Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 17 August 2018
Sec. Comparative Psychology

Commentary: Intranasal Oxytocin Treatment Increases Eye-Gaze Behavior toward the Owner in Ancient Japanese Dog Breeds

  • 1Department of Clinical, Neuro & Developmental Psychology, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Clinical Child and Adolescent Studies-Neurodevelopmental Disorders, Faculty of Social Sciences, Leiden University, Leiden, Netherlands
  • 3Discipline of Pharmacology, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Adelaide, Adelaide, SA, Australia

A Commentary on
Intranasal Oxytocin Treatment Increases Eye-Gaze Behavior toward the Owner in Ancient Japanese Dog Breeds

by Nagasawa, M., Ogawa, M., Mogi, K., and Kikusui, T. (2017). Front. Psychol. 8:1624. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01624

Oxytocin in mutually-gazing humans and dogs: Rewarding or stress bonding?

Nagasawa et al. (2017) build on their previous research demonstrating a positive loop between mutual eye-gazing of European dog breeds and owners and increased oxytocin during interaction (Nagasawa et al., 2015). They now investigated whether this positive loop is present during interactions between humans and ancient Japanese dog breeds. These dogs are considered to be genetically closer to wolves and more aggressive. Nagasawa's team set to increase the stressfulness of the situation for the dogs by placing two strangers in the experimentation room with the dog and its owner. Additionally, owners and strangers were instructed not to talk to each other or to talk to and touch the dog on their initiative. Oxytocin treatment of the dogs enhanced the dogs' gazing behavior and increased their owners' urinary oxytocin levels; the longer the duration of touch, the lower the increase of oxytocin in owners. Surprisingly, psychophysiological measures suggested that interaction with oxytocin-treated dogs enhanced owner's tension/arousal. Based on these and previous results, Nagasawa et al. (2015, 2017) propose the existence of an oxytocin-mediated positive loop in human-dog relationships.

Nagasawa et al. (2017) proposed that this positive loop was due to social rewarding effects of oxytocin release as seen in mother-infant relations. We feel there are other valid explanations and interpretation of these findings and want to stress the importance of considering and testing alternative explanations. As highlighted by Rault et al. (2017), it is common place in oxytocin research to assume positive valence without controlling for negative valence and arousal. Additionally, it is surprising that there is a distinction between effect of increased dog gazing and increased touch on oxytocin levels in the owner. We want to draw attention to an alternative explanation for the Nagasawa and other findings, which centers on the role of oxytocin in habituation of threat responses.

We want to draw attention to findings that demonstrated that oxytocin may alter social attention by suppressing vigilance toward threatening social stimuli, which explains many of its observed prosocial effects. Moreover, altered social attention may be implicated in a role of oxytocin in social habituation processes, particularly in mammals, whereby an individual decreases their response to an unfamiliar, dominant, or emotional social stimulus after repeated exposure (Ebitz et al., 2013; Weitekamp et al., 2017). Habituation of threat responses may be a primal function of oxytocin in mother-infant and human-dog relations (Buttner, 2016). Dogs can pose a physical threat to people and are perceived as threatening by many, for example when they are unfamiliar or angry (ÖHman, 1986). Threat appraisals in dogs are likely to be activated during gazing, because dogs tend to use eye contact as a threat among conspecifics (Bradshaw and Nott, 1995). Similarly, socially anxious humans avoid direct gaze in their social interaction, which they may perceive as threatening (Roelofs et al., 2010). Moreover, dogs look referentially to the owner after looking at an unfamiliar object and show behavioral conditioning that mirrors the owner's facial and vocal emotional message (Merola et al., 2012). This data suggests the hypothesis that dogs and humans may still show context-dependent conditioned and unconditioned responses to each-other's gaze. Such threat appraisals may trigger oxytocin responses in an attempt to habituate stress responses to unfamiliar faces and eyes (Ebitz et al., 2013). Consistent with this function, administered oxytocin decreases unconscious vigilance to threat signals from the eye region (Ebitz et al., 2013; Kanat et al., 2015). Importantly, dogs given oxytocin specifically fixated less on angry faces (Kis et al., 2017) and on the eye region of angry faces while physiological arousal was lower (Somppi et al., 2017). This highlights that oxytocin both decreased arousal and reduced eye gazing toward the eyes of angry humans, which can suggest that eye gazing was used during perceived threat.

Research from animals and human studies suggest a primal function of oxytocin in threat perception, with oxytocin reducing aversive avoidance responses to newborns or potential partners (Insel and Young, 2001; Cong et al., 2015). Similarly, oxytocin facilitates relaxed physiological states during reproduction-related interactions/situations that may otherwise be perceived as physically threatening, such as birth, breast feeding, and consensual sexual behavior (Porges, 2003). Interestingly, mothers displayed higher oxytocin levels after close, physical interactions with previously unknown children than with their own children (Bick and Dozier, 2010). Such findings are consistent with an association of oxytocin with coping that requires formation of trust, which in turn facilitates social stress habituation (Kéri and Kiss, 2011; Kiss et al., 2011; Tops et al., 2013a,b cf. von Dawans et al., 2012). Effective coping supported by oxytocin release, which increases social support seeking, may subsequently counter-regulate the oxytocin response itself (Tops et al., 2013b). Voluntary touch may be an important aspect of effective coping of pet dogs and their owners during perceived threat, which can than result in a counter-regulation of oxytocin (Uvnäs-Moberg et al., 2015).

