Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 15 June 2018
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Lexical Tone Perception in Infants and Young Children: Empirical studies and theoretical perspectives View all 23 articles

PRIMIR on Tone

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
  • 2Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

Processing Rich Information from Multidimensional Interactive Representations (PRIMIR) was first developed to address divergent findings in the literature surrounding early speech perception and word learning (Werker and Curtin, 2005; Curtin and Werker, 2007). Specifically, there was a controversy in the literature at the time indicating that while under some circumstances infants and toddlers are able to discriminate a mispronunciation of a known word, in other testing circumstances, they had difficulty using phonetic/phonological differences to guide word learning. PRIMIR was developed as a framework for understanding why infants attend to some kinds of information over others in different testing situations. Integral to PRIMIR are the representations and processing that simultaneously impact how the infant interprets the information in the signal. PRIMIR assumes infants have access to a rich signal containing multi-sensory information. It further assumes that infants have initial biases that work together with learning mechanisms (e.g., statistical, comparison/contrast) to help process, organize, and store the information gleaned from the signal. Information is stored within emergent interactive representational planes: general perceptual, word form, and phoneme, and is processed using three dynamic filters: initial biases (e.g., preferences for speech, native-language rhythm, and infant-directed speech), the developmental level of the infant, and the task that the child is facing (e.g., discriminating sounds, learning words). While the initial biases draw the infant's attention to the linguistic signal, the developmental level and the task will influence what information is attended to, stored, and used at any given time. That is, the states of the infant's various representations and the task itself will shift how the infant interprets the information. With this in mind, rather than clear-cut developmental boundaries of when an infant might show evidence of acquiring any particular linguistic ability, the process is more multiplexed, with the use of some types of information potentiated only once the relevant representations are in place. While the input that the infant has access to is rich, some aspects of the signal are inherently more salient than others, and some of those properties that are perhaps not as inherently salient can become enhanced through experience, development, and their contribution to category formation. In other words, some sound properties have raw acoustic salience, while others require converging sources of evidence (be it multi-sensory or contextual) to be processed, discriminated, or learned. Tone is a paradigmatic example of how acoustic salience (pitch), and the representational structures that are in place influence its processing.

In considering the interface of initial perceptual biases, the extraction of word forms and connecting them to concepts, and the emergence of phonemes, PRIMIR initially focused on segments as both carriers of linguistic contrast (e.g., /dag/ “dog” vs. /bag/ “bog”) and of indexical cues, such as talker and affect. However, pitch is perhaps an even more telling example of a perceptual property that can both be implemented as a tone (carried on vowels, voiced/sonorant consonants, or syllables) to designate and contrast meaning as well as an indexical signal of an array of functions from focus to emotional valence.The range of articles in this special issue eloquently describe these multiple functions and address the unique challenges for learners. We highlight some of these findings to illustrate how PRIMIR can help explain how infants simultaneously learn to use pitch for indexical information and tone for word learning.

Consistent with PRIMIR are a number of studies examining the discrimination and functional use of native and non-native tones. In some studies findings are consistent with how phonetic sensitivities develop with the general perceptual plane. For example, both babies growing up in tone and non-tone languages discriminate tones at a younger age (4–6 months), and stop doing so by 9-months unless they grow up in a tone language (e.g., Mattock and Burnham, 2006; Yeung et al., 2013; Liu and Kager, 2014). However, also consistent with PRIMIR there are variations in performance as a function of the discrimination procedure (Gotz et al., this issue), and as a function of the acoustic salience of the tones being compared (Chen et al., this issue; Cheng and Lee, this issue; Tsao, this issue). A finding in the tone literature that uniquely supports PRIMIR, is that even in the cases where there is a decline in non-native tone discrimination between 6- and 9-months, there is a rebound in discrimination by 18-months (Gotz et al., this issue; Liu and Kager, this issue) but an inability to use these (non-native) tone distinctions to guide word learning at 18 months (Burnham et al., this issue; Liu and Kager, this issue). These findings reveal simultaneous access to multiple representational planes. Importantly, within the PRIMIR framework, emergent phonemes help to direct information about lexical processing around 18 months of age. Thus, while infants of this age can still access acoustic information contained within the General Perceptual plane and direct attention in a discrimination task (as seen in the rebound in tone discrimination), the native phonological system constrains the mapping of non-native contrasts to distinct words further suggesting that by 18 months, infants treat lexical tones as phonemic elements. In our refocused version of PRIMIR (Curtin et al., 2011), we began to explore how bilingual infants' experiences with their dual language input may result in them approaching tasks differently than monolingual peers. We had not yet considered, however, that their dual language experience might boost their attention to acoustic and phonetic information in tone. Nor have we considered what type of dual language experience (e.g., tone/non-tone, two non-tone) might boost attention. Bilingual infants learning two non-tone languages show a rebound in tone discrimination 6 months earlier than monolingual infants (Liu and Kager, 2017). Bilingual English-Mandarin infants demonstrate an ability to use the acoustically salient Thai tone contrast in a word learning task while monolingual Mandarin infants do not (Burnham et al., this issue). The extent to which dual language experience shifts infants' use of the dynamic filters across the representational planes is an exciting new direction for the further development of PRIMIR.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

The writing of this article was supporting by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (grant 435-2017-0120 to SC and grant 435-2014-0917 to JW) and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (grant 327319-2012 to SC).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Curtin, S., Byers-Heinlein, K., and Werker, J. F. (2011). Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus. J. Phon. 39, 492–504. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2010.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtin, S., and Werker, J. F. (2007). “The perceptual foundations of phonological development,” in The Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics, ed G. Gaskell (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 579–599.

Google Scholar

Liu, L., and Kager, R. (2014). Perception of tones by infants learning a non-tone language. Cognition 133, 385–394. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, L., and Kager, R. (2017). Perception of tones by bilingual infants learning non-tone languages. Bilingual. Lang. Cogn. 20, 561–575. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattock, K., and Burnham, D. (2006). Chinese and English infants' tone perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization. Infancy 10, 241–265. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in1003_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werker, J. F., and Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Lang. Learn. Dev. 1, 197–234. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2005.9684216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, H. H., Chen, K. H., and Werker, J. F. (2013). When does native language input affect phonetic perception? The precocious case of lexical tone. J. Mem. Lang. 68, 123–139. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: PRIMIR, tone, speech perception, language acquisition, word learning, bilingualism, infancy

Citation: Curtin S and Werker JF (2018) PRIMIR on Tone. Front. Psychol. 9:1007. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01007

Received: 03 May 2018; Accepted: 30 May 2018;
Published: 15 June 2018.

Edited by:

Leher Singh, National University of Singapore, Singapore

Reviewed by:

René Kager, Utrecht University, Netherlands

Copyright © 2018 Curtin and Werker. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Suzanne Curtin, scurtin@ucalgary.ca
Janet F. Werker, jwerker@psych.ubc.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.