved in the analysis and interpretation of data and the drafting of manuscripts. GC and AI have been critical of the critical revision.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

1. ^The phenomenon seems to affect about 13.2% of the general population (Johns et al., 2014).

2. ^The DMN is a network of brain regions which are active when the individual is not concentrating on the outside world and the brain is at wakeful rest (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford, 2012).

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Andrew, E. M., Gray, N. S., and Snowden, R. J. (2008). The relationship between trauma and beliefs about hearing voices: a study of psychiatric and non-psychiatric voice hearers. Psychol. Med. 38, 1409–1417. doi: 10.1017/S003329170700253X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badcock, J. C., and Chhabra, S. (2013). Voices to reckon with: perceptions of voice identity in clinical and non-clinical voice hearers. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:114. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentall, R. P. (2000). “Hallucinatory experiences,” in Varieties of Anomalous Experience, eds E. Cardeña, S. J. Lynn, and S. Krippner (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 85–120. doi: 10.1037/10371-003

CrossRef Full Text

Chadwick, P. D., and Birchwood, M. (1994). The omnipotence of voices. A cognitive approach to auditory hallucinations. Brit. J. Psychiatry 39, 839–844. doi: 10.1192/bjp.164.2.190

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daalman, K., Boks, M. P. M., Diederen, K. M. J., De Weijer, A. D., Blom, J. D., Kahn, R. S., et al. (2011). The same or different? A phenomenological comparison of auditory verbal hallucinations in healthy and psychotic individuals. J. Clin. Psychiatry 72, 320–325. doi: 10.4088/JCP.09m05797yel

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

David, A. S. (2004). The cognitive neuropsychiatry of auditory verbal hallucinations: an overview. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 9, 107–123. doi: 10.1080/13546800344000183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Weijer, A. D., Neggers, S. F., Diederen, K., Mandl, R. C., Kahn, R. S., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., et al. (2013). Aberrations in the arcuate fasciculus are associated with auditory verbal hallucinations in psychotic and in non-psychotic individuals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 626–634. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dillon, J., and Hornstein, G. A. (2013). Hearing voices peer support groups: a powerful alternative for people in distress. Psychol. Soc. Integr. Approach. 5, 286–295. doi: 10.1080/17522439.2013.843020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faccio, E., Nardin, A., and Cipolletta, S. (2016). Becoming ex-obese: narrations about identity changes before and after the experience of the bariatric surgery. J. Clin. Nurs. 25, 1713–1720. doi: 10.1111/jocn.13222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faccio, E., Romaioli, D., Dagani, J., and Cipolletta, S. (2013). Auditory hallucinations as a personal experience: analysis of non-psychiatric voice hearers' narrations. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 20, 761–767. doi: 10.1111/jpm.12008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernyhough, C. (2004). Alien voices and inner dialogue: towards a developmental account of auditory hallucinations. New Ideas Psychol. 22, 49–68. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2004.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, C. D., and Done, D. J. (1998). Towards a neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry 153:437–443.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fusar-Poli, P., Carpenter, W. T., Woods, S., and McGlashan, T. (2014). Attenuated psychosis syndrome: ready for DSM-5.1? Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 10, 155–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodwin, D. W., Alderson, P., and Rosenthal, R. (1971). Clinical significance of hallucinations in psychiatric disorders. A study of 116 hallucinatory patients. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 24, 76–80. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1971.01750070078011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harre, R., and van Langenhov, L. (1999). Positioning Theory: Moral Contexts of Intentional Action. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Hill, K., and Linden, D. E. J. (2013). “Hallucinatory experiences in non-clinical populations”, in The Neuroscience of Hallucinations, eds R. Jardri, A. Cachia, P. Thomas, and D. Pins (New York, NY: Springer), 21–41.

Google Scholar

Honig, A., Romme, M. A. J., Ensink, B. J., Escher, S. D., Escher, S. D., Pennings, M. H., deVries, M. W., et al. (1998). Auditory hallucinations: a comparison between patients and nonpatients. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 186, 646–651. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199810000-00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iudici, A. (2015). Health Promotion in School: Theory, Practice and Clinical Implications. New York, NY: Nova.

