Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 20 June 2018
Sec. Cognitive Science

Outcome Evaluation in Social Context Measured by Event-Related Potentials Is Partially Dependent on the Partner’s Sex

\r\nJinping LiuJinping Liu1Shurui WangShurui Wang1Zhenbiao JiaZhenbiao Jia1Entao Zhang*Entao Zhang2*Mengping YaoMengping Yao3
  • 1Institute of Psychology and Behavior, Henan University, Kaifeng, China
  • 2Institute of Cognition, Brain and Health, Henan University, Kaifeng, China
  • 3Chongqing Vocational College of Public Transporation, Chongqing, China

Outcome evaluation is a cognitive process that people rely on feedback information to evaluate behavior results. It can help people to modify the previous mistakes in order to facilitate the performance of the behavior. In the present study, we examined sex differences in outcome evaluation when men and women performed a “Chuck-A-Luck” dice game with a same-versus opposite-sex partner. We recruited 40 college students (Half of women) to perform the gambling game task, and event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded for outcome feed back when male or female participants performed the game alone, or with same-versus opposite-sex partners. Two main findings are reported in our study. (1) FRN amplitude of same-sex condition was significantly greater than alone condition for male when the feedback was loss. However, FRN amplitude of opposite-sex condition was significantly greater than alone condition for female when feedback was loss. (2) The loss feedback induced greater P300 than gain only in alone condition. It suggests that sex differences in outcome evaluation is a complex process that is partially influenced by the partner’s sex.

Introduction

Individuals can quickly and accurately evaluate the valence of feedback information, and modify the previous mistakes in order to facilitate the performance of the behavior, which is called outcome evaluation (Sun and Luo, 2008). It is one of the important functions of the cognitive system. Outcome evaluation can help people monitor the behavior results, detect and correct mistakes in time, adjust the follow-up behaviors and improve the behavior efficiency.

In recent years, the neural mechanism of the outcome evaluation has gradually became a hot topic in the field of cognitive neuroscience (Miltner et al., 1997; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005). Many studies have traditionally measured the brain activities of outcome evaluation when participants were performing a task alone (Zhou et al., 2010; Luo et al., 2011). However, in daily life, our outcome evaluations are not carried out in isolation, but are modulated by social context, especially by the presence or role of other persons (Leng and Zhou, 2010; Boksem et al., 2011; Yuan et al., 2012; Yilei et al., 2017). For example, Boksem et al. (2011) found that the feedback-related negativity (FRN), reflecting a fast outcome evaluation, was larger when an individual’s own reward was worse than that for others. Unlike the task used by Boksem et al. (2011), using a joint task in which multiple participants performed Chuck-a-Luck dice game together, Li et al. (2010) found that the FRN effect for monetary gains and losses associated with outcomes in joint task became smaller when they played as part of a team compared to when they played alone. These findings have shown that a person’s outcome evaluation is sensitive to the influence of others. More importantly, an outcome is regarded as positive or negative depends on its relevance to the self-interest of the observer (Leng and Zhou, 2010). However, it remains unclear whether there are sex differences in outcome evaluation during two participants performed a task together.

Studies examining sex differences have found important differences between male and female cognition and behavior. Traditionally, women are expected to be more sensitive to social stimuli (Maccoby and Jacklin, 1974). In line with this view, there is some evidence that women tend to pay closer attention to others, including others’ presence, emotions and behaviors, while men tend to focus on their own outcomes over others’ outcomes (Oswald et al., 2004; Mercadillo et al., 2011). However, there is also conflicting evidence for this view (Lithari et al., 2010; Lamm et al., 2011). A recent meta-analytic study indicates that sex differences in social sensitivity depend on contextual factors, for example, increase shared performance is found for male–male groups than female–female groups (Balliet et al., 2011). For mixed-sex condition, other studies have found that individuals perform better while interacting with opposite-sex partners than with same-sex partners (Hirnstein et al., 2014; Cheng et al., 2015). These findings suggest that sex differences in social sensitivity might be modulated by the social context of same- versus opposite-sex partner. Thus, it may be more productive to investigate sex differences in outcome evaluation when men and women perform a task with a same-versus opposite-sex partner.

