Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 21 March 2018
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Intra- and Inter-Individual Variability of Executive Functions: Determinant and Modulating Factors in Healthy and Pathological Conditions View all 40 articles

Executive Functions and Prosodic Abilities in Children With High-Functioning Autism

  • 1Center of Linguistics, School of Arts and Humanities, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal
  • 2Centre for Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal

Little is known about the relationship between prosodic abilities and executive function skills. As deficits in executive functions (EFs) and prosodic impairments are characteristics of autism, we examined how EFs are related to prosodic performance in children with high-functioning autism (HFA). Fifteen children with HFA (M = 7.4 years; SD = 1.12), matched to 15 typically developing peers on age, gender, and non-verbal intelligence participated in the study. The Profiling Elements of Prosody in Speech-Communication (PEPS-C) was used to assess prosodic performance. The Children’s Color Trails Test (CCTT-1, CCTT-2, and CCTT Interference Index) was used as an indicator of executive control abilities. Our findings suggest no relation between prosodic abilities and visual search and processing speed (assessed by CCTT-1), but a significant link between prosodic skills and divided attention, working memory/sequencing, set-switching, and inhibition (assessed by CCTT-2 and CCTT Interference Index). These findings may be of clinical relevance since difficulties in EFs and prosodic deficits are characteristic of many neurodevelopmental disorders. Future studies are needed to further investigate the nature of the relationship between impaired prosody and executive (dys)function.

Introduction

There has been a recent interest in the study of the relationship between executive functions (EFs) and communication skills in typical and atypical development (e.g., Bishop and Norbury, 2005; Ellis Weismer et al., 2005; Im-Bolter et al., 2006; Henry et al., 2012; Vugs et al., 2014). In typical development, a link has been suggested between inhibition and lexical and syntactic disambiguation in children and young adults (Khanna and Boland, 2010). Working memory has been associated with auditory and written sentence comprehension in children and adults (e.g., Daneman and Carpenter, 1980; Roberts et al., 2007) and with sentence production in young adults (Slevc, 2011). In atypical development, difficulties in EFs have been observed in populations with communication impairments. For example, children with specific language impairment tend to have lower scores than typically developing (TD) peers on measures that assess EFs, including inhibition (Bishop and Norbury, 2005; Im-Bolter et al., 2006), task-shifting (Marton, 2008), and working memory (Ellis Weismer et al., 2005; Henry et al., 2012; Vugs et al., 2014). Deficits in EFs have also been observed in other disorders that include communication challenges, such as aphasia (Yeung and Law, 2010), traumatic brain injury (e.g., Sainson et al., 2014), and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (e.g., Joseph et al., 2005). Crucially, EFs and language abilities seem to be related, both in comprehension and production, and prior findings suggest a link between communication impairments and deficits in EFs.

In line with evidence suggesting that EFs are closely related to language abilities, several models of language impairment now propose that language performance includes cognitive factors such as processing speed, attention, and EFs in addition to linguistic ability (Montgomery, 2002; Gomes et al., 2007; Leonard et al., 2007; Montgomery and Windsor, 2007; Bishop et al., 2014). Bishop et al. (2014) proposed three models to explain the relationship between EFs and language skills: (a) EFs influence the development of language; (b) children use verbal facilitation to assist them in EFs tasks; and (c) there is no causal relationship between these skills, and it is possible that shared problems in the development of the nervous system could account for the correlations. Gooch et al. (2016) described a further alternative: EFs and language skills may develop in a reciprocal interaction, and the relationship could change over time. In this context, longitudinal studies provided a starting point for the understanding of this relationship. Kuhn et al. (2014) studied the link between children’s early communicative gestures at 15 months, language abilities at 2/3 years, and EFs at 4 years of age, and they found that early language skills predicted later EFs. Exploring the relationship between language and EFs in children at-risk for language learning impairments in the transition from preschool to schooling, Gooch et al. (2016) found a strong concurrent relationship between language and EFs. Therefore, EFs and language performance are related, and theoretically this could also be true for prosodic performance.

Prosody plays an important role in communication disorders, as difficulties with prosodic skills can impact on language abilities in general and dramatically influence daily conversations, social interactions (Shriberg et al., 2001; Paul et al., 2005), and even typical language development (e.g., Cutler and Swinney, 1987; Frota et al., 2016). For example, prosody has been shown to play an important role in lexical and syntactic acquisition (Christophe et al., 2008; Hawthorne and Gerken, 2014; de Carvalho et al., 2016). Indeed, prosody is crucial for the production and comprehension of the organization of speech, manifested by patterns of intonation, rhythm, prominence, and chunking of the speech continuum (Wagner and Watson, 2010). Prosodic features impact not only on “how we say it” but also on “what we say.”

However, very little is known about the relationship between cognitive processes and prosodic abilities, and an important theoretical question is whether prosody is independent of other cognitive aspects such as EFs. Given that deficits in EFs and prosodic impairments are both characteristics of autism, this study investigates how EFs are related to prosodic performance in children with high-functioning autism (HFA), thus contributing to our understanding about the cognitive mechanisms that underlie language development.