Oxytocin may facilitate attachment formation between humans and dogs through its function in threat-coping responses which increase trust (Tops et al., 2013a, 2014). A primal function of oxytocin in processes of social stress habituation may also facilitate attachment between humans and dogs that are seen as guards against threats from others. In line with a role of oxytocin in protective behavior and proximity seeking to protect others, humans show increased support seeking from high-threat allies (De Dreu et al., 2012). Additionally, intranasal oxytocin induces protective aggression and in-group vs. out-group favoritism (De Dreu, 2012). Importantly, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn (2017) highlight that human oxytocin studies often neglect to identify protection as an important dimension of parenting and mother-infant relations.

Studies which focus on identifying the role of oxytocin in social reward often neglect the potential role of stress habituation and do not collect measures to support or refute these distinct theories. Taking the results of Nagasawa et al. (2017) for example, it seems unlikely that the Japanese dogs after oxytocin treatment engaged in a threatening type of gazing such as staring at their owners. Still, the effects of oxytocin are believed to be context-dependent: the unfamiliar testing conditions involving two strangers and instructed inhibition of interactions were potentially stressful to both dogs and owners. The ambiguity of unnaturally suppressed interaction may have increased reassurance and protection seeking, and activation of unconditioned threat appraisals of gazing between dogs and humans.

Importantly, both threat response and positive valence may direct gazing behavior, but the current study does not let us distinguish the two. Moreover, oxytocin responses triggered by low-level threat and by social reward and protection in other contexts may still be implicated in beneficial effects of living in good relationships with dogs impacting on human well-being, coping and health (Julius et al., 2012; Kotrschal, 2016, 2018). Furthermore, oxytocin's role in stress coping responses and habituation may help explain seemingly inconsistent and context-dependent results in oxytocin research in general. For instance, several studies in humans found that intranasal oxytocin application produced different or even opposite effects depending on whether it was applied in novel or familiarized, habituated conditions (Tops, 2017). To fully understand the role of oxytocin in eye-gaze between humans and dogs, it is essential to more fully take into account valence, arousal, familiarity, and habituation (Tops et al., 2014; Rault et al., 2017).

Author Contributions

As the first author, MT was involved in all steps of the process, and was the primary writer of the text. SH and FB-P contributed to the write-up.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2017). Oxytocin and human sensitive and protective parenting. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 35, 421–448. doi: 10.1007/7854_2017_23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bick, J., and Dozier, M. (2010). Mothers' and children's concentrations of oxytocin following close, physical interactions with biological and non-biological children. Dev. Psychobiol. 52, 100–107.doi: 10.1002/dev.20411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradshaw, J. W. S., and Nott, H. M. R. (1995). “Social and communication behaviour of companion dogs,” in The Domestic Dog, its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People, ed J. Serpell (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 116–130.

Buttner, A. P. (2016). Neurobiological underpinnings of dogs' human-like social competence: how interactions between stress response systems and oxytocin mediate dogs' social skills. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 71, 198–214. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cong, X., Ludington-Hoe, S. M., Hussain, N., Cusson, R. M., Walsh, S., Vazquez, V., et al. (2015). Parental oxytocin responses during skin-to-skin contact in pre-term infants. Early Hum. Dev. 91, 401–406. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Dreu, C. K. (2012). Oxytocin modulates cooperation within and competition between groups: an integrative review and research agenda. Horm. Behav. 61, 419–428. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Dreu, C. K., Greer, L. L., Handgraaf, M. J., Shalvi, S., and Van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Oxytocin modulates selection of allies in intergroup conflict. Proc. Biol. Sci. 279, 1150–1154. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebitz, R. B., Watson, K. K., and Platt, M. L. (2013). Oxytocin blunts social vigilance in the rhesus macaque. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 11630–11635. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1305230110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T. R., and Young, L. J. (2001). The neurobiology of attachment. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 129–136. doi: 10.1038/35053579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Julius, H., Beetz, A., Kotrschal, K., Turner, D., and Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (2012). Attachment to Pets: An Integrative View of Human-Animal Relationships with Implications for Therapeutic Practice. Göttingen.

