Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 March 2018
Sec. Educational Psychology

Self-Reported Risk and Delinquent Behavior and Problem Behavioral Intention in Hong Kong Adolescents: The Role of Moral Competence and Spirituality

\r\nDaniel T. L. Shek,,,,,*&#x;Daniel T. L. Shek1,2,3,4,5,6*†Xiaoqin Zhu&#x;Xiaoqin Zhu1†
  • 1Department of Applied Social Sciences, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Kowloon, Hong Kong
  • 2Centre for Innovation Programmes for Adolescents and Families, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Kowloon, Hong Kong
  • 3Department of Social Work, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 4Kiang Wu Nursing College of Macau, Macau, China
  • 5Hong Kong Institute of Service Leadership & Management Limited, Wanchai, Hong Kong
  • 6Division of Adolescent Medicine, Kentucky Children’s Hospital, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, United States

Based on the six-wave data collected from Grade 7 to Grade 12 students (N = 3,328 at Wave 1), this pioneer study examined the development of problem behaviors (risk and delinquent behavior and problem behavioral intention) and the predictors (moral competence and spirituality) among adolescents in Hong Kong. Individual growth curve models revealed that while risk and delinquent behavior accelerated and then slowed down in the high school years, adolescent problem behavioral intention slightly accelerated over time. After controlling the background socio-demographic factors, moral competence and spirituality were negatively associated with risk and delinquent behavior as well as problem behavioral intention across all waves as predicted. Regarding the rate of change in the outcome measures, while the initial level of spirituality was positively linked to the growth rate of risk and delinquent behavior, the initial level of moral competence was negatively associated with the growth rate of problem behavioral intention. The theoretical and practical implications of the present findings are discussed with reference to the role of moral competence and spirituality in the development of adolescent problem behavior.

Introduction

Adolescence is portrayed as a life stage full of “storm and stress.” While most adolescents can successfully adapt to adolescent challenges, some young people have maladaptive problems (Cheung, 2014; Tsitsika et al., 2015; Calado et al., 2017). Adolescent problem behaviors not only hinder adolescents’ current healthy functioning but also adversely impact their well-being in the long run. For instance, adolescents with higher probability of engaging in fighting and petty theft were more likely to report depression, substance abuse problems and dropping out of high school in early adulthood (Cook et al., 2015). A review based on studies published over a 25-year span showed that adolescents using alcohol, tobacco and other drugs frequently performed more badly academically, completed fewer years of education and were less likely to attend colleges compared with their peers without substance abuse problems (Bradley and Greene, 2013).

As adolescent problem behaviors have high personal, family and social costs, there is an urgent need to prevent adolescent problem behaviors. As some researchers argued that some internal assets could act as protective factors to reduce the occurrence of adolescent problem behaviors (Lo et al., 2011), the present study focused on two developmental assets (moral competence and spirituality) and examined how they predicted the externalizing behaviors indicated by risk and delinquent behavior and the intentions to engage in problem behaviors in Hong Kong adolescents.

Positive Youth Development and Externalizing Problem Behaviors

Positive youth development (PYD) attributes are well recognized as protective factors of adolescent externalizing problems (Lerner et al., 2013; Shek and Leung, 2013). The PYD perspective holds that multiple developmental assets help to buffer the negative influence of life difficulties during adolescence, which prevents adolescents from involving in problem behaviors (Lerner et al., 2013). For instance, in Geldhof et al.’s (2014) longitudinal study involving American students from Grade 5–12 (i.e., eight waves), PYD assessed as five Cs (i.e., competence, confidence, connection, character and caring) was inversely related to delinquency and substance use in all waves. Likewise, in a large-scale study which defined PYD in terms of 15 developmental assets (e.g., resilience, behavioral competence, moral competence, emotional competence, and spirituality), Shek and collaborators found that PYD had inverse concurrent and longitudinal associations with externalizing problem behaviors among Chinese adolescents (Sun and Shek, 2012; Shek and Lin, 2016a,b).

Besides, there are findings showing the linkage between problem behaviors and specific developmental assets such as moral competence and spirituality (e.g., Salas-Wright et al., 2013; Hardy et al., 2014), both of which are important elements of the 15 PYD constructs highlighted by Catalano et al. (2002). Unfortunately, most of the existing studies are based on cross-sectional data obtained in the Western societies. Although common psychosocial correlates (e.g., self-control) were found for delinquency in the Western and Chinese cultures (Chui and Chan, 2016), cultural differences were also observed with respect to adolescent perceptions and evaluations of delinquent behaviors (Tisak et al., 2017). Therefore, much remains unknown regarding the effect of a single developmental asset on adolescent externalizing problems in the Chinese societies.

Regarding the development of problem behaviors such as delinquency, the research findings in the Western and Chinese contexts suggested an inverted U-shaped trajectory featured with an increase in early adolescence and a decline in early adulthood (Deković et al., 2004; Shek and Lin, 2016a). However, relatively fewer studies attempted to uncover the relationship between the PYD attributes and the rate of change in adolescent problem behaviors. Among those who did, Shek and Lin (2016a,b) investigated how initial general PYD attributes encompassing the 15 developmental assets predicted the growth rate of delinquency over time. However, no research has to date been conducted to examine how specific developmental assets are related to the growth rate of risk and delinquent behavior and problem behavioral intention. To fill this gap, the present study focused on two specific developmental assets, namely moral competence and spirituality.

Moral Competence and Externalizing Problem Behaviors

In the literature, two conceptions of moral competence can be distinguished: the social-cognitive framework and the virtue approach. Within the social-cognitive framework, moral maturity is regarded as “the capacity to make decisions and judgments which are moral (that is, based on internal principles) and to act in accordance with such judgments” (Kohlberg, 1964, p. 425). According to this view, moral competence is conceived as moral judgment ability which develops with age along a six-stage process involving the transforming egocentric thinking to more social and ethical oriented thinking (Kohlberg, 1984; Gibbs et al., 2007; Gibbs, 2014). Recently, the last two and less common “principled” stages were reinterpreted as representing reflective philosophies indicative of an “existential developmental phase” (Gibbs et al., 2007; Gibbs, 2014).

Alternatively, using a virtue approach, Park and Peterson (2006) stated that “moral competence among the adolescents can be approached in terms of good character” (p. 891) where good character is “a family of positive traits reflected in thoughts, feelings and behavior” (p. 893). In their Values in Action (VIA) Classification framework, 24 character strengths, such as “appreciation of beauty and excellence” and “self-regulation,” are proposed (Park and Peterson, 2006). Moral competence as virtues is also emphasized in the management field. For example, Morales-Sánchez and Cabello-Medina (2013) proposed that “moral virtues in the workplace would be moral competencies” (p. 721). Martin and Austin (2010) proposed that moral competences consist of ten positive traits, including keeping promises and active caring about others.

These two conceptions also differ in their measurement of moral competence. Compared with moral judgment which is commonly assessed by dilemma-based moral judgment interviews or dilemma-free production tests (Gibbs et al., 2007), moral virtues are usually measured by self-report methods (e.g., Seider et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2015). Besides, scholars have developed validated self-report measures of moral competence as virtues in both the Western (Park and Peterson, 2006) and Chinese (Shek et al., 2007) contexts.

Despite these differences, the two approaches share a common feature regarding the relationship between moral competence and adolescent development. It is widely acknowledged that developmentally delayed moral judgment is associated with adolescent immoral behavior, such as delinquency (Kohlberg, 1984; Stams et al., 2006; Gibbs, 2014). Within the virtue approach, it is asserted that a good life can be constructed by building and promoting one’s strength of character (Seligman, 2002). Likewise, in their review of successful PYD programs in the United States, Catalano et al. (2002) asserted that moral competence is an important PYD construct which positively influences youth developmental outcomes. Research showed that certain character traits are negatively related to youth problems (e.g., bullying) while positively related to desired achievement and wellness (e.g., happiness) (Park, 2004; de Kemp et al., 2009). For instance, Kim and Brody (2005) reported that adolescent self-regulation negatively predicted the externalizing (i.e., delinquency) and internalizing problems (e.g., depression).

