Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 15 June 2018
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Commentary: Are groups more or less than the sum of their members? The moderating role of individual identification

  • 1Department of Psychology, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou, China
  • 2Research Centre for Brain Function and Psychological Science, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
  • 3Department of Psychology, Utah State University, Logan, UT, United States
  • 4Shenzhen Institute of Neuroscience, Shenzhen, China
  • 5Institute of Affective and Social Neuroscience, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China

A commentary on
Are groups more or less than the sum of their members? The moderating role of individual identification

by Baumeister, R. F., Ainsworth, S. E., and Vohs, K. D. (2016). Behav. Brain Sci. 39, 1–56. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X15000618

Baumeister et al. (2016) proposed that people perform better in groups only “when members of the group are individually identified and responsible” (p. 2), and conversely, that people perform worse in groups when they “are not publicly identified or rewarded” (p. 2). In other words, they emphasized how individual responsibility contributes to group success. However, we argue that shared identity, whereby group members share a common responsibility, can also facilitate group success in many circumstances, and thus should not be discounted. Several authors have shared the same view in the open peer commentary published in Behavioral and Brain Sciences about the ideas of Baumeister et al. (e.g., Budescu and Maciejovsky, 2016; Haslam and Ellemers, 2016; Nijstad and De Dreu, 2016). These authors had noted the special role of shared identity or having a common goal in facilitating bonds between members. Consistent with this, Ein-Dor and Hirschberger (2016) show how forming a cohesive group is a prerequisite for whether differentiation can have its maximal effect on group success. We argue two additional factors ignored by Baumeister et al. influence the effect of individual identity on group success: cultural differences and task characteristics.

We hold that the ideas of Baumeister et al. do not generalize beyond individualistic cultures, and do not generalize to all kinds of tasks. We begin by describing important differences between individualistic and collectivist cultures. Later, we distinguish between survival and development tasks. Ultimately, we argue that the benefit of individuation in groups may only apply to a small portion of the space defined by individualistic vs. collectivist cultures, and survival vs. development tasks, as schematically depicted in Figure 1. By contrast, the benefit of shared identity applies to any survival task, and to any task undertaken within a collectivist culture.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The benefit of differentiated identity over shared identity to group success is limited to specific tasks and cultures. ST denotes survival tasks; DT denotes developmental tasks; IC denotes individualistic culture; CC denotes collectivistic culture. The benefit of differentiated identities (vs. shared identity) is confined to developmental tasks in individualistic cultures (yellow area).

Baumeister et al. (2016) ignore culture as a factor that influences how people perform in groups. Previous work has highlighted the prominent differences between individualistic and collectivistic cultures where self-identity is concerned: whereas individual identity is emphasized in individualistic cultures, shared identity is emphasized in collectivistic cultures. According to the self-construal theory of individualism and collectivism (Triandis et al., 1988; Markus and Kitayama, 1991; Chiao et al., 2009; Saad et al., 2015), there are basic differences in how people construe themselves and their relationships between individualistic and collectivistic cultures. For example, in a task where subjects are supposed to draw themselves within a group, members of individualistic cultures draw themselves bigger than others in the group, whereas members of collectivist cultures draw themselves equal or even smaller than others in the group (Kitayama et al., 2009; Talhelm et al., 2014). Generally, people in individualistic cultures have independent self-construal such as autonomy and distinctiveness, and competitiveness/conflict is encouraged. In contrast, people in collectivistic cultures have interdependent self-construal that is largely defined by their surrounding context and relationships: sharing, cooperation, and group harmony are most important. These differences are especially relevant to group dynamics and relationships within groups. For example, reaction to honesty or deception is tempered by the intimacy of the relationship more so in collectivist cultures than in individualistic cultures (Wang et al., 2011). Neural data also supports differences in self-construal between collectivist and individualistic cultures. Collectivists have a shared neural representation of self and close family members (e.g., mother) whereas individualists do not (Zhu et al., 2007), In addition, Chiao et al. (2009) showed that medial prefrontal cortex activation is greater for collectivists in response to contextual self-description (shared identity), but is greater for individualists in response to general self-description (individual identity).