Autism Spectrum Disorders

Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are a complex group of neurodevelopmental disorders, evident by early childhood, in which the severity of symptoms ranges from minor to incapacitating impairments. Common manifestations are repetitive or stereotyped interests, mannerisms, and difficulties in social communication (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Regarding intellectual abilities, 44% of children affected with ASD are reported to have an average intellectual ability, 24% have a borderline intelligence quotient (IQ), and 32% have an intellectual disability (Christensen et al., 2016). The children without intellectual disabilities are often referred to as having HFA. Although autism is a disorder characterized by multiple impairments, including deficits in EFs and prosody, research has failed to clearly document the relationship between ASD, language, cognition, and EFs.

Executive Functions in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Impairments in EFs have been considered a central deficit in autism (e.g., Rajendran and Mitchell, 2007), and investigating aspects of EFs in ASD has been an active area of research. Specifically, studies have indicated that children with ASD struggle with tasks requiring working memory, inhibition, and set-shifting abilities (e.g., Ozonoff et al., 1991, 2005; Hughes et al., 1994; Ozonoff and McEvoy, 1994; Ozonoff and Jensen, 1999; Adams and Jarrold, 2012). Additionally, a dysfunction frequently found is the perseveration of behavior, that is, the tendency to continue to perform actions that are no longer appropriate to the context (e.g., Rumsey and Hamburger, 1988; Prior and Hoffman, 1990). Children with ASD have also shown deficits in shifting attention, and in sustained or selective attention (Noterdaeme et al., 2001; Landry and Bryson, 2004). Furthermore, O’Hearn et al. (2008), in a review, reported that impairments in tasks requiring response inhibition, working memory, planning, and attention are also present in adulthood. In fact, impairments in EFs could be a potential explanation for many features of ADS, including difficulties with planning, inhibition, flexibility, and working memory.

Prosodic Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Prosodic impairments appeared amongst the first clinical descriptions of autism (Kanner, 1943; Asperger, 1944), and currently diagnostic tools of ASD include atypical expressive prosody as a feature of ASD (e.g., the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised, ADI-R, Lord et al., 1994; and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, ADOS, Lord et al., 1989).

Prosodic impairments in ASD have been extensively investigated from the viewpoint of perception and production. Deficits in the perception of prosodic features in individuals with ASD have been described, for example, in the comprehension of emphatic stress (Paul et al., 2005), as well as in the perception of pairs of the same auditory stimuli as prosodically different (Peppé et al., 2007). Impairments in expressive prosody in individuals with ASD have been described for rhythm, rate of speech, intonation patterns (e.g., Shriberg et al., 2001; McCann and Peppé, 2003; Paul et al., 2005), and the use of prosody to convey phrase-level prominence (McCann et al., 2007). However, some findings are controversial. For instance, monotone intonation has been reported but so has exaggerated intonation (Kanner, 1943; Baltaxe and Simmons, 1985; Sharda et al., 2010; Bonneh et al., 2011; DePape et al., 2012; Filipe et al., 2014), and slow syllabic speech has been described together with fast articulation rate (Baron-Cohen and Staunton, 1994; for a review, see McCann and Peppé, 2003).

In sum, results on prosody in ASD are mixed, with no agreement between studies. So far, no convincing explanation for these discrepant findings has been put forward. This atypical variation might be explained by methodological problems related to the assessment of prosody, poor diagnostic data, small sample sizes, and lack of appropriate comparison groups (e.g., McCann and Peppé, 2003; Diehl et al., 2009), but also by the multiplicity and heterogeneity of symptoms in ASD (Shriberg et al., 2001; Rice et al., 2005). Therefore, there is a current need for research in this field that takes in account the link between symptoms in ASD, such as cognitive abilities and prosodic skills.

Present Study

This study examines EFs and prosody in children with HFA and TD peers. To the best of our knowledge, no study has yet analyzed the relation between EFs and prosodic skills, although evidence suggests that EFs are closely related to language abilities and that these are crucial foundations for development and learning. Since deficits in EFs and prosodic impairments may be a common feature of many disorders, including neurodevelopmental disorders such as ASD, we examined EFs performance and prosodic performance in HFA to determine whether prosodic abilities are associated with EFs, and if so to what extent and with what particular functions. Furthermore, we wanted to investigate if prosodic abilities are mediating differences in EFs performance, or if the reverse pattern was found. This specific clinical population offers methodological advantages because it separates out the confounding cognitive issues seen in other atypical populations. Specifically, the analyses aim to address the following research questions: (a) Does atypical development (i.e., HFA) affect performance on tests that assess prosodic skills and EFs?; (b) Do prosodic skills correlate with EFs measures in the HFA group?; and (c) Do prosodic skills mediate the differences in EFs between the HFA group and the TD group, or does the reverse pattern hold?