However, the related empirical evidence is far from conclusive. First, instead of examining how moral virtues affect delinquency, the prior studies primarily focused on the nature of virtues or virtue development (Berkowitz and Hoppe, 2009; Wang et al., 2015). Second, among the limited studies, the findings were not conclusive. For example, in Seider et al.’s (2013) study, although students’ misconduct in the school was negatively correlated with their integrity and perseverance, the predicting effect of the two attributes was not identified in the regression analyses. Third, the inconclusiveness of the picture may also be, at least in part, due to the recruitment of small samples and the lack of longitudinal studies (Seider et al., 2013).

Another unresolved theoretical issue is whether and how early development of moral virtues affects the developmental trajectories of problem behaviors. It is commonly conjectured that better development of positive characters including moral competence helps adolescents to cope with life stress more constructively, thus establishing a solid foundation for adolescent long-term adaptive adjustment (Lerner et al., 2013; Shek and Leung, 2013). For example, in Shek and Yu’s (2012) study, the adolescents who joined a PYD program demonstrated a slower increment of delinquency compared with their peers who did not receive the program. Two recent studies conducted by Shek and Lin (2016a,b) directly related the developmental trajectory of delinquency to a composite measure of adolescent PYD (e.g., emotional competence, moral competence and spirituality). Discordant with common sense, it was found that better initial PYD status was associated with a faster increasing rate of delinquency over the adolescent years. The researchers suggested that the experimentation of “safer” delinquent behaviors or other problem acts among the better-functioning adolescents may help explain the unexpected findings (Shek and Lin, 2016a). However, how the developmental trajectory of externalizing problem behaviors is affected by the level of moral competence remains unknown given such limited evidence.

To fill these research gaps, using a longitudinal design, this study investigated how moral competence defined by the virtues predict the level of as well the rate of change in externalizing problems among adolescents with specific reference to the Chinese context. As such, this study focused on three virtues that have traditionally been emphasized in the Chinese culture and echo the Western conception of character strengths.

The first virtue is the Confucian emphasis of “a perfect and virtuous man” (“Junzi” in Chinese) that connotes high moral expectation for oneself (Chan, 2008). It is strongly emphasized that one should strive to have high morality in Chinese education (Wang, 2004; Gu, 2006). This virtue is conceptually consistent with such virtues as “appreciation of beauty and excellence” and “courage” in the VIA model (Park and Peterson, 2006). The second virtue is “Xin” (trustworthiness), which is one of the five cardinal virtues of Confucianism (Shek et al., 2013). This virtue echoes the character strength of “persistence in one’s words and behaviors” in the VIA model (Park and Peterson, 2006) which has been emphasized by other researchers (Jormsri et al., 2005; Schniter et al., 2013). The third one is self-reflection and self-evaluation. The Confucians always advocate continuous and frequent self-evaluation and reflection as a way to examine one’s own morality (Wang, 2004). This attribute is also included in the VIA model (Park and Peterson, 2006) and is subsumed under “prudence” in the moral competency model proposed by Morales-Sánchez and Cabello-Medina (2013).

Another operational question related to conducting longitudinal study on problem behaviors is the assessment issue. Under the social-cognitive framework, some studies compared the officially defined delinquents with the non-delinquent controls and identified a clear negative linkage between moral judgment and delinquency (See Stams et al., 2006 for a review). In contrast, the relationships between moral judgment and self-report delinquency were much weaker (e.g., Tarry and Emler, 2007; Beerthuizen et al., 2013). While the known-group comparison involving official records is more objective, it has well-documented shortcomings such as its vulnerability to social class and race biases (Emler and Tarry, 2007). Hence, many researchers used the self-report methods (e.g., Emler and Tarry, 2007). Moreover, it is difficult, if not impossible, to use the officially defined delinquent behavior in longitudinal studies. This may be one of the reasons that almost all studies adopting this method are cross-sectional (Stams et al., 2006). Thus, similar to other longitudinal research (e.g., Raaijmakers et al., 2005), the present study employed self-report externalizing problem behaviors.

Spirituality and Externalizing Problem Behaviors

Although there is no agreement on the definition of spirituality, some essential elements are generally highlighted, including the purpose and meaning of life, hope, compassion, relationship, connectedness and beliefs in the higher being (Shek and Ho, 2016). As King and Boyatzis (2004) asserted, adolescence is “an age period of intense ideological hunger, a striving for meaning and purpose, and desire for relationships and connectedness” (p. 2). This notion is clearly supported by the human history and research studies (Shek, 2012). For example, Astin et al. (2005) reported that approximately 80% of the freshmen in the United States regarded themselves as “spiritual beings” who were interested in spirituality and believed in the sacredness of life. Similar findings were mentioned in other studies (Gallup and Bezilla, 1992; Bibby, 2006).

Many theorists contended that spirituality is an important source of resilience, which is an asset in coping with negative life experiences and can help adolescents deal with life difficulties and problems in an adaptive way (Crawford et al., 2006; Koenig, 2010). This argument echoes the theory proposed by Frankl (1967) which states that psychological and adjustment problems will fill the existential vacuum caused by the loss of meaning of life. Empirically, higher levels of spirituality are closely related to higher life satisfaction, less depressive symptoms, better self-images and less problem behaviors among adolescents (e.g., Wong et al., 2006; Kub and Solari-Twadell, 2013; Salas-Wright et al., 2013).

Despite the general proposal that spirituality is negatively related to adolescent problem behavior, there are still several research gaps in the literature. First, most studies conducted in the Western communities adopted the concept of religiosity without differentiating between the concepts of spirituality and religiosity, leading to a concept called “religiosity/spirituality (R/S)” (Kub and Solari-Twadell, 2013). Although it makes sense in the contexts in which the definition of spirituality is broad enough to include religious spirituality as an essential element (Worthington et al., 2011), the situation may be different in the Chinese communities where religion is not an indispensable part of many people’s lives. For example, a recent study reported that nearly 70% of the Chinese-speaking adolescents in Hong Kong did not have a religious belief and religiosity only accounted for a very small part of their life satisfaction (Yuen et al., 2016). Thus, for the Chinese adolescents, religion may not play a major role in the scope of spirituality compared with other critical components such as “the meaning of life” (Lau, 2006). This may be the reason why the meaning or purpose of life is usually the focus of the studies and measures pertaining to spirituality of the Chinese adolescents (Shek et al., 2007; Shek, 2010, 2012).

Second, similar to the case of moral competence, there is a lack of longitudinal research on the relationship between spirituality and adolescent problem behaviors regarding the level and the rate of change over time. To fill these two major knowledge gaps, the present study investigated the predictive effects of Chinese adolescents’ spirituality on both the levels and the developmental trajectories of risk and delinquent behavior as well as intentions to engage in problem behaviors.

The Present Study

The present study attempted to investigate whether adolescents’ moral competence and spirituality predicted the levels and change courses of the externalizing problem behaviors indexed by risk and delinquent behavior and problem behavioral intention across the high school years (i.e., from Grade 7 to Grade 12). There are several research questions in this study. First, we asked what the developmental trajectories of problem behaviors over the high school years are. Based on the previous findings (Shek and Lin, 2016a,b), we expected that there would be an increasing trend in adolescent risk and delinquent behavior (Hypothesis 1a) and problem behavioral intention (Hypothesis 1b) over time.

Second, we asked whether the moral competence and spirituality predicted the levels of risk and delinquent behavior and problem behavioral intention. Based on the PYD perspective and previous findings (Kim and Brody, 2005; Salas-Wright et al., 2013), we expected that adolescents with lower levels of moral competence or spirituality would demonstrate higher levels of risk and delinquent behavior (Hypothesis 2a and Hypothesis 2b, respectively) and higher problem behavioral intention (Hypothesis 2c and Hypothesis 2d, respectively).

Third, we asked whether moral competence and spirituality predicted the change rates of risk and delinquent behavior and problem behavioral intention. Based on the previous findings, we expected that lower levels of moral competence or spirituality would predict faster linear growth rates of risk and delinquent behavior (Hypothesis 3a and Hypothesis 3b, respectively) and problem behavioral intention (Hypothesis 3c and Hypothesis 3d, respectively).