The present study tested sex differences in outcome evaluation when men and women performed a “Chuck-A-Luck” dice game with a same-versus opposite -sex partner. We used the “Chuck-A-Luck” game in order to create a joint task context in which participants conducted a gambling game alone or co-acting with the same or opposite-sex partners. We adopted electroencephalography (EEG) to access correlated neural activity of outcome evaluation. Two important potentials were analyzed, including the FRN and P300 components. The FRN component is a negative-going deflection that peaks approximately 250–300 ms after the onset of external feedback. It is thought to be a fast and coarse evaluation of external feedback leading to a simple distinction between good and bad outcomes (Hajcak et al., 2005). FRN amplitude has been found to be affected by the extent of self-relevance for the outcome, with larger FRN amplitudes corresponding to higher self-relevance for the outcome (Yeung et al., 2005; Li et al., 2010; Ma et al., 2011). However, the degree to which sex differences affects the FRN remains relatively unexplored. If sex differences in an early outcome evaluation depends on partners’ sex of women and men, we predict a interactive effect between sex and social context in the FRN.

Another ERP component is the P300, which is the most positive peak in the period of 200–600 ms. It has also been found to be related to outcome evaluation or reward processing (Hajcak et al., 2005; Feng et al., 2016). Amplitude of the P300 has also been reported to be sensitive to social contexts, with larger P300 being associated with closer interpersonal distance (Ma et al., 2011). If partner sex could be underlying sex differences in outcome evaluation, we expect to observe sex differences in P300 responses to outcomes, which were modulated by sex of partner.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Forty students (mean age 20.5 ± 1.8 years, range 19–24; 20 males) were recruited from the local college. A male participant’s data was deleted, because his EEG is distorted. All participants were right-handed and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. They were in good health with no previous history of organic brain disorders, and all were experimentally naïve. The experiments were approved by the Henan University Ethics board, and all subjects signed an informed consent form. Subjects were compensated for their participation. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Experimental Task Protocol

The joint task consisted of a computerized presentation of a “Chuck-A-Luck” dice game. One 6-sided dice and a blank space representing a second dice are presented to the subject on the computer screen. The subject then attempts to guess whether the result of the second dice being thrown will cause a “win” or a “loss”. If the sum of the two dice on the screen is greater than or equal to 7, the result is a win; less than 7 is a loss. The win/loss result is presented prior to revealing the second dice result. The presentation of stimuli was developed and controlled using E-prime 2.0 professional (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., United States). All images were presented in the white central region of the computer screen on a neutral gray background. Participants were seated in a quiet and electromagnetically shielded room approximately 1 m from a computer screen with the horizontal and vertical visual angles below 5°.

The experiment consisted of three blocks, one solo block (see Figure 1A) and two partner blocks (see Figure 1B). At the beginning of each trial participants were told this task was a simple dice game, and they were to use their index fingers to press the “F” and “J” keys for the left and right frames, respectively. The fixation point was presented in the center of the screen, and participants were told to press F when the frames appeared on the screen. The frames then disappeared, and shortly thereafter a random dice appeared in the left frame and the participants were told to press J. Then the screen blanked again, followed by the appearance of a question mark in the right frame. During this period the participant used the given dice roll to evaluate the probable outcome. After a blank screen, a score was flashed on the screen: “+50” if the result was a win, and “-50” if a loss. Feedback (the score) continued for 800 ms, then after another blank screen, the outcome (the pair of dice) was shown. The screen was blanked and the next trial begun.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. (A) Schematic of a trial in the solo block. (B) Schematic of a trial within the partner blocks.

For the partner blocks, the participants were seated in the laboratory as instructed by the experimenter. The participants were told prior to beginning the trials that this experiment would be completed together with an unfamiliar partner (the lab assistant). For each trial, the partner would tell the participant that the partner should act first, then press F to begin the dice rolling. When the left frame dice was presented, the participant would press J as described above, and the trial would proceed as described above for the solo block.

Before formal experiment, each participant was given 200 Yuan Token money as the initial funds. According to the results of each trial (lose/win), the number of tokens owned by the subject will be reduced or increased. After the experiment, we exchange subjects’ tokens for cash in appropriate proportions as a reward for participating in the experiment.