Iudici, A., and Gagliardo Corsi, A. (2017). Evaluation in the field of social services for minors: measuring the efficacy of interventions in the Italian Service for Health Protection and Promotion. Eval. Program Plann. 61, 160–168. doi: 10.1016/j.evalprogplan.2016.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iudici, A., Salvini, A., Faccio, E., and Castelnuovo, G. (2015). The clinical assessment in the legal field: an empirical study of bias and limitations in forensic expertise. Front. Psychol. 6:1831. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iudici, A., and Verdecchia, M. (2015). Homophobic labeling in the process of identity construction. Sex. Cult. 19, 737–758. doi: 10.1007/s12119-015-9287-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jardri, R., Pouchet, A., Pins, D., and Thomas, P. (2011). Cortical activations during auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 73–81. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09101522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johns, L. C., Kompus, K., Connell, M., Humpston, C., Lincoln, T. M., Longden, E., et al. (2014). Auditory verbal hallucinations in persons with and without a need for care. Schizophr. Bull. 40, S255–S264. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu005.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johns, L. C., and van Os, J. (2001). The continuity of psychotic experiences in the general population. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 21, 1125–1141. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00103-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larøi, F., Sommer, I. E., Blom, J. D., Fernyhough, C., Ffytche, D. H., Hugdahl, K., et al. (2012). The characteristic features of auditory verbal hallucinations in clinical and nonclinical groups: state-of-the art overview and future directions. Schizophr. Bull. 38, 724–733. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, C., Jones, J., and Cooper, M. (2010). Hearing voices in a non-psychiatric population. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 38, 363–373. doi: 10.1017/S1352465810000172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leudar, I., and Thomas, P. (2000). Voices of Reason, Voices of Insanity. Studies of Verbal Hallucinations. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Liester, M. B. (1996). Inner voices: distinguishing transcendent and pathological characteristics. J. Transpers. Psychol. 28, 1–30.

Google Scholar

Longden, E., Madill, A., and Waterman, M. G. (2012). Dissociation, trauma, and the role of lived experience: toward a new conceptualization of voice hearing. Psychol. Bull. 138, 28–76. doi: 10.1037/a0025995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mawson, A., Cohen, K., and Berry, K. (2010). Reviewing evidence for the cognitive model of auditory hallucinations: the relationship between cognitive voice appraisals and distress during psychosis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 248–258. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy-Jones, S., Krueger, J., Larøi, F., Broome, M., and Fernyhough, C. (2013). Stop, look, listen: the need for philosophical phenomenological perspectives on auditory verbal hallucinations. Front Hum. Neurosci. 7:127. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy-Jones, S., and Longden, E. (2013). The voices others cannot hear. Psychologist 26, 570–575. Available online at: https://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-26/edition-8/voices-others-cannot-hear

Google Scholar

McGuire, P., Silbersweig, D. A., Wright, I., Murray, R. M., David, A. S., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (1995). Abnormal monitoring of inner speech: a physiological basis for auditory hallucinations. Lancet 346, 596–600. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(95)91435-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mechelli, A., Allen, P., Amaro, E., Fu, C. H., Williams, S. C., Brammer, M. J., et al. (2007). Misattribution of speech and impaired connectivity in patients with auditory verbal hallucinations. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 1213–1222. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, L. J., O'Connor, E., and Di Pasquale, T. (1993). Patients' attitudes toward hallucinations. Am. J. Psychiatry 150, 584–588.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nayani, T. H., and David, A. S. (1996). The auditory hallucination: a phenomenological survey. Psychol. Med. 26, 177–190. doi: 10.1017/S003329170003381X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearson, D., Burrow, A., FitzGerald, C., Green, K., Lee, G., and Wise, N. (2001). Auditory hallucinations in normal child populations. Pers. Individ. Dif. 31, 401–407. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00145-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romme, M., and Escher, A. D. M. A. C. (1989). Hearing voices. Schizophr. Bull. 15, 209–216. doi: 10.1093/schbul/15.2.209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenfeld, A. J., Lieberman, J. A., and Jarskog, L. F. (2011). Oxytocin, dopamine, and the amygdala: a neurofunctional model of social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 37, 1077–1087. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saavedra, J., Santamaria, A., Crawford, P., and Lucius-Hoene, G. (2012). Auditory hallucinations as social self-positions: a theoretical discussion from a single-case study. J. Constr. Psychol. 25, 132–153. doi: 10.1080/10720537.2012.651067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salvini, A., and Bottini, R. (2011). Il Nostro Inquilino Segreto: Psicologia e Psicoterapia della Coscienza. Milano: Ponte alle Grazie.