Materials and Methods

Participants

Fifteen children (3 girls, 12 boys) with HFA (6–9 years; M = 7.40, SD = 1.12), who met the DSM-5 criteria for Autism (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), participated in the study. A team of child-psychiatrists and psychologists made the diagnosis of ASD. The materials used in the diagnostic procedure were the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Lord et al., 1994) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 1989). Participants characteristics are shown in Table 1. All HFA participants were required to have an IQ of 80 or higher (assessed with Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III; Wechsler, 1991), to control for poor performance on the prosodic tasks not being a general consequence of cognitive impairments. Exclusion criteria were obsessive–compulsive disorders, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and learning disorders, according to DSM-5. The group with HFA was matched to a TD group on age (M = 7.53, SD = 0.99), gender, and non-verbal intelligence (HFA: M = 25.33, SD = 5.10; TD: M = 24, SD = 4.22; assessed with Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices, Raven, 1995; Portuguese version, Simões, 2000). The groups were significantly different in general language level (HFA: M = 83.46, SD = 17.22; TD: M = 96.89, SD = 4.97; assessed with Griffiths Mental Development Scales 2–8 years – Sub-scale Language, GMDS; Luiz et al., 2007), but the difference between groups for receptive vocabulary was non-significant (HFA: M = 120.07, SD = 34.42; TD: M = 142.07, SD = 31.51; assessed with Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, PPVT; Dunn and Dunn, 2007; Vicente et al., 2011, unpublished). All participants were native speakers of European Portuguese, born and raised in monolingual homes in the North of Portugal, with no visual or hearing problems.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Mean (M), standard deviation (SD) and range for age, non-verbal intelligence, language, and vocabulary of the participants in the high-functioning autism (HFA) and typically developing (TD) groups.

Material

Prosody

Participants were evaluated with the European Portuguese version of the Profiling Elements of Prosody in Speech-Communication (PEPS-C; original version: Peppé and McCann, 2003; Portuguese version: Filipe et al., 2017). This test assesses prosodic skills through twelve subtests: six of the subtests address receptive abilities and the other six address expressive abilities. Each subtest comprises 2 example, 2 training, and 16 experimental items. The following is a description of each subtest.

(1) Short-Item Discrimination: assesses the ability to perceive intonation in short-utterances of 1–3 syllables through same/different trials. Two sounds are presented, and the participant indicates whether the sounds are the same or different by clicking on either a symbol for ‘same’ (two red circles) or one for ‘different’ (red circle and green square).

(2) Short-Item Imitation: assesses the ability to imitate intonation in short-utterances of 1–3 syllables. The participant imitates different types of intonation patterns.

(3) Long-Item Discrimination: assesses the ability to perceive prosodic differences in utterances of 3–6 words. The task is the same as Short-Item Discrimination, but with longer stimuli.

(4) Long-Item Imitation: assesses the ability to imitate prosodic differences in utterances of 3–6 words. The participant imitates different types of intonation patterns.

(5) Affect Reception: assesses the ability to understand liking or disliking intonation. A sound stimulus with a liking or disliking intonation is presented simultaneously with a picture of the stimulus. Then two images showing a happy and a sad face appear, and the participant selects the image corresponding to the intonation pattern heard (i.e., happy face for liking intonation or sad face for disliking intonation).

(6) Affect Expression: assesses the ability to produce liking or disliking intonation. The participant produces liking or disliking intonation, and shows what he/she wants to convey with the utterance produced by pointing to the sad or happy face.

(7) Turn-end Reception: assesses the ability to understand question versus statement intonation. The participant hears a declarative or interrogative pattern and identifies the pattern selecting one of two pictures (i.e., the participant chooses the picture of a child offering a food item, when hearing a question; or chooses the picture of a child reading a book showing the object mentioned in the utterance, when hearing a declarative).

(8) Turn-end Expression: assesses the ability to produce question versus statement intonation. One picture of food offered or read out appears on the screen, and the participant says the item with suitable intonation (i.e., interrogative or declarative pattern).

(9) Chunking Reception: assesses the ability to comprehend syntactically ambiguous phrases disambiguated by prosody. The participant hears an auditory stimulus that may correspond to two or three items (e.g., Fish-Fingers and Fruit vs. Fish, Fingers and Fruit). Then, he/she chooses if the utterance heard matches a picture with two or three items.

(10) Chunking Expression: assesses the ability to produce utterances disambiguated by prosody. Pictures with two or three items (e.g., Fish-Fingers and Fruit vs. Fish, Fingers and Fruit) are presented and the participant describes what he/she is seeing.

(11) Focus Reception: assesses the ability to identify focus. The participant sees two colors on the screen and hears an utterance with focus (i.e., main stress) in one color (e.g., Blue and BLACK socks). Then, he/she points to the color that was focused (e.g., Black);

(12) Focus Expression: assesses the ability to produce focus. The participant sees a picture and hears a sentence that does not match the picture (e.g., The black cow has the ball). Then, he/she corrects the speaker producing the matching sentence (e.g., The RED cow has the ball).

Different reasons led us to prefer the PEPS-C test to other ways of testing prosody: (1) it is a comprehensive prosodic test already used with children with autism; (2) to our knowledge, it is the only prosodic test available for European Portuguese that assess both receptive and expressive prosodic abilities; (3) it does not require specialized transcription skills; and (4) responses are the same for all participants.

Executive Functions

Participants were evaluated with the Children’s Color Trails Test (CCTT, Llorente et al., 2003). The CCTT consists of two parts: CCTT-1 and CCTT-2. The CCTT-1 measures visual tracking, processing speed, and graphomotor skills. The CCTT-2 is a more complex task that adds divided attention, set-switching, inhibition, and working memory/sequencing. In both parts, participants connect circled numbers (1–15) with a pencil in ascending order, but in CCTT-2 the numbers alternate in color (pink and yellow). Moreover, the CCTT allows the computation of an Interference Index that measures the added task requirements of CCTT-2 using Time raw scores (raw scores completion time in seconds) through the following formula: (CCTT-2 Time raw score – CCTT-1 Time raw score)/Time CCTT-1 Time raw score.