Regarding the predictive effects of moral competence and spirituality on the quadratic changes of two problem behavior indicators, we did not make any specific hypotheses due to the lack of scientific literature on the theorization. As the previous studies have provided support for the influence of the socio-demographic factors (e.g., gender and economic disadvantage) on the problem behaviors (Shek and Lin, 2016b), the present study considered three socio-demographic factors as control variables.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedures

The present study used data obtained in a large-scale longitudinal project in Hong Kong, which commenced in 2009–2010 school year. In this project, a cohort of Grade 7 students from 28 randomly selected secondary schools in Hong Kong was followed through in six consecutive years. These students completed the same battery of measures each year, resulting in six waves of data. The interval between the first five successive waves was approximately 1 year while Wave 6 data collection took place approximately 10 months after Wave 5. This is because Grade 12 students in Hong Kong need to sit the public examination in the last few months during their high school study.

The sample consisted of 3,328 Grade 7 students (Mage = 12.59 ± .74 years) at Wave 1, including 1,735 (52.1%) boys, 1,584 (47.6%) girls and 9 (0.3%) who did not provide gender information. According to Shek et al. (2014), these participants constitute a representative sample of adolescents in Hong Kong as they compared favorably with general secondary school student population in Hong Kong regarding demographic characteristics. From Wave 2 to Wave 6, the number of participants joining the project decreased from 2,905 to 2,385. Therefore, the present attrition rates ranged between 12.7% and 28.3% across waves, which were favorable compared to previous longitudinal research lasting for multiple years (Bongers et al., 2003).

Before Wave 1 data collection, the participating schools and students’ parents were well informed that the data collected from the students would be kept confidential and used only for research purposes. Their written consent was obtained. Before each wave of data collection, the principles of voluntary participation and confidentiality were explained to the participating students and their written consent was also obtained. In each occasion of data collection, the questionnaires were administrated by the trained research staff in quiet classrooms in each participating school. The participants were instructed to give their own true opinion for each question and not to discuss with their classmates.

Instruments

The project included multiple measures, including the demographic characteristics (e.g., gender), positive developmental assets (e.g., moral competence), problem behaviors and family functioning (see Shek et al., 2014). The positive developmental assets were assessed using the short version of the Chinese Positive Youth Development Scale (CPYDS), which was developed and validated by Shek et al. (2007) and Shek and Ma (2010). Noteworthy, the CPYDS has been widely adopted to assess PYD attributes among Chinese adolescents (e.g., Shek and Lin, 2016a; Shek et al., 2016). A short version was used to reduce fatigue concerning the length of the whole questionnaire. To ensure the reliability and validity, three items having the highest factor loadings within each subscale were selected to form the short version of the CPYDS (Shek and Ma, 2010). The present study focused on four measures: moral competence, spirituality, delinquency and the problem behavioral intention.

Moral Competence

Moral competence was assessed by three items: (1) “I have high moral expectation about my behavior”; (2) “I will fulfill my promise”; and (3) “I have the habit of self-evaluation.” As aforementioned, these items are closely related to moral virtues in the Chinese Confucian thoughts. Moreover, they echo the character strengths in the VIA model: “appreciation for excellence,” “keeping one’s promise” and “self-reflection” respectively (Park and Peterson, 2006). A 6-point rating scale (1 = strongly disagree, 6 = strongly agree) was used and moral competence was computed as the average score across the three items. In the current study, reliability of the 3-item scale was supported (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics of key variables and reliability (Cronbach’s α) of scales across six waves.

Spirituality

The spirituality subscale of the CPYDS was modeled after the Chinese version of the Purpose in Life Questionnaire (Shek et al., 2007). The three items employed in the present study assessed whether the respondent perceived the life as colorful, meaningful and purposeful (e.g., “My life is dull versus colorful” and “My life is empty versus full”). The previous studies showed that the scale had good psychometric properties (Shek and Ma, 2010). The average score across the three items was calculated and a higher score represents a higher level of spirituality. As shown in Table 1, the 3-item spirituality scale showed good reliability in the current study.

Risk and Delinquent Behavior

This construct was assessed by the frequency of having 12 problem behaviors including those are illegal and acts are not so serious but considered as risk in the previous 1 year. These behaviors included “stealing,” “cheating,” “truancy,” “running away from home,” “damaging others’ properties,” “beating others,” “having sexual intercourse with others,” “gang fighting,” “speaking foul language,” “staying outside the home overnight without parental consent,” “bullying” and “trespassing.” A 7-point scale was used with a higher score representing a higher level of risk and delinquent behavior. A composite score was calculated by averaging the scores across all items and the scale reliability was supported (see Table 1).

Problem Behavioral Intention

On the 4-point scale (1 = absolutely will not, 4 = absolutely will), the participants reported their intention to engage in five problem behaviors, including drinking, smoking, gambling, drug abuse and having sexual intercourse, in the next 2 years. An average score of the scale was calculated for each participant. The 5-item scale demonstrated acceptable reliability (see Table 1).

Control Variables

Three socio-demographic factors were considered as control variables, including gender, family economic disadvantage and family intactness. In Hong Kong, living on welfare of “Comprehensive Social Security Assistance Scheme” (CSSA) provided by the government can be regarded as a valid index of family economic disadvantage. In the current study, 225 (6.76%) participants living on CSSA in Wave 1 were defined as having family economic disadvantage. Other 2,606 (78.31%) adolescents not living on CSSA were regarded as not having economic disadvantage. Family intactness was operationalized as the marital status of the participants’ parents. A total of 2,781 (83.56%) participants who indicated that their parents were in the first marriage were classified as the intact family group while 515 (15.47%) participants whose parents were divorced, separated or in their second marriage were categorized into non-intact family group. The participants with missing values in the control variables were excluded from statistical analysis involving the control variables.

Attrition Analyses and Missing Values Imputation

We compared the participants who had been joining the study with those who dropped out for each wave in terms of their baseline situation at Wave 1. Compared with the dropouts, those adolescents participating in the study were slightly younger, consisted of a higher proportion of girls, had higher levels of moral competence and spirituality and had lower levels of externalizing problem behaviors. To reduce bias induced by the systematic attrition, we used multiple imputation to impute the missing values of the core research variables. Following the guidelines given by Asendorpf et al. (2014), we applied the “multiple imputation” procedure of SPSS, applied the option of “Predictive Mean Matching” (PMM) and changed the default five imputations to forty. Besides, the three control variables were included as auxiliary variables in addition to the core research variables in the imputation equation. After this procedure, we had forty imputed datasets in addition to the original one.

After multiple imputation, statistical analyses including correlation analyses and individual growth curve (IGC) analyses were performed based on the original data set and the forty imputed data sets separately. For each statistical analysis parameter, an average value calculated across the imputation samples was the pooled result (Rubin, 1987). Analyses based on the original data set and the imputed data sets yielded similar results, suggesting that attrition was not a big problem in the present study. In the sections below, the pooled results were reported for correlations and the IGC models. This approach has been widely used recently and is highly recommended to minimize the attrition effect in the longitudinal studies (e.g., Asendorpf et al., 2014; Huijbers et al., 2015).

Data Analysis

Data analysis was conducted using SPSS for Windows, version 23.0 (IBM Corp., Somers, NY, United States). The reliability analysis and descriptive analyses were first conducted, followed by the correlation analyses and IGC analyses. The IGC analyses were conducted to examine the developmental trajectories of the adolescent problem behaviors and whether they were predicted by moral competence and spirituality, with the effects of the socio-demographic variables being controlled.