We agree with the view that for many types of tasks, the promotion of individual identity can facilitate group success, whereas promoting shared identity can reduce success. However, we argue that this result applies most strongly to individualistic cultures, and may not be observed at all when testing collectivist cultures (also pointed out by Brown, 2016). More crucially, we argue that the role of perceived identity in group success is modulated by culture. Specifically, the promotion of individual identity can facilitate group success for individualists but not collectivists. In contrast, the promotion of shared identity can bolster group work for collectivists but not for individualist. There is ample evidence for this position. First, individualists and collectivists show the opposite pattern of performance when working in groups vs. when working alone. For example, individualists are more likely to exhibit social loafing (exerting less effort when in a group) whereas collectivists are more likely to exert more effort in a group (Earley, 1989, 1993; Karau and Williams, 1993; Wagner, 1995). Second, but more important, the effects of individualism on group performance is typically driven by criticism, competitiveness, and the desire to be unique, whereas the effect of collectivism on group performance is driven by cooperation (Tjosvold et al., 2003; Goncalo and Staw, 2006). As such, recent evidence shows that priming collectivist values facilitates team ideation by promoting cooperation (Ye and Robert, 2017). Furthermore, criticism improves performance on creative brainstorming tasks for members of individualistic cultures (Nemeth et al., 2004; Saad et al., 2015), but criticism does not benefit members of collectivist cultures (Saad et al., 2015).

Baumeister et al. (2016) also ignore how task characteristics can modulate performance in groups. We note a critical distinction between survival tasks and development tasks. Survival tasks refer to the struggle to remain living by securing sufficient food and shelter, and avoiding predators etc. (Buss, 2008). In terms of group goals, survival tasks are tasks directed at preventing the group from shrinking. In contrast, development tasks are directed at making the group thrive and grow. By our definition, the sorts of creativity tasks reviewed by Baumeister et al. constitute development tasks. However, Baumeister et al. implicitly generalize their ideas to any type of task directed toward group goals. They do allude to Tuckman's (1965) “group forming” stage, in which the benefits of shared group identity are highlighted, but Baumeister et al. (2016) diminish the importance of these findings as they relate to group success, relegating them to the “aegis of social identity theory” (p. 3). They go on to espouse the benefit of group differentiation, implicitly linking it to all other circumstances, ignoring that survival tasks are a central feature in the evolutionary history of humankind (Buss, 2008).

The probability of survival is increased by shared identity because shared identity promotes helping (Penner et al., 2005), loyalty (Van Vugt and Hart, 2004), contribution (De Cremer et al., 2008), and resource sharing (Tyler and Degoey, 1995), all of which facilitate group success for survival tasks. In contrast, development is essential for thriving beyond mere survival. Development requires diversity and differentiation. Most of the tasks addressed by Baumeister et al. (2016) do not involve survival tasks and thus can be labeled as development tasks. We agree that individuation largely facilitates group success for development tasks such as group performance on creativity tasks. In this vein, we argue that differentiated identities can contribute to group success for developmental tasks whereas shared identity has a larger influence on group success for survival tasks.

In short, Baumeister et al. (2016) have provided a valuable framework for understanding the way individuals contribute to group performance. We argue that the perceived benefit of individual vs. shared identity to group success is dependent on culture and task characteristics. We hope our argument contributes to a broader perspective on this issue.

Author Contributions

ZZ drafted the manuscript. CW provided revisions and language editing. YL, QX, and HL provided revisions.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31571153, 31571144, and 31671150); the Guangdong province colleges and universities innovation team construction project (2015KCXTD009); the Shenzhen Fundamental Research Projects (JCYJ20150729104249783); the “12th Five-Year plan” of Guangzhou Education Science (1201421342); the Shenzhen Peacock Plan (KQTD2015033016104926) and the Guangdong Pearl River Talents Plan Innovative and Entrepreneurial Team grant (2016ZT06S220).

References

Baumeister, R. F., Ainsworth, S. E., and Vohs, K. D. (2016). Are groups more or less than the sum of their members? The moderating role of individual identification. Behav. Brain Sci. 39, 1–56. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X15000618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. (2016). Social, not individual, identification is the key to understanding group phenomena. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e143. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X15001314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Budescu, D., and Maciejovsky, B. (2016). The subtle effects of incentives and competition on group performance. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e144. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X15001326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, D. M. (2008). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind. Boston, MA: Omegatype Typography, Inc.