The CCTT test was chosen to test EFs for the following reasons: (1) it reduces the impact of linguistic components including administration guidelines, because visual instructions allow administration without the linguistic component; and (2) it overcomes limitations of an older similar test, the Children’s Trail Making Test (Reitan, 1971), which uses a combination of the English alphabet with colors, that might exclude children with language or learning disabilities.

Procedure

Informed consent was obtained from participants’ parents/caregivers, who had the opportunity to ask for further information about the study. Each child was assessed individually in a quiet room with adequate lightning conditions, at their school, in their home, or at the University of Porto.

The assessment was performed in two to three sessions completed within a month and lasting approximately 45 min each. Administration order was the same for all the participants: CCTT-1, CCTT-2, and PEPS-C (Short-Item, Long Item, Turn-End, Affect, Chunking, and Focus). In PEPS-C, half of the participants started with the receptive tasks and the other half with the expressive tasks.

Results

For the PEPS-C tasks, each participant’s answer was scored as correct (with 1 point) or incorrect (with 0 points). PEPS-C allows the computation of a score for each subtest (maximum = 16), and the computation of a total score that corresponds to the sum of all subtests (maximum = 192). For the CCTT test, scores involved the completion test time in seconds of CCTT-1 and CCTT-2. Additionally, the CCTT Interference Index was computed to measure the added task requirements of CCTT-2.

HFA and Typically Developing Group Comparisons

To examine performance differences between the HFA and TD groups on the PEPS-C and the CCTT, a comparative analysis was conducted. The results were analyzed separately for each PEPS-C task (Short-Item Discrimination, Short-Item Imitation, Long-Item Discrimination, Long-Item Imitation, Turn-End Reception, Turn-End Expression, Affect Reception, Affect Expression, Chunking Reception, Chunking Expression, Focus Reception, and Focus Expression) and for each CCTT component (CCTT-1, CCTT-2, and CCTT Interference Index; see Table 2 for details).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Scores in PEPS-C tasks and CCTT components in the high-functioning autism (HFA) and typically developing (TD) groups.

In the PEPS-C, the difference between groups on the overall mean score was significant: F(1,28) = 5.214, p = 0.030; η2 = 0.157. In all the PEPS-C tasks, HFA children showed lower scores, however the differences were significant only for Short-Item Discrimination [F(1,28) = 4.244, p = 0.049; η2 = 0.132], Short-Item Imitation [F(1,28) = 10.975, p = 0.003; η2 = 0.282), Turn-End Reception (F(1,28) = 4.847, p = 0.036; η2 = 0.148), Turn-End Expression (F(1,28) = 4.959, p = 0.034; η2 = 0.150), and Affect Expression (F(1,28) = 6.322, p = 0.018; η2 = 0.184).

In the CCTT, no difference between groups was found for the time required to complete CCTT-1 or CCTT-2 (F(1,28) > 1; F(1,28) = 2.503, p = 0.125; respectively). However, a significant difference between groups for the CCTT Interference Index was found (F(1,28) = 6.710, p = 0.015; η2 = 0.193; see Table 2).

Correlations Between the EFs Test and the Prosodic Test

In order to analyze the relation between possible prosodic impairments and other basic deficits, we computed Pearson correlations between variables. We used the overall mean score of the PEPS-C and the scores in the different components of the CCTT. For both groups together (i.e., HFA and TD children), we found no correlation between PEPS-C and CCTT-1 (see Figure 1), but moderate correlations were found between the PEPS-C and CCTT-2 (Pearson’s r = 0.50, p < 0.001; see Figure 2), and between the PEPS-C and the CCTT Interference Index (Pearson’s r = 0.48, p < 0.001; see Figure 3). Additionally, correlations between PEPS-C individual tasks and CCTT components were also calculated, with receptive tasks being more correlated with CCTT components than expressive tasks (the exception is Affect Expression), and CCTT-2 and CCTT Interference Index generally showing stronger correlations with PEPS-C tasks (see Table 3 for details). However, when the groups were considered separately, the correlations lost statistical significance.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Scatter plot displaying the correlation between PEPS-C and CCTT-1.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Scatter plot displaying the correlation between PEPS-C and CCTT-2.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Scatter plot displaying the correlation between PEPS-C and CCTT Interference Index (CCTT-IF).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Correlations between PEPS-C tasks, CCTT-1, CCTT-2, and CCTT interference index.

Mediation Analysis

To explore the possible link between EFs (assessed by the CCTT Interference Index) and prosodic impairments in HFA, and to further analyze the group effect, we used a mediation analysis following Baron and Kenny (1986) in the assumption that the effect of an independent variable on a dependent variable is mediated by a mediating variable. First, we examined the hypothesis that prosodic abilities mediate the differences in EFs between the HFA group and the TD group. This hypothesis would be supported if the effect of Prosody (i.e., the mediator) on EFs (i.e., the dependent variable) is greater than the effect of Group (i.e., the independent variable) on EFs, and the effect of Group on EFs is significantly reduced or absent after controlling for prosody. A series of regression analyses were thus conducted to assess (see Figure 4 for details): (a) the direct effect of Group on Prosody; (b) the direct effect of Prosody on EFs; (c) and the direct effect of Group on EFs.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Illustration of mediation analysis (path a = relation between Group and Prosody; path b = relation between Prosody and Executive Functions; path c’ = absence of remaining relation between Group and Executive Functions once Prosody has been added as a mediating factor.