Salvini, A., and Quarato, M. (2011). “Ascoltare voci: sdoppiamenti e dissociazioni nella coscienza di sè”, in “Il Nostro Inquilino Segreto” eds A. Salvini and R. Bottini (Milano: Ponte alle Grazie), 325–361.

Seal, M. L., Aleman, A., and McGuire, P. K. (2004). Compelling imagery, unanticipated speech and deceptive memory: neurocognitive models of auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 9, 43–72. doi: 10.1080/13546800344000156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seok, J.-H., Park, H.-J., Chun, J.-W., Lee, S. K., Cho, H. S., Kwon, J. S., et al. (2007). White matter abnormalities associated with auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia: a combined study of voxel-based analyses of diffusion tensor imaging and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry Res. 156, 93–104. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shergill, S. S., Brammer, M. J., Williams, S. C., Murray, R. M., and McGuire, P. K. (2000). Mapping auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 1033–1038. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.11.1033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, D. M., Grant, B., Fisher, D. J., Borracci, G., Labelle, A., and Knott, V. J. (2013). Auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia correlate with P50 gating. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 1329–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanghellini, G., and Cutting, J. (2003). Auditory verbal hallucinations breaking the silence of inner dialogue. Psychopathology 36, 120–128. doi: 10.1159/000071256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Upthegrove, R., Broome, M. R., Caldwell, K., Ives, J., Oyebode, F., and Wood, S. J. (2016). Understanding auditory verbal hallucinations: a systematic review of current evidence. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 133, 352–367. doi: 10.1111/acps.12531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Upthegrove, R., Chard, C., Jones, L., Gordon-Smith, K., Forty, L., Jones, I., Craddock, N., et al. (2015). Adverse childhood events and psychosis in bipolar affective disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry 206, 191–197. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.152611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Os, J. (2009). ‘Salience syndrome’ replaces ‘schizophrenia’ in DSM-V and ICD-11: psychiatry's evidence-based entry into the 21st century? Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 120, 363–372. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01456.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Os, J., Hansen, M., Bijl, R. V., and Ravelli, A. (2000). Strauss (1969). Revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophr. Res. 45, 11–20. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)90323-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vercammen, A., Knegtering, H., den Boer, J. A., Liemburg, E. J., and Aleman, A. (2010). Auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia are associated with reduced functional connectivity of the temporo-parietal area. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 912–918. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., and Ford, J. M. (2012). Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 8, 49–76. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: auditory verbal hallucination, psychopathology, clinical psychology, social sciences, psychiatry, hallucination experiences, clinical competence

Citation: Iudici A, Faccio E, Quarato M, Neri J and Castelnuovo G (2017) Getting Better Acquainted with Auditory Voice Hallucinations (AVHs): A Need for Clinical and Social Change. Front. Psychol. 8:1978. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01978

Received: 17 August 2017; Accepted: 30 October 2017;
Published: 14 November 2017.

Edited by:

Lorys Castelli, Università degli Studi di Torino, Italy

Reviewed by:

Roberto Bottini, University of Trento, Italy

Copyright © 2017 Iudici, Faccio, Quarato, Neri and Castelnuovo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Antonio Iudici, antonio.iudici@unipd.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.