Data Acquisition and Analysis

The EEG was acquired using an elastic cap wired with Ag/AgCl electrodes in the extended 10/20 electrode system, plus two electrodes reserved for electro-oculography (EOG) placed above the left and right eyes. The reference electrodes were behind the right and left mastoid, while the ground electrode was at the midpoint between FP2 and F2. Electrode impedance was kept below 5 KΩ through the experiment. The bandpass was filtered to 0.01–30 Hz, and the sampling rate was 500 Hz. Data were recorded and analyzed via Brain Products ERP (Brain Products GmbH, Germany). Epoch length for offline analysis was 1200 ms including baseline correction; epochs extended 200 ms before stimulus presentation, and continued 1000 ms after presentation. Artifacts from muscle activity with an amplitude ±80 μV or greater were deleted in processing.

Statistics

Event-related potential values were imported to SPSS 20.0 (IBM, United States). The FRN amplitude can be calculated in two ways, applying grand-averaged waveforms or computing a difference wave (dFRN) between loss and gain trials within 250–350 ms at F3, Fz, F4, FC1, and FC2. And P3 was measured in 350–450 ms at P3, Pz, P4, CP1, and CP2. Two within-subjects factors were used in our studies: joint task type (three levels: alone, same sex, and opposite sex), result (two levels: win and loss).

Results

FRN Analysis

The main effects of Electrode (F(4,34) = 18.682, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.336) was significant. Further comparison found that FRN reached a maximum amplitude appeared at frontal lobe. The interaction effect of Task type, Result, Electrode and gender were significant, F(8,30) = 2.710, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.068. The simple effect analysis revealed that FRN amplitude of same condition was significantly greater than alone condition for male when the result was loss. On electrode F3, F(2,17) = 5.61, p < 0.01. On electrode F4, F(2,17) = 3.23, p = 0.051. On electrode Fz, F(2,17) = 4.09, p < 0.05. On electrode FC1, F(2,17) = 4.56, p < 0.05, no significant was found on FC2. However, FRN amplitude of opposite condition was significantly greater than alone condition for female when the result was loss. On electrode Fz, F(2,18) = 3.82, p < 0.05. On electrode FC1, F(2,18) = 3.14, p = 0.055. No significant was found on F3, F4 and FC2. No gender differences were found, F = 0.454, p = 0.505 (see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. The FRN of male/female under the condition of different joint task in loss/win.

Previous research suggests additional useful information can be obtained by analyzing the amplitude differentials (Holroyd and Krigolson, 2007; Li et al., 2010), so we analyzed dFRN between the gain and loss conditions via a repeated measures 3 × 5 (Task type × Electrode conditions, see above) ANOVA. The interaction effect of task type, Electrode and gender were significant, F(8,30) = 2.710, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.068. Further analysis found no significant difference. And no gender differences were found, F = 0.145, p = 0.706. See Tables 1, 2 and Figure 3.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Results of FRN Analysis.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Results of dFRN Analysis.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. The dFRN of male/female in different joint task conditions.

P300 Analysis

The main effects of Electrode (F(4,34) = 15.368, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.293) was significant. Further comparison found that P3 reached a maximum amplitude appeared at parietal lobe. The Task type × Result interaction was significant, F(2,36) = 4.309, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.104. Further simple effect analysis indicated that result of loss induced greater P300 than gain in alone condition, F(1,37) = 3.783, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.093. This effect was absent in joint conditions (same and opposite). In addition, no other main effects and interactions were significant. No gender differences were found, F = 1.088, p = 0.304. See Table 3 and Figure 4.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Results of P3 Analysis.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. The P3 of male/female under the condition of different joint task in loss/win.

Discussion

The main result of our experiment is that, for male, the FRN amplitude of same sex condition was significantly greater than alone condition when the result was loss; For female, FRN amplitude of opposite sex condition was significantly greater than alone condition when the result was loss. The data also showed that result of loss induced greater P300 than gain in alone condition. Although the main effects of sex and task context were absent, our results still show that the amplitudes of the FRN and P300 associated with outcome evaluation were partially modulated by partner’s sex and joint task context. Next, we would explain our findings and discuss their implications by comparing these findings with relevant studies.

Our first mainly result was that: For male, the FRN amplitude of same sex condition was significantly greater than alone condition when the result was loss. For female, FRN amplitude of opposite sex condition was significantly greater than alone condition when the result was loss. These findings could be explained in terms of more strong social stress induced by male partner. For a male participant, his same-sex partner was a male, while her opposite-sex was also a male for a female participant. Take together, male partner have shown a stronger social impact both for men and women. There is one possibility that male partner is usually regarded as more powerful than female. However, this explanation should be taken with caution, as there is no direct evidence for this assertion.