The direct effect of Group on Prosody (path a in Figure 4) showed an adjusted R2 of.127 (β = -0.39; t = -2,28, p = 0.03); the direct effect of Prosody on EFs (path b in Figure 4) showed an adjusted R2 of.229 (β = 0.48; t = 2,87, p = 0.008); and the direct effect of Group on EFs (path c in Figure 4) showed an adjusted R2 of.165 (β = 0.44; t = 2,59, p = 0.015). All models were significant. However, the effect of Prosody on EFs was larger than the effect of Group on EFs, and the effect of Group on EFs after controlling for Prosody became not significant (path c’ in Figure 4; β = 0.30; t = 1,70, p = 0.101).

Secondly, to analyze the direction of this effect, the reverse regression was performed exploring the effect of Group on Prosody after controlling for EFs. Results showed that Group also became not significant after controlling for EFs (β = 0.23; t = -1,26, p = 0.218). This suggests that Prosody is mediating differences in EFs between groups, but the reverse pattern also holds.

Discussion

The relation between EFs and prosody is of interest to researchers and clinicians. The present study extends research on prosodic skills and EFs in autism by investigating these abilities in children with HFA compared to TD peers, and examining the relations between these abilities. Fifteen children with HFA were matched to 15 TD peers on chronological age, gender, and non-verbal intelligence. The PEPS-C was used to assess prosodic performance and the CCTT as an indicator of non-verbal executive control abilities. The results of the present study point to three main findings.

First, HFA children scored significantly lower on the PEPS-C than TD children, pointing to impaired prosodic skills. Lower performance on EFs was also found for HFA children: for CCTT-1 there was no difference between groups; for CCTT-2 the HFA scored worse than TD, although this difference was non-significant; for CCTT Interference Index the difference between groups was significant. These findings show that atypical development affects both prosody and EFs. These results are consistent with findings from other studies reporting that children with HFA performed significantly less well than controls in prosodic tasks (e.g., Rutherford et al., 2002; Peppé et al., 2007) and in EFs tests (e.g., Rajendran and Mitchell, 2007).

Second, the examination of the relation between cognitive processes and prosodic performance in the clinical group showed no correlation between PEPS-C and CCTT-1, but moderate correlations between PEPS-C and CCTT-2, and between PEPS-C and the CCTT Interference Index. Our findings thus suggest no relation between prosodic abilities and visual search and processing speed (assessed by CCTT-1), but a significant association between prosodic deficits and divided attention, working memory/sequencing, set-switching, and inhibition (assessed by CCTT-2 and CCTT Interference Index). Prior research involving children with atypical development also found that deficits in aspects of communication and EFs are associated (e.g., Bishop and Norbury, 2005; Ellis Weismer et al., 2005; Im-Bolter et al., 2006; Henry et al., 2012; Vugs et al., 2014). The current study extends these findings to prosodic abilities and EFs skills.

Third, the results from the mediation analysis showed that the effect of Prosody on EFs was greater than the effect of Group (HFA vs. TD) on EFs, and the effect of Group on EFs after controlling for Prosody became non-significant, thus confirming the hypothesis that prosody influences EFs. The reverse pattern was also found, however, showing that EFs also affect prosodic skills. These results highlight the important (bidirectional) link between EFs skills and prosodic abilities.

Although several studies have described expressive and receptive prosodic impairments in ASD, no consensus has emerged on the characterization of the prosodic profile of this clinical population. From earlier research it is also not clear whether impaired prosody is related to specific cognitive profiles. It is unknown whether deficits in EFs lead to poor communication or whether other cognitive aspects are also at play, influencing the development of EFs and communication (e.g., Bishop et al., 2014). The present study, by focusing on children with HFA, sheds some light on the link between prosodic abilities and EFs, while controlling for the confounding cognitive difficulties related to intellectual disabilities that usually characterize atypical populations. The finding of an association between prosodic impairments (and therefore communication deficits) and EFs in the current study thus presents an important contribution to this research field. Such association is evident not only in the fact that prosodic abilities and EFs are related, but also in the mediating role of prosodic abilities in EFs performance, and vice-versa, with our results pointing to poorer prosodic abilities leading to poorer EFs, and poorer EFs influencing poorer prosodic abilities. Even though our findings did not provide a clear answer about the direction of the relation between EFs and prosodic abilities, as they suggest that the influence is bidirectional, this strong influence raises the important question that shared genetic mechanisms could be involved in the development of both abilities. Bishop et al. (2014) suggest that delayed development of frontal lobes may impact on brain regions that are important for EFs and language processing. Both EF and prosodic abilities emerge early in development, but continue to develop until later ages, with adult-level performance on many tests of EF and prosodic skills being reached at puberty, and performance on many measures continuing to change into adulthood (e.g., Anderson, 2002; Peppé and McCann, 2003; Wells et al., 2004; Best et al., 2009; Filipe et al., 2017). Therefore, the comorbidity between difficulties in EF and prosodic impairments could be a consequence of shared genetic mechanisms.