Individual growth curve is an analytic approach that has been frequently used to track the adolescent developmental trajectory and explore the individual differences in the growth rate (Shek and Yu, 2012; Shek and Lin, 2016b). Following the analysis procedures suggested by Shek and Ma (2011), we utilized a 2-level hierarchical model, which nested time (level 1) into individual (level 2). The model was tested in two steps. First, for each problem behavior indicator, three unconditional models using level-1 predictors (Model 1: unconditional mean model; Model 2: linear growth model; Model 3: quadratic growth model) were examined to identify the growth curve. Time was coded as 0, 1, 2, 3, 4, and 4.83 for Wave 1, Wave 2, Wave 3, Wave 4, Wave 5 and Wave 6, respectively. Second, three control variables and the two predictor variables were examined as time-invariant covariates to explore any individual variability in the shape of the growth trajectories with regard to the two problem behaviors. To illustrate, the 2-level model is depicted as follows:

Level1:Yij=β0j+β1j(Time)+β2j(Time2)+rij
Level − 2: β0j=γ00+γ01(gender)+γ02(economicdisadvantage)+γ03(familyintactness)+γ04(moralcompetence)+γ05(spirituality)+u0jβ1j=γ10+γ11(gender)+γ12(economicdisadvantage)+γ13(familyintactness)+γ14(moralcompetence)+γ15(spirituality)+u1jβ2j=γ20+γ21(gender)+γ22(economicdisadvantage)+γ23(familyintactness)+γ24(moralcompetence)+γ25(spirituality)+u2j

In each analysis, the model fit was indexed by three indices: -2log likelihood (i.e., deviance), Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), with a small index indicating a better model fit (Shek and Ma, 2011). Furthermore, the individual growth curves were plotted using the prototypical values.

Prior to the IGC analyses, the three control variables were dummy coded as follows: female = –1, male = 1; having economic disadvantage = -1, not having economic disadvantage = 1; non-intact family = -1, intact family = 1. Besides, the two predictors (i.e., moral competence and spirituality) were standardized so that the gamma coefficients of each predictor in intercept, linear slope and quadratic slope represented the changes of problem behavior under investigation, in mean value, rate of linear change and curvature, respectively, per unit of change in the predictor. Furthermore, a natural logarithmic transformation was performed for each problem behavior indicator to reduce the skewness in data distribution.

Results

The percentages of the adolescents who committed risk and delinquent behaviors at least once in the past 1 year are demonstrated in Table 2. Overall speaking, cheating and speaking foul language were the two most common misconduct. Specifically, the percentages of the adolescents who had ever cheated and spoken foul language more than once in the past 1 year ranged between 56.61 and 62.66%, and between 68.35 and 70.90%, respectively, across the six waves. Further examination of the inter-item correlations regarding delinquency showed acceptable mean inter-item correlations (i.e., around.30) across the six waves (Schmitt, 1996). Similar results were observed regarding the items in the problem behavioral intention measure.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Percentages of participants committing each risk or delinquent behavior at least once in the past one year in each wave.

As shown in Table 3, moral competence and spirituality levels were associated with risk and delinquent behavior and problem behavioral intention levels across the six waves. The IGC analyses were then conducted and the results are illustrated in Tables 47.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Pooled correlations among variables based on forty computed data sets.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Pooled results of IGC models with level-1 predictors for adolescent risk and delinquent behavior.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Pooled results of IGC models with level-2 predictors for adolescent risk and delinquent behavior.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 6. Pooled results of IGC models with level-1 predictors for adolescent problem behavioral intention.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 7. Pooled results of IGC models with level-2 predictors for adolescent problem behavioral intention.

Risk and Delinquent Behavior Over Time and Its Predictors

The results of the unconditional mean model (Model 1 in Table 4) indicated an intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) of.569, which implied that 56.9% of the total variance in adolescent risk and delinquent behavior was related to the inter-individual differences. Thus, there is necessity to perform the multi-level analyses (Shek and Ma, 2011). Moreover, the unconditional quadratic model (i.e., Model 3) showed a better model fit than the unconditional linear model, i.e., Model 2 [Δχ2(4) = 174.76; p < 0.001; ΔAIC = 166.76; ΔBIC = 135.16]. According to Model 3 (see Table 4), there was a significant linear increase in the whole sample’s average level of risk and delinquent behavior (β = 0.030, p < 0.001). Hence, Hypothesis 1a was supported. A negative and significant quadratic effect was also observed (β = –0.003, p < 0.001), although the quadratic change (-0.003) was small compared to the linear increasing trend (0.030). These results suggested that adolescent risk and delinquent behavior increased during the secondary school years, but such an increasing trend slightly slowed down over time (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Growth trajectory of adolescent risk and delinquent behavior and problem behavioral intention for the overall sample based on Model 3 in Tables 4, 6, respectively.

As the variability was significant in the intercept, linear slope and quadratic slope in Model 3, we examined the predictive effects in the intercept, linear and quadratic parameters. Model 4 only considered the control variables while Model 5 considered all predictors including moral competence and spirituality. As shown in Table 5, compared to Model 4, Model 5 had a better model fit [Δχ2(6) = 427.44; p < 0.001; ΔAIC = 415.44; ΔBIC = 369.10]. Also, Model 5 better fitted the data than did Model 3 [Δχ2(15) = 487.04; p < 0.001; ΔAIC = 457.04; ΔBIC = 342.97]. Therefore, the results were interpreted based on Model 5.

As shown in Model 5, adolescent risk and delinquent behavior increased over time (β = 0.044, p < 0.001) with the growth rate slightly slowing down (β = –0.006, p < 0.001). For the intercept, family intactness, moral competence and spirituality were the significant predictors of the variability. The results indicated that the students living in non-intact families reported a higher level of risk and delinquent behavior than those living in intact families. Post hoc analyses suggested that such a difference remained at a significant level across the six waves (ts > 3.29, ps < 0.001). Furthermore, the adolescents with a lower level of moral competence or spirituality reported a higher level of risk and delinquent behavior at the initial stage, giving support for Hypotheses 2a and 2b. As for the linear change rate, gender, economic disadvantage and spirituality were the significant predictors while moral competence was not (i.e., Hypothesis 3a was not supported). Specifically, the growth rate of risk and delinquent behavior was slower among the female adolescents, the adolescents without family economic disadvantage and the adolescents with a lower level of spirituality (see Figure 2). The predicting effect of spirituality on the linear growth rate of risk and delinquent behavior was just contrary to our Hypothesis 3b.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Growth trajectories of adolescent risk and delinquent behavior as a function of spirituality. The figures were plotted based on Model 5 in Table 5. High level indicates 1 SD higher than the mean value; low level indicates 1 SD lower than the mean value.

It is noteworthy that the greater increasing rate as suggested by the gamma coefficients in the adolescents with better development of spirituality did not offset the higher initial level of risk and delinquent behavior among those adolescents with a lower level of spirituality (see Figure 2). Post hoc analyses showed that the adolescents who had better development of moral competence or spirituality always reported lower levels of risk and delinquent behavior as compared with their counterparts who developed badly on these two PYD constructs. Such differences were supported by the significant and negative correlations between moral competence/spirituality and adolescent risk and delinquent behavior across the six waves (see Table 3).

Problem Behavioral Intention Over Time and Its Predictors

The results for the adolescent problem behavioral intention were largely similar to those for adolescent delinquency. First, as shown in Table 6, the unconditional quadratic model (Model 3) fitted the data significantly better than did the unconditional linear model (Model 2) [Δχ2(4) = 292.57; p < 0.001; ΔAIC = 284.57; ΔBIC = 252.96]. The results showed a significant and positive linear slope (β = 0.029, p < 0.001) as well as a significant and positive quadratic effect (β = 0.003, p < 0.001). As evident in Figure 1, the adolescent problem behavioral intention increased over the higher school years and such an increasing trend slightly accelerated over time. Thus, Hypothesis 1b was supported.

The predictive effects were also tested in the intercept, linear and quadratic parameters with the same data analysis procedures used for delinquency. The results in Table 7 showed that Model 5 including all predictors had the best model fit, compared to Model 3 [Δχ2(15) = 211.14; p < 0.001; ΔAIC = 181.14; ΔBIC = 67.07] and Model 4 [Δχ2(6) = 245.59; p < 0.001; ΔAIC = 133.59; ΔBIC = 187.25]. So, the results for the behavioral intention were also interpreted based on Model 5.

