Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 29 August 2018
Sec. Educational Psychology

The Big-Fish-Little-Pond Effect on Academic Self-Concept: A Meta-Analysis

\r\nJunyan FangJunyan FangXitong HuangXitong HuangMinqiang Zhang*Minqiang Zhang*Feifei HuangFeifei HuangZhe LiZhe LiQiting YuanQiting Yuan
  • Scool of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China

The Big-fish-little-Pond effect is well acknowledged as the negative effect of class/school average achievement on student academic self-concept, which profoundly impacts student academic performance and mental development. Although a few studies have been done with regard to this effect, inconsistence exists in the effect size with little success in finding moderators. Here, we present a meta-analysis to synthesize related literatures to reach a summary conclusion on the BFLPE. Furthermore, student age, comparison target, academic self-concept domain, student location, sample size, and publication year were examined as potential moderators. Thirty-three studies with fifty-six effect sizes (total N = 1,276,838) were finally included. The random effects model led to a mean of the BFLPE at β = −0.28 (p < 0.001). Moreover, moderator analyses revealed that the Big-Fish-Little-Pond effect is an age-based process and an intercultural phenomenon, which is stronger among high school students, in Asia and when verbal self-concept is considered. This meta-analysis is the first quantitative systematic overview of BFLPE, whose results are valuable to the understanding of BFLPE and reveal the necessity for educators from all countries to learn about operative means to help students avoid the potential negative effect. Future research expectations are offered subsequently.

Introduction

In educational psychology, Academic Self-Concept (ASC) refers to students' self-perception in specific disciplines (e.g., math self-concept, science self-concept) or more general academic areas (i.e., global/general ASC) (Marsh et al., 2008a). As a prominent construct in educational psychology, student ASC showed substantial positive relations with many desirable educational outcomes, such as academic effort (Traütwein et al., 2006), academic interest and long-term educational attainment (Marsh et al., 2005, 2007; Pinxten et al., 2010). Earlier empirical researches and a meta-analysis manifested that academic achievement and ASC are reciprocally related (Guay et al., 2003; Valentine and Dubois, 2005; Marsh and Craven, 2006). Positive ASC is an important means of facilitating student academic accomplishments and has been regarded as one of the key objectives of education (Seaton et al., 2009), therefore delving into the ASC forming process and revealing the forming mechanism make an impact both academically and practically.

The Big Fish Little Pond Effect (BFLPE) is one of the most influential theories about student ASC forming process, which was proposed by Marsh (1984) to describe the phenomenon that students in selective schools always have lower ASC compared to those with comparable ability but attend regular schools, which means that being a big fish in a small pond does good to one's ASC. Considerable evidence substantiated that the BFLPE is thought to be the outcome of individuals comparing their ability with the average ability of their group (Marsh, 1987; Plieninger and Dickhäuser, 2015).

It has been demonstrated that student's ASC is shaped not only by his or her performance but also by social comparisons (Marsh, 1988; Marsh et al., 1995; Möller et al., 2009; Parker et al., 2013; Niepel et al., 2014). Students compare their own achievement with that of their class- or schoolmates, which leads them to feel more negative about their own competencies in high-achieving atmosphere than in low-achieving atmosphere. Marsh (1987) argued that this social comparison mechanism lies at the heart of the BFLPE.

Evidence accumulated for several decades supported the BFLPE (Marsh and Hau, 2003; Huguet et al., 2009; Chiu, 2012; Becker and Neumann, 2016; Areepattamannil et al., 2017). The BFLPE was proved to be intercultural and stable: Marsh and Hau (2003) found that the effect of school-average achievement on student ASC is negative in 26 countries (β¯ = −0.20, SD = 0.08), and it exhibits across all student ability levels. Besides, the BFLPE was also observed for students who were at the end of high school or even graduated 2 years or 4 years later (Marsh et al., 2007), students with special education needs (Marsh and Craven, 2006), and students who were identified as gifted (Preckel et al., 2008).

While the BFLPE generally occurs, there are exceptions. Researches by Sung et al. (2014) and Liou (2014) provided evidence for no BFLPE. And results on the size of the BFLPE have been largely mixed. The size of this negative effect ranges from extremely weak (Thijs et al., 2010; Liou, 2014; Becker and Neumann, 2016), to weak (Nagengast and Marsh, 2012; Marsh, 2016) and to moderate (Huguet et al., 2009; Chiu, 2012). These inconsistencies in the reported findings make it difficult to draw a general conclusion concerning the BFLPE and provide useful suggestions for educational practice. As it usually makes more sense to summary existing researches than doing further research (Card, 2012), it is of great importance to carry out a systematic review of the BFLPE. While Marsh et al. (2008b) have summarized the theoretical model underlying the BFLPE, there still lacks quantitative summary in this field.