Childhood communication disorders are associated with different neuropsychological problems. The most commonly associated neuropsychological deficits are problems involving attention and EFs that are usually a common denominator in the different clinical pictures of language disorders. Although the linguistic signs of these disorders are fairly well understood, the associated neuropsychological signs have yet not been studied. It is hoped that the present study is a first step in this direction. As EFs play an important role in the cognitive control of behavior, clinicians, such as speech-language pathologists, should be aware of the relation between cognitive behavior and communicative impairments. Our findings suggest that clinicians responsible for the evaluation of patients with a wide variety of cognitive disorders and language impairments should test both language and EFs in their assessments.

This study has a number of limitations that should be carefully considered. One limitation is the use of the PEPS-C as the only measure of prosodic abilities. The PEPS-C involves the explicit use of prosody, and this makes the tasks easier for children with autism because these individuals tend to not attend to socially relevant information, but might be able to process information when their attention is navigated toward it (Senju, 2012). Future studies should provide another kind of measures for prosodic skills, such as acoustic and phonological analysis for expressive skills and online perception tasks for receptive skills, in order to draw a more comprehensive and accurate view of prosodic deficits in autism. In addition, although the test for EFs was carefully chosen for its non-verbal demands, using the CCTT as the only measure of EFs is certainly a limitation. The fact that the mediation analysis relies only on the CCTT Interference Index is yet another limitation. Future studies should measure other components of EFs to better characterize this multidimensional construct.

Future research should also explore the relation between EFs and prosodic abilities with larger sample sizes and more robust statistical analyses to verify if the present pattern of findings can be replicated. An interesting approach to use in future research is the latent variables approach to capture the link between EFs skills and language domains. Furthermore, some studies have shown that methylphenidate, which produces effects in alertness, combats fatigue, and improves attention, also improves language processing in children (Westby and Watson, 2004; McInnes et al., 2007). Thus further evidence for the possible causal relation between EFs and language domains could be provided by studies addressing whether improving EFs also improves prosodic/language/communicative performance.

Conclusion

The field of communication impairments and EFs promises to continue as an important area of research concerning the challenging problems of autism. The present study provides important and exciting new directions in the research on prosodic and EFs skills in autism, and may be of considerable interest for clinical practice since EFs and prosodic impairments are characteristic of many neurodevelopmental disorders.

Author Contributions

MF, SF, and SV contributed to the design of the work. MF prepared the first draft of the manuscript. SF and SV revised it critically for important intellectual content. Final version of the manuscript was approved by all authors.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of ‘European Union Agency for Fundamental Rights’ with written informed consent from all subjects, following Portuguese regulations. All caregivers gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by schools’ boards because there is no review board at the University of Porto or Lisbon when this project was conducted. Participants or caregivers of participants were selected via notices in schools and were informed about: (a) The purpose of the research, expected duration, and procedures; (b) Participants’ rights to decline to participate and to withdraw from the research once it has started, as well as the anticipated consequences of doing so; (c) Factors that may influence their willingness to participate, such as potential discomfort or adverse effects; (d) Any prospective research benefits; (e) Limits of confidentiality, such as data coding, disposal, sharing and archiving.

Funding

This research was supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (SFRH/BD/64166/2009, BPD/100696/2014, PEst-OE/LIN/UI0214/2013, and UID/PSI/000050/2013).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adams, N. C., and Jarrold, C. (2012). Inhibition in autism: children with autism have difficulty inhibiting irrelevant distractors but not prepotent responses. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 1052–1063. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1345-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Arlington, TX: American Psychiatric Publishing. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.744053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 8, 71–82. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asperger, H. (1944). Die ‘autistischen psychopathen’ im kindesalter. Arch. Psychiatrie Nerv. Enkrankheiten 117, 76–136. doi: 10.1007/BF01837709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltaxe, C., and Simmons, J. (1985). “Prosodic development in normal and autistic children,” in Communication Problems in Autism, eds E. Schopler and G. B. Mesibov (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 95–125.

Google Scholar

Baron, R. M., and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., and Staunton, R. (1994). Do children with autism acquire the phonology of their peers? An examination of group identification through the window of bilingualism. First Lang. 14, 241–248. doi: 10.1177/014272379401404216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J., Miller, P., and Jones, L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: changes and correlates. Dev. Rev. 29, 180–200. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V., Nation, K., and Patterson, K. (2014). When words fail us: insights into language processing from developmental and acquired disorders. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 369:20120403. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. M., and Norbury, C. F. (2005). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology: II. Response inhibition. Autism 9, 29–43. doi: 10.1177/1362361305049028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonneh, Y. S., Levanon, Y., Dean-Pardo, O., Lossos, L., and Adini, Y. (2011). Abnormal speech spectrum and increased pitch variability in young autistic children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:237. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christensen, D. L., Bilder, D. A., Zahorodny, W., Pettygrove, S., Durkin, M. S., Fitzgerald, R. T., et al. (2016). Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among 4-year-old children in the autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 37, 1–8. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christophe, A., Millotte, S., Bernal, S., and Lidz, J. (2008). Bootstrapping lexical and syntactic acquisition. Lang. Speech 51, 61–75. doi: 10.1177/00238309080510010501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cutler, A., and Swinney, D. A. (1987). Prosody and the development of comprehension. J. Child Lang. 14, 145–167. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900012782

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daneman, M., and Carpenter, P. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 19, 450–466. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Carvalho, A., Lidz, J., Tieu, L., Bleam, T., and Christophe, A. (2016). English-speaking preschoolers can use phrasal prosody for syntactic parsing. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 139:EL216. doi: 10.1121/1.4954385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DePape, A. M. R., Chen, A., Hall, G. B. C., and Trainor, L. J. (2012). Use of prosody and information structure in high functioning adults with autism in relation to language ability. Front. Psychol. 3:72. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diehl, J. J., Watson, D., Bennetto, L., McDonough, J., and Gunlogson, C. (2009). An acoustic analysis of prosody in high-functioning autism. Appl. Psycholinguist. 30, 385–404. doi: 10.1017/S0142716409090201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, D. M., and Dunn, L. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th Edn. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson, Inc.