Our second finding was that the P300 effect was modulated by task context and the result feedback. The stronger P300 responses following loss feedback compared with win feedback, however, this P300 effect only reach significant in alone condition, and disappeared in joint task conditions (both same and opposite). Given that the P300 is generally thought to be related to processes of attentional allocation or motivational evaluation (Yeung and Sanfey, 2004; Wu and Zhou, 2009; Leng and Zhou, 2010). Our experiments may reflect that difference in motivation between alone condition and joint condition. Responsibility can drive motivation as well. Li et al. (2010) found that a high sense of responsibility was correlated with a larger P300. Nevertheless, Tian et al. (2013) found that electrical components induced by evaluation of results when other people are present could generate larger ERPs than the same evaluation when completing the task alone. Our findings were partially in line with Li et al. (2010), result. This may be the result of motivation being decreased simply by the presence of another.

Besides, we did not find the main effect of task context on the FRN. More specifically, the amplitude of FRN is not affected by the social context in which individuals performed a game with partners. Our result is not consistent with previous studies that smaller FRN effect was found in multiple players joint task (Li et al., 2010; Zhou et al., 2010). The absence of a significant FRN effect in joint task context may reflect the fact that real partners were used in our experiment, whereas just imagined or virtual partners were used in previous studies. The difference of partners could account for the absent FRN effect.

Taken together, during a joint task women and men performed a task with women or men partner to complete a common goal, men partners had more important impacts on their earlier processing of outcome evaluation, which was reflected that FRN effect was larger for men partner condition. However, in a late P300 component, evaluating gambling performance was attenuated by joint task context, and this P300 effect was independent of sex of individual and their partners.

Conclusion

These findings suggest that an earlier and a later brain responses in outcome evaluation may be modulated by partner’s sex and partner self. Specifically, partner’s sex play a role in the earlier feedback-monitor stage, and partner effect occurs in the late attention-sensitive stage. However, because of joint tasks’ limitation, we can not investigate the real interaction between individual and partners, the most classic ultimatum game may should be taken in future studies. Besides, because of EEG techniques’ limitation, the neural activities were not tracked deeply, FMRI technique would be a good choice in our future studies.

Author Contributions

EZ and JL designed this study, drafted the manuscript and revised the manuscript. ZJ performed the study and analyzed the data. SW and MY re-conducted the study and analyzed the relevant data.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Education Science Project under Grant BBA130014, Soft Science Project of Henan Province under Grant 122400450005, Key Project of Science and Technology Research of Henan Provincial Department of Education under Grant 14A190003, Key Scientific Research Project for Universities in Henan Province under Grant 16A190001, and the National Natural Science Fund under Grant 31700952.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Balliet, D., Li, N. P., Macfarlan, S. J., and Van Vugt, M. (2011). Sex differences in cooperation: a meta-analytic review of social dilemmas. Psychol. Bull. 137, 881–909. doi: 10.1037/a0025354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boksem, M. A., Kostermans, E., and de Cremer, D. (2011). Failing where others have succeeded: medial frontal negativity tracks failure in a social context. Psychophysiology 48, 973–979. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01163.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, X., Li, X., and Hu, Y. (2015). Synchronous brain activity during cooperative exchange depends on gender of partner: a fNIRS-based hyperscanning study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 2039–2048. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, C., Deshpande, G., Liu, C., Gu, R., Luo, Y. J., and Krueger, F. (2016). Diffusion of responsibility attenuates altruistic punishment: a functional magnetic resonance imaging effective connectivity study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 663–677. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hajcak, G., Holroyd, C. B., Moser, J. S., and Simons, R. F. (2005). Brain potentials associated with expected and unexpected good and bad outcomes. Psychophysiology 42, 161–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00278.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirnstein, M., Andrews, L. C., and Hausmann, M. (2014). Gender-stereotyping and cognitive sex differences in mixed- and same-sex groups. Arch. Sex. Behav. 43, 1663–1673. doi: 10.1007/s10508-014-0311-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holroyd, C. B., and Krigolson, O. E. (2007). Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task. Psychophysiology 44, 913–917. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00561.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamm, C., Decety, J., and Singer, T. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54, 2492–2502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leng, Y., and Zhou, X. (2010). Modulation of the brain activity in outcome evaluation by interpersonal relationship: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia 48, 448–455. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, P., Jia, S., Feng, T., Liu, Q., Suo, T., and Li, H. (2010). The influence of the diffusion of responsibility effect on outcome evaluations: electrophysiological evidence from an ERP study. Neuroimage 52, 1727–1733. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lithari, C., Frantzidis, C. A., Papadelis, C., Vivas, A. B., Klados, M. A., Kourtidou-Papadeli, C., et al. (2010). Are females more responsive to emotional stimuli? A neurophysiological study across arousal and valence dimensions. Brain Topogr. 23, 27–40. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0130-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, Y. J., Sun, S. Y., Mai, X. Q., Gu, R. L., and Zhang, H. J. (2011). Outcome Evaluation in Decision Making: ERP Studies. Culture and Neural Frames of Cognition and Communication. Berlin: Springer.