The results of Model 5 indicated an increasing trend in the adolescent problem behavioral intention over time (β = 0.041, p < 0.001). The results also showed that family economic disadvantage significantly accounted for the variability in the intercept, with the adolescents without economic disadvantage reporting a greater problem behavioral intention. Nevertheless, the post hoc analyses revealed that the adolescents with or without family economic disadvantage did not differ significantly in their problem behavioral intention across the six waves (ts < 1.75, ps > 0.08). Family intactness was also a significant predictor of variability in intercept, with the adolescents living in the non-intact families having a higher initial level of problem behavioral intention. As revealed by the post hoc analyses, such a difference reached a significant level at all waves (ts > 2.98, ps < 0.003). Furthermore, the adolescents with a lower level of moral competence or spirituality demonstrated a greater problem behavioral intention in the initial assessment. Hence, Hypotheses 2c and 2d were also supported.

Regarding the linear growth rate, gender, economic disadvantage and moral competence were the three significant predictors of the variability while spirituality was not. Specifically, the growth rate of the problem behavioral intention was slower among females, the adolescents not having family economic disadvantage and the adolescents with a higher level of moral competence (see Figure 3). Hence, our Hypothesis 3c was supported while Hypothesis 3d was not. Furthermore, despite a relatively small gamma coefficient, the significant predicting effects of economic disadvantage and moral competence on the quadratic change rate may partially offset their predicting effects on the linear change rate, resulting in not-so-obvious differences in the shape of the change trajectories among the adolescents with or without economic disadvantage, or the adolescents having better or worse development in moral competence (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Growth trajectories of adolescent problem behavioral intention as a function of moral competence. The figures were plotted based on Model 5 in Table 7. High level indicates 1SD higher than the mean value; low level indicates 1 SD lower than the mean value.

Similar to delinquency, post hoc analyses also indicated that the adolescents who had better development of moral competence or spirituality always reported lower levels of intention to engage in problem behaviors as compared with other adolescents who had lower levels of moral competence or spirituality. Such differences were also reflected by the significant and negative correlations between moral competence/spirituality and the adolescent problem behavioral intention across the six waves (see Table 3).

Discussion

This pioneer study employed two indicators (risk and delinquent behavior, and problem behavioral intention) and examined how they were predicted by moral competence and spirituality in a 6-year longitudinal study. Going beyond the associations between the level of problem behavior and its correlates, the current study also investigated how moral competence and spirituality predicted the developmental trajectories of adolescent problem behaviors. Obviously, the current study enriches our knowledge of Chinese adolescent problem behaviors and their correlates based on a large representative sample in Hong Kong, helping to promote our understanding of the related theoretical issues involved.

As hypothesized, there was a rise in risk and delinquent behavior as well as problem behavioral intention during the secondary school years. The results coincide with the previous findings in the Western societies (Bongers et al., 2003; Deković et al., 2004). While the growth rate of risk and delinquent behavior was gradually slowing down, the rate of change in problem behavioral intention slightly accelerated over time. Such a difference in the growth curves of the two indicators suggested that although adolescent problem behaviors may peak in mid-adolescence, the exact time point may not be identical for the different outcome measures. This finding reflected the complexity of the youth problem behaviors.

Congruent with the previous cross-sectional research in the Western culture (Kim and Brody, 2005; de Kemp et al., 2009), the current study found that the adolescents with a lower level of moral competence defined as virtues reported higher levels of risk and delinquent behavior and intention to engage in problem behavior in future at different time points. In addition to a “snapshot” of the inverse relationship between morality and the adolescents’ problem behaviors found in the cross-sectional research, our study further demonstrated its stability over the secondary school stage. This finding reaffirms the great importance of shaping adolescent moral competence in the high school years when young people restructure the concepts of self and others and start to develop a mature self-identity. According to Hardy et al. (2014), helping adolescents to internalize the social norms and moral principles may enable them to develop good characters and positive moral identities through integrating morality with self-concept.

Similar to moral competence, the results suggested that spirituality is also an important protective factor to lessen the likelihood of problem behaviors for the Chinese adolescents. Theoretically, spirituality defined in terms of the meaning of life helps the adolescents cope with adversity and renders them less likely to externalize the negative life experiences to problem behaviors (Shek, 2012). However, it may be a different case in the Western samples. For example, the Western theorists once suggested that religion may impact on the adolescents earlier while spirituality may play a bigger role during late adolescence and emerging adulthood (Kub and Solari-Twadell, 2013). It is noteworthy that most previous research using the Western samples usually combined spirituality with religiousness due to the high degree of overlap between the two concepts (Yonker et al., 2012), although the studies showed different influences of the two concepts (Salas-Wright et al., 2013).

Noteworthy, the present study also found that students without economic disadvantage or living in intact families demonstrated lower level and slower growth rate in externalizing problems, suggesting that both adolescents’ internal assets (e.g., moral competence and spirituality) and external assets serve as protective factors. Moral competence in the scope of Chinese virtues may imply one’s self-regulation that promote one’s internal motivation to behave in a morally and socially acceptable manner. One can argue that this type of self-regulation not only suggests one’s ability to exercise self-control, but also reduces the likelihood that adolescents treat misconduct as an alternative for action, both of which would significantly lower the risk of problem behaviors (Wikström and Treiber, 2007; Svensson et al., 2010). Referring to the theory of self-regulated and external-regulated learning and other behaviors (de la Fuente-Arias, 2017), while adolescents’ internal assets (e.g., moral competence) promote self-regulated moral behaviors, familial assets as external-regulators reinforce adolescent personal-regulation.

The present study also shed light on the predicting effects of the two developmental assets on the changes of problem behaviors over time. For risk and delinquent behavior, while moral competence did not show a significant effect on its linear change rate, a higher level of spirituality was associated with a faster linear increasing rate. For problem behavioral intention, a higher level of moral competence predicted a slower linear growth rate, whereas spirituality did not significantly predict the growth rate. There are two possible explanations for the insignificant predicting effects of moral competence on delinquency growth rate and of spirituality on problem behavioral intention growth rate. First, with risk and delinquent behavior gradually reaching its maximum level, the predicting effects of the developmental assets on the growth rate may confront a ceiling effect. This mechanism may also explain the prior findings that the predicting effects of morality on delinquency were rather small and would decline during middle and late adolescence (Matsueda, 1989; Raaijmakers et al., 2005). Second, given that the problem behavioral intention in the present study was at an early developmental stage featured with an overall low level, the floor effect may operate on the predicting effects of the developmental assets.

There may be other underlying mechanisms coexisting with the ceiling effect or floor effect. For risk and delinquent behavior, the positive association between its growth rate and spirituality was in line with the previous findings that the adolescents with better overall PYD had a faster increasing rate of delinquent behavior (Shek and Lin, 2016b). As suggested by Shek and Lin (2016b), the better-developed adolescents were more motivated to “experiment” on the “safe” risk acts. This might be valid as the present study found the high occurrences of those “safe” misconducts such as cheating and speaking foul language. However, such a mechanism may not work for the problem behavioral intention assessed in the present study, as this indicator included activities that are not “safe,” such as drinking and drug abuse, which may cause severe damage to one’s health and may be perceived as seriously wrong by the Chinese adolescents (Tisak et al., 2017). Instead, the negative relationship between moral competence and the linear change rate of the problem behavioral intention echoes the common PYD reasoning that better development of personal assets builds a constructive groundwork for adolescent behavioral adjustment (Lerner et al., 2013; Shek and Leung, 2013). Nevertheless, the faster increasing rate of risk and delinquent behavior among the adolescents with high spirituality was a novel finding which should be further addressed in future studies. Moreover, it should be noted that consistent with the prediction of the PYD literature, the levels of problem behaviors were consistently lower in the groups with higher spirituality.

Based on the current findings, it can be conjectured that different underlying mechanisms may operate at different developmental stages of problem behaviors, and the impact patterns may differ between different psychosocial competencies. While the present findings are pioneer, it is suggested that with more waves of investigation covering a longer period (e.g., from late childhood to emerging adulthood) or with different age cohorts being longitudinally assessed simultaneously, different predictive effects of one developmental asset at different developmental stages of one problem behavior could be uncovered. For example, Raaijmakers et al. (2005) reported that the longitudinal relationship between morality and delinquency followed different pathways among three age cohorts. Besides, more types of developmental assets should be examined simultaneously to enable the comparison of their relative effects.