Google Scholar

Ellis Weismer, S., Plante, E., Jones, M., and Tomblin, J. B. (2005). A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of verbal working memory in adolescents with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 48, 405–425. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/028)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filipe, M. G., Frota, S., Castro, S. L., and Vicente, S. G. (2014). Atypical prosody in Asperger syndrome: perceptual and acoustic measurements. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 1972–1981. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2073-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filipe, M. G., Peppé, S., Frota, S., and Vicente, S. G. (2017). Prosodic development in European Portuguese from childhood to adulthood. Appl. Psycholinguist. 38, 1045–1070. doi: 10.1017/S0142716417000030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frota, S., Matos, N., Cruz, M., and Vigário, M. (2016). “Early prosodic development: emerging intonation and phrasing in European Portuguese,” in Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics: Interdisciplinary Approaches to Intonational Grammar in Ibero-Romance, eds M. Armstrong, M. M. Vanrell and N. C. Henriksen (Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins), 389.

Google Scholar

Gomes, H., Wolfson, V., and Halperin, J. M. (2007). Is there a selective relationship between language functioning and auditory attention in children? J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 29, 660–668. doi: 10.1080/13803390600920455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gooch, D., Thompson, P., Nash, H. M., Snowling, M. J., and Hulme, C. (2016). The development of executive function and language skills in the early school years. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57, 180–187. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawthorne, K., and Gerken, L. (2014). From pauses to clauses: prosody facilitates learning of syntactic constituency. Cognition 133, 420–428. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, L. A., Messer, D. J., and Nash, G. (2012). Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 37–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02430.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, C., Russell, J., and Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia 32, 477–492. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90092-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., and Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: the role of executive function. Child Dev. 77, 1822–1841. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00976.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joseph, R. M., McGrath, L. M., and Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Executive dysfunction and its relation to language ability in verbal school-age children with autism. Dev. Neuropsychol. 27, 361–378. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2703_4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv. Child 2, 217–250.

Google Scholar

Khanna, M. M., and Boland, J. E. (2010). Children’s use of language context in lexical ambiguity resolution. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 63, 160–193. doi: 10.1080/17470210902866664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Wilbourn, M. P., Vernon-Feagans, L., Blair, C. B., and Family Life Project Key Investigators (2014). Early communicative gestures prospectively predict language development and executive function in early childhood. Child Dev. 85, 1898–1914. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landry, R., and Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 1115–1122. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00304.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, L. B., Weismer, S. E., Miller, C. A., Francis, D. J., Tomblin, J. B., and Kail, R. V. (2007). Speed of processing, working memory, and language impairment in children. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 50, 408–428. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/029)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llorente, A. M., Williams, J., Satz, P., and D‘Elia, L. F. (2003). Children’s Color Trails Test: Professional Manual. Florida, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Google Scholar

Lord, C., Rutter, M. L., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., et al. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: a standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 19, 185–212. doi: 10.1007/BF02211841

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, C., Rutter, M. L., and Le Couteur, A. (1994). The autism diagnostic interview—revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luiz, D. M., Barnard, A., Knoesen, M. P., Kotras, N., Horrocks, S., McAlinden, P., et al. (2007). Escala de Desenvolvimento Mental de Griffiths — Extensão Revista (Revisão de 2006) dos 2 aos 8 anos: Manual de Administração. Lisboa: Cegoc-Tea.

Google Scholar

Marton, K. (2008). Visuo-spatial processing and executive functions in children with specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 43, 181–200. doi: 10.1080/16066350701340719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCann, J., and Peppé, S. (2003). Prosody in autism spectrum disorders: a critical review. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 38, 325–350. doi: 10.1080/1368282031000154204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCann, J., Peppé, S., Gibbon, F. E., O’Hare, A., and Rutherford, M. (2007). Prosody and its relationship to language in school-aged children with high-functioning autism. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 42, 682–702. doi: 10.1080/13682820601170102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McInnes, A., Bedard, A., Hogg-Johnson, S., and Tannock, R. (2007). Preliminary evidence of beneficial effects of methylphenidate on listening comprehension in children with Attention- Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 17, 35–49. doi: 10.1089/cap.2006.0051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montgomery, J. W. (2002). Information processing and language comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Top. Lang. Disord. 22, 62–84. doi: 10.1097/00011363-200205000-00007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montgomery, J. W., and Windsor, J. (2007). Examining the language performances of children with and without specific language impairment: contributions of phonological short-term memory and speed of processing. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 50, 778–797. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/054)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noterdaeme, M., Amorosa, H., Mildenberger, K., Sitter, S., and Minow, F. (2001). Evaluation of attention problems in children with autism and children with a specific language disorder. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 10, 58–66. doi: 10.1007/s007870170048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Hearn, K., Asato, M., Ordaz, S., and Luna, B. (2008). Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 20, 1103–1132. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozonoff, S., and Jensen, J. (1999). Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 29, 171–177. doi: 10.1023/A:1023052913110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozonoff, S., and McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 6, 415–431. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400006027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., and Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 32, 1081–1105. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00351.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozonoff, S., South, M., and Provencal, S. (2005). “Executive functions,” in Handbook of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3rd Edn, eds F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin and D. Cohen (New York, NY: Wiley), 606–627. doi: 10.1002/9780470939345.ch22