Google Scholar

Chiao, J. Y., Harada, T., Komeda, H., Li, Z., Mano, Y., Saito, D., et al. (2009). Neural basis of individualistic and collectivistic views of self. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2813–2820. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Cremer, D., Van Knippenberg, D., Van Dijk, E., and Van Leeuwen, E. (2008). Cooperating if one's goals are collective-based: social identification effects in social dilemmas as a function of goal transformation. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol.38, 1562–1579. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00359.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Earley, P. C. (1989). Social loafing and collectivism: a comparison of the United States and the People's Republic of China. Administr. Sci. Q. 34, 565–581. doi: 10.2307/2393567

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Earley, P. C. (1993). East meets west meets mideast: further explorations of collectivistic and individualistic work groups. Acad. Manag. J. 36, 319–348.

Google Scholar

Ein-Dor, T., and Hirschberger, G. (2016). Rethinking attachment theory: from a theory of relationships to a theory of individual and group survival. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 25, 223–227. doi: 10.1177/0963721416650684

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goncalo, J. A., and Staw, B. M. (2006). Individualism–collectivism and group creativity. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decision Process. 100, 96–109. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haslam, S. A., and Ellemers, N. (2016). Social identification is generally a prerequisite for group success and does not preclude intragroup differentiation. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e150. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X15001387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karau, S. J., and Williams, K. D. (1993). Social loafing: a meta-analytic review and theoretical integration. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 65:681. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.681

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A. T., Karasawa, M., and Uskul, A. K. (2009). A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: comparing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 97, 236–255. doi: 10.1037/a0015999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nemeth, C. J., Personnaz, B., Personnaz, M., and Goncalo, J. A. (2004). The liberating role of conflict in group creativity: a study in two countries. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 34, 365–374. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijstad, B. A., and De Dreu, C. K. W. (2016). Differentiated selves help only when identification is strong and tasks are complex. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e160. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X1500148X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., and Schroeder, D. A. (2005). Prosocial behavior: multilevel perspectives. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 56, 365–392. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saad, G., Cleveland, M., and Ho, L. (2015). Individualism–collectivism and the quantity versus quality dimensions of individual and group creative performance. J. Business Res. 68, 578–586. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talhelm, T., Zhang, X., Oishi, S., Shimin, C., Duan, D., Lan, X., et al. (2014). Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science 344, 603–608. doi: 10.1126/science.1246850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tjosvold, D., Law, K. S., and Sun, H. F. (2003). Collectivistic and individualistic values: their effects on group dynamics and productivity in china. Group Dec. Negot. 12, 243–263. doi: 10.1023/A:1023383200180

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C., Bontempo, R., Villareal, M. J., Asai, M., and Lucca, N. (1988). Individualism and collectivism: cross-cultural perspectives on self in group relationships. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 54, 323–338. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.2.323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuckman, B. W. (1965). Developmental sequence in small groups. Psychol. Bullet. 63, 384–399. doi: 10.1037/h0022100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, T. R., and Degoey, P. (1995). Collective restraint in social dilemmas: procedural justice and social identification effects on support for authorities. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 69, 482–497.

Google Scholar

Van Vugt, M., and Hart, C. M. (2004). Social identity as social glue: the origins of group loyalty. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 86, 585–598. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.4.585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, J. A. (1995). Studies of individualism–collectivism: effects on cooperation in groups. Acad. Manag. J. 38, 152–172. doi: 10.5465/256731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C. S., Leung, A. K.-Y., See, Y. H. M., and Gao, X. Y. (2011). The effects of culture and friendship on rewarding honesty and punishing deception. J. Exp.Soc. Psychol. 47, 1295–1299. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.04.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, T., and Robert, L. P. (2017). “Does collectivism inhibit individual creativity?: The effects of collectivism and perceived diversity on individual creativity and satisfaction in virtual ideation teams,” in ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work and Social Computing (New York, NY: ACM), 2344–2358. doi: 10.1145/2998181.2998261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, J., and Han, S. (2007). Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation. Neuroimage 34, 1310–1316. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: individuation, shared identity, culture, task characteristics, group success

Citation: Zhang Z, Warren CM, Lei Y, Xing Q and Li H (2018) Commentary: Are groups more or less than the sum of their members? The moderating role of individual identification. Front. Psychol. 9:999. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00999

Received: 14 February 2018; Accepted: 28 May 2018;
Published: 15 June 2018.

Edited by:

Tsachi Ein-Dor, Interdisciplinary Center Herzliya, Israel

Reviewed by:

Sarah E. Gaither, Duke University, United States

Copyright © 2018 Zhang, Warren, Lei, Xing and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yi Lei, leiyi821@vip.sina.com
Qiang Xing, qiang_xingpsy@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.