Kanat, M., Heinrichs, M., Mader, I., van Elst, L. T., and Domes, G. (2015). Oxytocin modulates amygdala reactivity to masked fearful eyes. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 2632–2638. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kéri, S., and Kiss, I. (2011). Oxytocin response in a trust game and habituation of arousal. Physiol. Behav. 102, 221–224. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kis, A., Hernádi, A., Miklósi, B., Kanizsár, O., and Topál, J. (2017). The way dogs (Canis familiaris) look at human emotional faces is modulated by oxytocin. An eye-tracking study. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11:210. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiss, I., Levy-Gigi, E., and Kéri, S. (2011). CD38 expression, attachment style and habituation of arousal in relation to trust-related oxytocin release. Biol. Psychol. 88, 223–226. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotrschal, K. (2016). “Do companion animals support social, emotional and cognitive development of children?,” in Social Neuroscience of Human-Animal Interaction, eds L. Freund, S. McCune, P. McCardle, L. Esposito, and N. Gee (Washington, DC: NIH-Waltham), 73–86.

Kotrschal, K. (2018). How wolves turned into dogs and how dogs are valuable in meeting human social needs. People Anim. Int. J. Res. Pract. 1:6. Available online at: https://docs.lib.purdue.edu/paij/vol1/iss1/6

Google Scholar

Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., and Marshall-Pescin, S. (2012). Social referencing in dog-owner dyads? Anim. Cogn. 15, 175–185. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0443-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagasawa, M., Mitsui, S., En, S., Ohtani, N., Ohta, M., Sakuma, Y., et al. (2015). Social evolution. Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science 348, 333–336. doi: 10.1126/science.1261022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagasawa, M., Ogawa, M., Mogi, K., and Kikusui, T. (2017). Intranasal oxytocin treatment increases eye-gaze behavior toward the owner in ancient Japanese dog breeds. Front. Psychol. 8:1624. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01624

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ÖHman, A. (1986). Face the beast and fear the face: animal and social fears as prototypes for evolutionary analyses of emotion. Psychophysiology 23, 123–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00608.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porges, S. W. (2003). Social engagement and attachment: a phylogenetic perspective. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1008, 31–47. doi: 10.1196/annals.1301.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rault, J. L., van den Munkhof, M., and Buisman-Pijlman, F. T. A. (2017). Oxytocin as an indicator of psychological and social well-being in domesticated animals: a critical review. Front. Psychol. 8:1521. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roelofs, K., Putman, P., Schouten, S., Lange, W. G., Volman, I., and Rinck, M. (2010). Gaze direction differentially affects avoidance tendencies to happy and angry faces in socially anxious individuals. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 290–294. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somppi, S., Törnqvist, H., Topál, J., Koskela, A., Hänninen, L., Krause, C. M., et al. (2017). Nasal oxytocin treatment biases dogs' visual attention and emotional response toward positive human facial expressions. Front. Psychol. 8:1854. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tops, M. (2017). Within-subject effects and the social habituation function of oxytocin. Horm. Behav. 96:1. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tops, M., Buisman-Pijlman, F. T. A., and Carter, C. S. (2013a). “Oxytocin and attachment facilitate a shift from seeking novelty to recognizing and preferring familiarity: The key to increasing resilience?,” in The Resilience Handbook: Approaches to Stress and Trauma, eds M. Kent, M. C. Davis, and J. W. Reich (New York, NY: Routledge Publishers), 115–130.

Tops, M., Huffmeijer, R., Linting, M., Grewen, K. M., Light, K. C., Koole, S. L., et al. (2013b). The role of oxytocin in familiarization-habituation responses to social novelty. Front. Psychol. 4:761. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tops, M., Koole, S. L., IJzerman, H., and Buisman-Pijlman, F. T. A. (2014). Why social attachment and oxytocin protect against addiction and stress: insights from the dynamics between ventral and dorsal corticostriatal systems. Pharm. Biochem. Behav. 119, 39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uvnäs-Moberg, K., Handlin, L., and Petersson, M. (2015). Self-soothing behaviors with particular reference to oxytocin release induced by non-noxious sensory stimulation. Front. Psychol. 5:1529. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Dawans, B., Fischbacher, U., Kirschbaum, C., Fehr, E., and Heinrichs, M. (2012). The social dimension of stress reactivity: acute stress increases prosocial behavior in humans. Psychol. Sci. 23, 651–660. doi: 10.1177/0956797611431576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weitekamp, C. A., Solomon-Lane, T. K., Del Valle, P., Triki, Z., Nugent, B. M., and Hofmann, H. A. (2017). A role for oxytocin-like receptor in social habituation in a teleost. Brain Behav. Evol. 89, 153–161. doi: 10.1159/000464098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: oxytocin, habituation, familiarization, human-dog bond, stress coping, threat response, protection, social attachment

Citation: Tops M, Huijbregts SCJ and Buisman-Pijlman FTA (2018) Commentary: Intranasal Oxytocin Treatment Increases Eye-Gaze Behavior toward the Owner in Ancient Japanese Dog Breeds. Front. Psychol. 9:1473. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01473

Received: 19 October 2017; Accepted: 26 July 2018;
Published: 17 August 2018.

Edited by:

Debbie M. Kelly, University of Manitoba, Canada

Reviewed by:

Kurt Kotrschal, Universität Wien, Austria
Sylvain Fiset, Université de Moncton, Canada

Copyright © 2018 Tops, Huijbregts and Buisman-Pijlman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mattie Tops, m.tops@vu.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.