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, R., Augustyn, A., Klin, A., and Volkmark, F. R. (2005). Perception and production of prosody by speakers with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 35, 205–220. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1999-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peppé, S., and McCann, J. (2003). Assessing intonation and prosody in children with atypical language development: the PEPS-C test and the revised version. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 17, 345–354. doi: 10.1080/0269920031000079994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O’Hare, A., and Rutherford, M. (2007). Receptive and expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning autism. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 50, 1015–1028. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/071)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prior, M., and Hoffman, W. (1990). Neuropsychological testing of autistic children through an exploration with frontal lobe tests. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 20, 581–590. doi: 10.1007/BF02216063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajendran, G., and Mitchell, P. (2007). Cognitive theories of autism. Dev. Rev. 27, 224–260. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raven, J. C. (1995). Manual for the Colored Progressive Matrices (Revised). Windsor: NFRE-Nelson.

Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M. (1971). Trail Making Test results for normal and brain-damaged children. Percept. Mot. Skills 33, 575–581. doi: 10.2466/pms.1971.33.2.575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rice, M. L., Warren, S. F., and Betz, S. K. (2005). Language symptoms of developmental language disorders: an overview of autism, Down syndrome, fragile X, specific language impairment, and Williams syndrome. Appl. Psycholinguist. 26, 7–27. doi: 10.1017/S0142716405050034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, L., Marinis, T., Felser, C., and Clahsen, H. (2007). Antecedent priming at trace positions in children’s sentence processing. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 36, 175–188. doi: 10.1007/s10936-006-9038-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rumsey, J. M., and Hamburger, S. D. (1988). Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism residual state. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 10, 201–221. doi: 10.1080/01688638808408236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutherford, M. D., Baron-Cohen, S., and Wheelwright, S. (2002). Reading the mind in the voice: a study with normal adults and adults with Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 32, 189–194. doi: 10.1023/A:1015497629971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sainson, C., Barat, M., and Aguert, M. (2014). Communication disorders and executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury: an exploratory study using the GALI (a grid for linguistic analysis of free conversational interchange). Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 57, 664–683. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2014.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senju, A. (2012). Spontaneous theory of mind and its absence in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroscientist 18, 108–113. doi: 10.1177/1073858410397208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharda, M., Subhadra, T. P., Sahay, S., Nagaraja, C., Singh, L., Mishra, R., et al. (2010). Sounds of melody—pitch patterns of speech in autism. Neurosci. Lett. 478, 42–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.04.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shriberg, L. D., Paul, R., McSweeny, J. L., Klin, A., Cohen, D. J., and Volkmar, F. R. (2001). Speech and prosody characteristics of adolescents and adults with high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 44, 1097–1115. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/087)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simões, M. (2000). Investigações no âmbito da aferição nacional do Teste das Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas de Raven (M.P.C.R). Lisboa: Fundação Calouste Gulbenkian.

Slevc, L. R. (2011). Saying what’s on your mind: working memory effects on sentence production. J. Exp. Psychol. 37, 1503–1514. doi: 10.1037/a0024350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vugs, B., Hendriks, M., Cuperus, J., and Verhoeven, L. (2014). Working memory performance and executive function behaviors in young children with SLI. Res. Dev. Disabil. 35, 62–74. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, M., and Watson, D. G. (2010). Experimental and theoretical advances in prosody: a review. Lang. Cogn. Process. 25, 905–945. doi: 10.1080/01690961003589492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III): Manual, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Wells, B., Peppé, S., and Goulandris, A. (2004). Intonation development from five to thirteen. J. Child Lang. 31, 749–778. doi: 10.1017/S030500090400652X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westby, C., and Watson, S. (2004). Perspectives on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: executive functions, working memory, and language disabilities. Semin. Speech Lang. 25, 241–254. doi: 10.1055/s-2004-833672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, O., and Law, S. P. (2010). Executive functions and aphasia treatment outcomes: data from an ortho-phonological cueing therapy for anomia in Chinese. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 12, 529–544. doi: 10.3109/17549507.2011.516840

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: executive functions, prosody, prosodic skills, high-functioning autism, autism spectrum disorders

Citation: Filipe MG, Frota S and Vicente SG (2018) Executive Functions and Prosodic Abilities in Children With High-Functioning Autism. Front. Psychol. 9:359. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00359

Received: 07 December 2017; Accepted: 05 March 2018;
Published: 21 March 2018.

Edited by:

Antonino Vallesi, Università degli Studi di Padova, Italy

Reviewed by:

Verena Erica Pritchard, Australian Catholic University, Australia
Bahia Guellai, University of Paris Ouest Nanterre, France

Copyright © 2018 Filipe, Frota and Vicente. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marisa G. Filipe, marisafilipe@fpce.up.pt; labfon@letras.ulisboa.pt

These authors are joint last authors.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.