Practically speaking, the present findings provide exciting evidence for enhancing adolescent health functioning and preventing the youth problem behaviors through promoting moral competence and/or spirituality. In fact, such programs have already been successfully implemented in the Western countries. For example, one international spirituality program entitled “All Round Training in Excellence Program” involved the adolescents from London, Vancouver, Johannesburg, and Mumbai, and the program was regarded as having positive influence on the participants’ mental health and well-being (Pandya, 2015). In addition, the PYD programs promoting moral competence, spirituality and other psychosocial competencies were perceived as effective in preventing problem behaviors and promoting health development in children and adolescents (Catalano et al., 2002, 2012). These programs seem warranted for the Chinese adolescents as well, given the worrying increasing trend of the adolescents’ problem behavior and the dearth of PYD programs attempting to nurture moral competence, spirituality and other developmental assets in the Chinese adolescents (Shek and Yu, 2011; Shek and Leung, 2013).

The present study has several limitations. First, participant attrition is non-random with boys, those with externalizing problem behaviors or lower level of developmental assets dropping out more frequently. This systematic attrition may cause bias to the research findings. Fortunately, we imputed the missing values of the core research variables using multiple imputation and the results showed that the findings obtained from the imputed data and original data were almost identical. This finding suggested that the non-random attrition did not significantly affect the present findings. Nevertheless, further research could try to retain the participants and avoid the systematic attrition problem in the first place.

Second, although the data of moral competence and spirituality were collected in each wave, only the initial levels of moral competence and spirituality were used to predict the occurrence and the change in adolescents’ problem behaviors. We only controlled the initial status of socio-demographic variables and did not consider the possibility that the participants’ economic condition and family structure might have changed across the 6 years. In addition, we measured competence and spirituality with shortened scales, which may cause content validity problems, although a usual procedure was utilized to select items and reliabilities of the two shortened scales were good. Therefore, future research could consider replicating the present findings by using the full scales.

Third, although most studies including the present one assumed the influence of developmental assets on adolescent problem behaviors, it is possible that the relationship between adolescent competence (e.g., moral competence) and problem behaviors is bidirectional in nature (Raaijmakers et al., 2005; Arbeit et al., 2014). Knowing that not addressing the reciprocity of the effects within a limited length is one limitation, we have been attempting to fill this research gap by examining the reciprocal relationship between adolescent moral competence in terms of virtues and externalizing problems in the next paper (Shek and Zhu, unpublished). Different from the current paper which used 6-wave data and individual growth curve modeling, the next paper will focus on three selected time points during early adolescence and use cross-lagged analyses.

Fourth, the data on moral competence, spirituality and problem behaviors were based on the self-report measures. The self-reported problem behaviors were regarded as efficient, precise, reliable and valid in longitudinal studies (Emler and Tarry, 2007). It is also a common practice to measure positive traits by self-report inventories (Park and Peterson, 2006; Shek et al., 2007). However, the participants might provide socially desired answers by under-reporting their problem behaviors or over-estimating their moral competence and spirituality. Given that the instructions of not communicating with classmates and emphasis on anonymity were made very clearly before questionnaire administration in each wave, social desirability bias and peer pressure might have been reduced. Hence, it would be helpful to use social desirability measures or obtain the data from other informants such as teachers and parents (de Kemp et al., 2009). Nevertheless, considering the ethical and practical concerns (e.g., the adolescents themselves know the most of their own lives and experiences), the self-report measures are most appropriate and widely used in adolescent research (e.g., Thornberry and Krohn, 2003).

Fifth, as the present study only examined two developmental assets, future studies may extend the present findings by investigating and comparing the effects of more developmental assets. In addition, within the scope of morality, there are also other constructs, such as moral beliefs and moral emotions (e.g., shame and guilty), which also serve as protective factors for reducing adolescent delinquency (Krettenauer and Eichler, 2006; Beerthuizen and Brugman, 2016; Piquero et al., 2016). It is possible that these constructs have interactions with moral competence, which need to be addressed in future research.

Last, problem behaviors were considered as the overall risk and delinquent behavior and intention to engage in problem behaviors, limiting the present findings to the overall level of problem behaviors. Concerning the not-particularly high reliability of measuring scales and the prevalence of different forms of risk and delinquent behaviors observed in the present study, it is possible that some behaviors are more common (e.g., speaking foul language) than others (e.g., gang fighting). However, as stated in the section of results, an examination of the inter-item correlation coefficients showed that they are acceptable. Nevertheless, it would be insightful to examine each form of risk and delinquent acts separately or compare risk behavior with delinquency that presumes illegal acts in the future research.

In short, the present study is the first longitudinal study in different Chinese societies which examined this issue in the high school years using the individual growth curve modeling. Second, the findings suggested that there is a need to differentiate between the levels and trajectories of adolescent problem behaviors. Third, the present findings suggested that moral competence and spirituality can function as important protective factors for lowering the level of problem behaviors among Chinese adolescents.

Ethics Statement

The project was approved by the Human Subjects Ethics Sub-committee (HSESC) (or its Delegate) of the authors’ university. The participating schools and students’ parents were well informed that the data collected from the students would be kept confidential and used only for research purposes. Their written consent was obtained. Before each wave of data collection, the principles of voluntary participation and confidentiality were explained to the participating students and their written consent was also obtained.

Author Contributions

DS designed the project and contributed to all steps of the work. XZ contributed to the idea construction and data interpretation of the work, drafted the work and revised it based on the critical comments provided by DS. All authors approved of the final version of the manuscript and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Funding

This research and the 6-wave longitudinal study are financially supported by The Hong Kong Jockey Club Charities Trust.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Arbeit, M. R., Johnson, S. K., Champine, R. B., Greenman, K. N., Lerner, J. V., and Lerner, R. M. (2014). Profiles of problematic behaviors across adolescence: covariations with indicators of positive youth development. J. Youth Adolesc. 43, 971–990. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0092-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asendorpf, J. B., van de Schoot, R., Denissen, J. J. A., and Hutteman, R. (2014). Reducing bias due to systematic attrition in longitudinal studies: the benefits of multiple imputation. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 38, 453–460. doi: 10.1177/0165025414542713

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astin, A. W., Astin, H. S., Lindholm, J. A., Bryant, A. N., Szelényi, K., and Calderone, S. (2005). The Spiritual Life of College Students: A National Study of College Students’ Search for Meaning and Purpose. Los Angeles, CA: Higher Education Research Institute.

Google Scholar

Beerthuizen, M. G. C. J., and Brugman, D. (2016). The relationship of moral value evaluation with externalizing behaviour across value areas in adolescents. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 13, 84–98. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2015.1066669

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beerthuizen, M. G. C. J., Brugman, D., and Basinger, K. S. (2013). Oppositional defiance, moral reasoning and moral value evaluation as predictors of self-reported juvenile delinquency. J. Moral Educ. 42, 460–474. doi: 10.1080/03057240.2013.803955

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkowitz, M. W., and Hoppe, M. A. (2009). Character education and gifted children. High Ability Stud. 20, 131–142. doi: 10.1080/13598130903358493

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bibby, R. W. (2006). The Boomer Factor: What Canada’s Most Famous Generation is Leaving Behind. Toronto, ON: Bastian Books.