Google Scholar

Ma, Q., Shen, Q., Xu, Q., Li, D., Shu, L., and Weber, B. (2011). Empathic responses to others’ gains and losses: an electrophysiological investigation. Neuroimage 54, 2472–2480. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maccoby, E. E., and Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The Psychology of Sex Differences. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.

Google Scholar

Mercadillo, R. E., Díaz, J. L., Pasaye, E. H., and Barrios, F. A. (2011). Perception of suffering and compassion experience: brain gender disparities. Brain Cogn. 76, 5–14. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miltner, W. H. R., Braun, C. H., and Coles, M. G. H. (1997). Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation task: evidence for a “generic” neural system for error detection. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 788–798. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieuwenhuis, S., Slagter, H. A., von Geusau, N. J., Heslenfeld, D. J., and Holroyd, C. B. (2005). Knowing good from bad: differential activation of human cortical areas by positive and negative outcomes. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 3161–3168. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04152.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oswald, D. L., Clark, E. M., and Kelly, C. M. (2004). Friendship maintenance: an analysis of individual and dyad behaviors. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 23, 413–441. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.3.413.35460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, S.-Y., and Luo, Y.-J. (2008). Feedback-related negativity in outcome evaluation with a deception task: feedback-related negativity in outcome evaluation with a deception task. Acta Psychol. Sin. 40, 693–700. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2008.00693

CrossRef Full Text

Tian, T., Chun, L., Feng, X., Gu, R., and Luo, Y. (2013). “The presence of others modulates the outcome evaluation in a guessing game: An ERP Study,” in Proceedings of the 16th National Academic Congress of Psychology, Nanjing, 444–445.

Yeung, N., Hilroyd, C. B., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). ERP correlates of feedback and reward processing in the presence and absence of response choice. Cereb. Cortex 15, 535–554. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, N., and Sanfey, A. G. (2004). Independent coding of reward magnitude and valence in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 24, 625–626. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4537-03.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yilei, F. U., Luo, Y., and Fang, C. (2017). Consistency of choice modulates outcome evaluation: evidence from ERP studies. Acta Psychol. Sin. 49:1089. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.01089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, B., Roberts, K., Zhang, Z., Yu, X., Simons, R. F., and Wang, Y. (2012). Interpersonal conflicts influenced outcome evaluation in chicken game: an ERP study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 85:426. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.07.161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Y., and Zhou, X. (2009). The p300 and reward valence, magnitude, and expectancy in outcome evaluation. Brain Res. 1286, 114–122. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Z., Yu, R., and Zhou, X. (2010). To do or not to do? Action enlarges the FRN and p300 effects in outcome evaluation. Neuropsychologia 48, 3606–3613. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social context, partner sex, outcome evaluation, sex differences, ERP

Citation: Liu J, Wang S, Jia Z, Zhang E and Yao M (2018) Outcome Evaluation in Social Context Measured by Event-Related Potentials Is Partially Dependent on the Partner’s Sex. Front. Psychol. 9:853. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00853

Received: 17 November 2017; Accepted: 11 May 2018;
Published: 20 June 2018.

Edited by:

Francesca Marina Bosco, Università degli Studi di Torino, Italy

Reviewed by:

Alexander Soutschek, Universität Zürich, Switzerland
Daniela Rabellino, University of Western Ontario, Canada

Copyright © 2018 Liu, Wang, Jia, Zhang and Yao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Entao Zhang, zhanget2011@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.