Google Scholar

Bongers, I. L., Koot, H. M., van der Ende, J., and Verhulst, F. C. (2003). The normative development of child and adolescent problem behavior. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 112, 179–192. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.2.179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, B. J., and Greene, A. C. (2013). Do health and education agencies in the United States share responsibility for academic achievement and health? A review of 25 years of evidence about the relationship of adolescents’ academic achievement and health behaviors. J. Adolesc. Health 52, 523–532. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calado, F., Alexandre, J., and Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Prevalence of adolescent problem gambling: a systematic review of recent research. J. Gambl. Stud. 33, 397–424. doi: 10.1007/s10899-016-9627-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catalano, R. F., Berglund, M. L., Ryan, J. A. M., Lonczak, H. S., and Hawkins, J. D. (2002). Positive youth development in the United States: research findings on evaluations of positive youth development programs. Prev. Treat. 5, 1–111. doi: 10.1037/1522-3736.5.1.515a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catalano, R. F., Fagan, A. A., Gavin, L. E., Greenberg, M. T., Irwin, CE Jr, Ross, D. A., et al. (2012). Worldwide application of prevention science in adolescent health. Lancet 379, 1653–1664. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60238-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, W. T. (2008). A Source Book in Chinese Philosophy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Cheung, N. W. T. (2014). Low self-control and co-occurrence of gambling with substance use and delinquency among Chinese adolescents. J. Gambl. Stud. 30, 105–124. doi: 10.1007/s10899-012-9351-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chui, W. H., and Chan, O. H. C. (2016). The gendered analysis of self-control on theft and violent delinquency: an examination of Hong Kong adolescent population. Crime Delinq. 62, 1648–1677. doi: 10.1177/0011128712470992

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, E. C., Pflieger, J. C., Connell, A. M., and Connell, C. M. (2015). Do specific transitional patterns of antisocial behavior during adolescence increase risk for problems in young adulthood? J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 43, 95–106. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9880-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, E., Wright, M. O., and Masten, A. S. (2006). “Resilience and spirituality in youth,” in The Handbook of Spiritual Development in Childhood and Adolescence, eds E. C. Roehlkepartain, P. E. King, L. Wagener, and P. L. Benson (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 355–370. doi: 10.4135/9781412976657.n25

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Kemp, R. A. T., Vermulst, A. A., Finkenauer, C., Scholte, R. H. J., Overbeek, G., Rommes, E. W. M., et al. (2009). Self-control and early adolescent antisocial behavior: a longitudinal analysis. J. Early Adolesc. 29, 497–517. doi: 10.1177/0272431608324474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente-Arias, J. (2017). Theory of self-vs. externally-regulated learningTM: fundamentals, evidence, and applicability. Front. Psychol. 8:1675. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deković, M., Buist, K. L., and Reitz, E. (2004). Stability and changes in problem behavior during adolescence: latent growth analysis. J. Youth Adolesc. 33, 1–12. doi: 10.1023/A:1027305312204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emler, N., and Tarry, H. (2007). Clutching at straws: is it time to abandon the moral judgment deficit explanation for delinquency? Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 25, 191–195. doi: 10.1348/026151007X178084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankl, V. E. (1967). Psychotherapy and Existentialism: Selected Papers on Logotherapy. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.

Google Scholar

Gallup, G. H., and Bezilla, R. (1992). The Religious Life of Young Americans: A Compendium of Surveys on the Spiritual Beliefs and Practices of Teenagers and Young Adults. Princeton, NJ: George H. Gallup International Institute,

Google Scholar

Geldhof, G. J., Bowers, E. P., Mueller, M. K., Napolitano, C. M., Callina, K. S., and Lerner, R. M. (2014). Longitudinal analysis of a very short measure of positive youth development. J. Youth Adolesc. 43, 933–949. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0093-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbs, J. C. (2014). Moral Development & Reality: Beyond the Theories of Kohlberg, Hoffman, and Haidt, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Gibbs, J. C., Basinger, K. S., Grime, R. L., and Snarey, J. R. (2007). Moral judgment development across cultures: revisiting Kohlberg’s universality claims. Dev. Rev. 27, 443–500. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, M. (2006). An analysis of the impact of traditional Chinese culture on Chinese education. Front. Educ. China 1:169–190. doi: 10.1007/s11516-006-0001-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, S. A., Walker, L. J., Olsen, J. A., Woodbury, R. D., and Hickman, J. R. (2014). Moral identity as moral ideal self: links to adolescent outcomes. Dev. Psychol. 50, 45–57. doi: 10.1037/a0033598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huijbers, M. J., Spinhoven, P., Spijker, J., Ruhé, H. G., van Schaik, D. J. F., van Oppen, P., et al. (2015). Adding mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to maintenance antidepressant medication for prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depressive disorder: randomised controlled trial. J. Affect. Disord. 187, 54–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.08.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jormsri, P., Kunaviktikul, W., Ketefian, S., and Chaowalit, A. (2005). Moral competence in nursing practice. Nurs. Ethics 12, 582–594. doi: 10.1191/0969733005ne828oa

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S., and Brody, G. H. (2005). Longitudinal pathways to psychological adjustment among black youth living in single-parent households. J. Fam. Psychol. 19, 305–313. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.2.305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, P. E., and Boyatzis, C. J. (2004). Exploring adolescent spiritual and religious development: current and future theoretical and empirical perspectives. Appl. Dev. Sci. 8, 2–6. doi: 10.1207/S1532480XADS0801_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, H. G. (2010). Spirituality and mental health. Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Stud. 7, 116–122. doi: 10.1002/aps.239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohlberg, L. (1964). “Development of moral character and moral ideology,” in Review of Child Development Research, Vol. 1, eds M. L. Hoffman and L. W. Hoffman (New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation), 383–431.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays on Moral Development: The Psychology of Moral Development, Vol. 2. San Francisco, CA: Harper & Row.

Google Scholar

Krettenauer, T., and Eichler, D. (2006). Adolescents’ self-attributed moral emotions following a moral transgression: relations with delinquency, confidence in moral judgment and age. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 24, 489–506. doi: 10.1348/026151005X50825

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kub, J., and Solari-Twadell, P. A. (2013). Religiosity/spirituality and substance use in adolescence as related to positive development: a literature review. J. Addict. Nurs. 24, 247–262. doi: 10.1097/JAN.0000000000000006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, P. S. Y. (2006). Spirituality as a positive youth development construct: conceptual bases and implications for curriculum development. Int. J. Adolesc. Med. Health 18, 363–370. doi: 10.1515/IJAMH.2006.18.3.363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lerner, J. V., Bowers, E. P., Minor, K., Boyd, M. J., Mueller, M. K., Schmid, K. L., et al. (2013). “Positive youth development: processes, philosophies, and programs,” in Handbook of Psychology: Developmental Psychology, Vol. 6, eds I. B. Weiner, R. M. Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks, and J. Mistry (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons), 365–392.

Google Scholar

Lo, T. W., Cheng, C. H. K., Wong, D. S. W., Rochelle, T. L., and Kwok, S. I. (2011). Self-esteem, self-efficacy and deviant behavior of young people in Hong Kong. Adv. Appl. Sociol. 1, 48–55. doi: 10.4236/aasoci.2011.11004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, D. E., and Austin, B. (2010). Validation of the moral competency inventory measurement instrument: content, construct, convergent and discriminant approaches. Manag. Res. Rev. 33, 437–451. doi: 10.1108/01409171011041884

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsueda, R. L. (1989). The dynamics of moral beliefs and minor deviance. Soc. Forces 68, 428–457. doi: 10.2307/2579255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morales-Sánchez, R., and Cabello-Medina, C. (2013). The role of four universal moral competencies in ethical decision-making. J. Bus. Ethics 116, 717–734. doi: 10.1007/s10551-013-1817-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandya, S. P. (2015). Adolescents, well-being and spirituality: insights from a spiritual program. Int. J. Child. Spiritual. 20, 29–49. doi: 10.1080/1364436X.2014.999230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, N. (2004). The role of subjective well-being in positive youth development. Ann. Am. Acad. Polit. Soc. Sci. 591, 25–39. doi: 10.1177/0002716203260078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, N., and Peterson, C. (2006). Moral competence and character strengths among adolescents: the development and validation of the values in action inventory of strengths for youth. J. Adolesc. 29, 891–909. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piquero, A. R., Bouffard, J. A., Piquero, N. L., and Craig, J. M. (2016). Does morality condition the deterrent effect of perceived certainty among incarcerated felons? Crime Delinq. 62, 3–25. doi: 10.1177/0011128713505484

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raaijmakers, Q. A. W., Engels, R. C. M. E., and Van Hoof, A. (2005). Delinquency and moral reasoning in adolescence and young adulthood. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 29, 247–258. doi: 10.1080/01650250544000035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. doi: 10.1002/9780470316696

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salas-Wright, C. P., Olate, R., and Vaughn, M. G. (2013). The protective effects of religious coping and spirituality on delinquency: results among high-risk and gang-involved Salvadoran youth. Crim. Justice Behav. 40, 988–1008. doi: 10.1177/0093854813482307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, N. (1996). Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha. Psychol. Assess. 8, 350–353. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.8.4.350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schniter, E., Sheremeta, R. M., and Sznycer, D. (2013). Building and rebuilding trust with promises and apologies. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 94, 242–256. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2012.09.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seider, S., Gilbert, J. K., Novick, S., and Gomez, J. (2013). The role of moral and performance character strengths in predicting achievement and conduct among urban middle school students. Teach. Coll. Rec. 115, 1–34.

Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic Happiness: Using the New Positive Psychology to Realize Your Potential for Lasting Fulfillment. New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L. (2010). “The spirituality of the Chinese people: a critical review,” in The Oxford Handbook of Chinese Psychology, ed. M. H. Bond (Oxford: Oxford University Press), c343–c366.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L. (2012). Spirituality as a positive youth development construct: a conceptual review. J. Altern. Med. Res. 4, 349–358. doi: 10.1100/2012/458953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L., and Ho, W. (2016). Spirituality as a key positive youth development construct for university students. Int. J. Disabil. Hum. Dev. 15, 175–180. doi: 10.1515/ijdhd-2016-0707

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L., and Leung, J. T. Y. (2013). “Adolescent developmental issues in Hong Kong: phenomena and implications for youth service,” in Development and Evaluation of Positive Adolescent Training Through Holistic Social Programs (P.A.T.H.S.), eds D. T. L. Shek and R. C. F. Sun (Singapore: Springer), 1–13. doi: 10.1007/978-981-4451-54-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L., and Lin, L. (2016a). Delinquent behavior in high school students in Hong Kong: sociodemographic, personal, and family determinants. J. Pediatr. Adolesc. Gynecol. 29, S61–S71. doi: 10.1016/j.jpag.2015.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L., and Lin, L. (2016b). What predicts adolescent delinquent behavior in Hong Kong? A longitudinal study of personal and family factors. Soc. Indic. Res. 129, 1291–1318. doi: 10.1007/s11205-015-1170-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L., and Ma, C. M. S. (2010). Dimensionality of the Chinese positive youth development scale: confirmatory factor analyses. Soc. Indic. Res. 98, 41–59. doi: 10.1007/s11205-009-9515-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L., and Ma, C. M. S. (2011). Longitudinal data analyses using linear mixed models in SPSS: concepts, procedures and illustrations. Sci. World J. 11, 42–76. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2011.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L., Siu, A. M. H., and Lee, T. Y. (2007). The Chinese positive youth development scale: a validation study. Res. Soc. Work Pract. 17, 380–391. doi: 10.1177/1049731506296196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L., Sun, R. C. F., and Ma, C. M. S. (2014). Chinese Adolescents in Hong Kong: Family Life, Psychological Well-Being and Risk Behavior. Singapore: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-981-287-143-5

CrossRef Full Text

Shek, D. T. L., and Yu, L. (2011). A review of validated youth prevention and positive youth development programs in Asia. Int. J. Adolesc. Med. Health 23, 317–324. doi: 10.1515/IJAMH.2011.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L., and Yu, L. (2012). Longitudinal impact of the Project P.A.T.H.S. on adolescent risk behavior: what happened after five years?. Sci. World J. 2012:316029. doi: 10.1100/2012/316029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L., Yu, L., and Fu, X. (2013). Confucian virtues and Chinese adolescent development: a conceptual review. Int. J. Adolesc. Med. Health 25, 335–344. doi: 10.1515/ijamh-2013-0031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L., Yu, L., Wu, F. K. Y., Zhu, X., and Chan, K. H. Y. (2016). A 4-year longitudinal study of well-being of Chinese university students in Hong Kong. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 12, 867–884. doi: 10.1007/s11482-016-9493-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stams, G. J., Brugman, D., Deković, M., van Rosmalen, L., van der Laan, P., and Gibbs, J. C. (2006). The moral judgment of juvenile delinquents: a meta-analysis. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 34, 697–713. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9056-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, R. C. F., and Shek, D. T. L. (2012). Positive youth development, life satisfaction and problem behaviour among Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong: a replication. Soc. Indic. Res. 105, 541–559. doi: 10.1007/s11205-011-9786-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svensson, R., Pauwels, L., and Weerman, F. M. (2010). Does the effect of self-control on adolescent offending vary by level of morality? A test in three countries. Crim. Justice Behav. 37, 732–743. doi: 10.1177/0093854810366542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarry, H., and Emler, N. (2007). Attitudes, values and moral reasoning as predictors of delinquency. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 25, 169–183. doi: 10.1348/026151006X113671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thornberry, T. P., and Krohn, M. D. (2003). “Comparison of self-report and official data for measuring crime,” in Measurement Problems in Criminal Justice Research: Workshop Summary, eds J. V. Pepper and C. V. Petrie (Washington, DC: The National Academies Press), 43–94.

Google Scholar

Tisak, M. S., Tisak, J., Chen, Y., Fang, Q., and Baker, E. R. (2017). Adolescent misconduct behaviors: a cross-cultural perspective of adolescents and their parents. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 48, 155–167. doi: 10.1177/0022022116681844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsitsika, A., Janikian, M., Wójcik, S., Makaruk, K., Tzavela, E., Tzavara, C., et al. (2015). Cyberbullying victimization prevalence and associations with internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents in six European countries. Comput. Human Behav. 51, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, F. (2004). Confucian thinking in traditional moral education: key ideas and fundamental features. J. Moral Educ. 33, 429–447. doi: 10.1080/0305724042000327984

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Ferris, K. A., Hershberg, R. M., and Lerner, R. M. (2015). Developmental trajectories of youth character: a five-wave longitudinal study of cub scouts and non-scout boys. J. Youth Adolesc. 44, 2359–2373. doi: 10.1007/s10964-015-0340-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wikström, P.-O. H., and Treiber, K. (2007). The role of self-control in crime causation: beyond Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime. Eur. J. Criminol. 4, 237–264. doi: 10.1177/1477370807074858

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, Y. J., Rew, L., and Slaikeu, K. D. (2006). A systematic review of recent research on adolescent religiosity/spirituality and mental health. Issues Ment. Health Nurs. 27, 161–183. doi: 10.1080/01612840500436941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worthington, E. L. Jr., Hook, J. N., Davis, D. E., and McDaniel, M. A. (2011). Religion and spirituality. J. Clin. Psychol. 67, 204–214. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yonker, J. E., Schnabelrauch, C. A., and DeHaan, L. G. (2012). The relationship between spirituality and religiosity on psychological outcomes in adolescents and emerging adults: a meta-analytic review. J. Adolesc. 35, 299–314. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuen, C. Y. M., Lee, M., and Leung, S. C. S. (2016). Religious belief and its association with life satisfaction of adolescents in Hong Kong. J. Belief Values 37, 103–113. doi: 10.1080/13617672.2016.1141533

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: delinquent behavior, longitudinal study, developmental trajectory, developmental asset, Chinese adolescents

Citation: Shek DTL and Zhu X (2018) Self-Reported Risk and Delinquent Behavior and Problem Behavioral Intention in Hong Kong Adolescents: The Role of Moral Competence and Spirituality. Front. Psychol. 9:430. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00430

Received: 11 February 2018; Accepted: 15 March 2018;
Published: 29 March 2018.

Edited by:

Jesus de la Fuente, University of Almería, Spain

Reviewed by:

Neven Ricijas, University of Zagreb, Croatia
Matt DeLisi, Iowa State University, United States

Copyright © 2018 Shek and Zhu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Daniel T. L. Shek, daniel.shek@polyu.edu.hk

These authors have contributed equally to this work.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.