Discrepancies in reported results provide sufficient incentive for a meta-analysis, and also suggest that there might exist moderating factors accounting for different links. Identifying constructs that may moderate the BFLPE can help further BFLPE theory (Seaton et al., 2009), while little progress has been made in finding factors that strengthen or weaken this effect. Hence, the principal focus of the present investigation is to examine potential moderating variables.

Related results indicated that there may exist one or more variables moderating the BFLPE, such as student age, comparison target, and ASC domain. The first is student age. Marsh (1987) proposed that the BFLPE is more likely to occur when young children begin to form ASC, and Becker and Neumann (2016) supposed that older students are capable enough to deal with conflicting information obtained from contexts, so that they may not suffer the BFLPE. Subjects from a wide range of age groups have been included in BFLPE researches completed to date. Some researchers focused on 15-year-olds from the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) (e.g., Nagengast and Marsh, 2012; Marsh, 2016), some took sample of students at grade 4 and grade 8 from the Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) (e.g., Chiu, 2012; Liou, 2014), and others assessed independent samples at different ages. They usually came out with different results. In Marsh's 2016 study, 276,165 students from PISA 2003 led to the BFLPE at −0.30, while in Preckel's study carried out in 2010, which took a sample of 722 primary school students got a weaker effect (−0.19). Liou (2014) found that the BFLPE was stronger in 8th grade students than 4th grade students, but he didn't do further moderating analysis. The second is the comparison target. In BFLPE researches, students' comparison target was assumed to be a generalized other (Marsh et al., 2008b), which was operationalized by either class-average achievement (e.g., Huguet et al., 2009; Marsh et al., 2009; Preckel and Brull, 2010; Thijs et al., 2010) or school-average achievement (e.g., Seaton et al., 2009; Chiu, 2012; Marsh, 2016; Areepattamannil et al., 2017), and the results varied accordingly. Areepattamannil et al. (2017) assessed the school effect and got the BFLPE at −0.43, while Preckel and Brull (2010) took the class-average achievement as comparison target and got a weaker effect (−0.19). The third is ASC domain. Among the numerous researches about ASC in the BFLPE, some focused on general ASC (e.g., Marsh et al., 2008b; Albert and Dahling, 2016), while others were interested in domain-specific ASC (e.g., Huguet et al., 2009; Jansen et al., 2014), and the size of the effect varies correspondingly. For example, Marsh et al. (2008b) measured general ASC and math ASC in two independent samples simultaneously, while the former got the effect of −0.20, and the latter was −0.44.

In addition to above-mentioned three potential moderators, other study characteristic variables, such as sample size, publication year and student location that have been examined in many published meta-analysis articles were also included in the moderation analyses. Summing up, six potential moderators would be examined in this meta-analysis: student age, comparison target, ASC domain, sample size, publication year, and student location.

We present the first Meta-analysis of the BFLPE synthesizing previous researches on the BFLPE to: (1) provide an integrated effect size of the BFLPE; (2) investigate whether the size of BFLPE will change accordingly when student age changes; (3) find out whether taking class-average achievement as comparison target will lead to different effect size compared with taking school-average achievement as reference; (4) explore the influence of ASC domain on the size of BFLPE; (5) other potential moderating variables, such as sample size, publication year and student location were also examined.

Methods

Literature Search

Search Strategies

We systematically searched the quantitative studies evaluating the effect of class- or school-average achievement on student ASC. To find all articles that met our criteria, we conducted a literature search using the Educational Database, Research Library, Psychology Database, PsycARTICLES, PsycINFO, and ERIC. Each database was searched using the following key terms: Big fish little pond or academic self-concept in the abstract and average in the full text. We searched for all full-text and peer-review articles written in English and published from January 1st 1984 to January 1st 2018. Because the BFLPE was first put forward by Marsh and Parker (1984). The initial search revealed 386 articles in total.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Articles were included based on the following criteria: (1) quantitative researches whose topic was the BFLPE on student ASC; (2) used the classic BFLPE model that test the class/school effect after controlling for student effect; (3) explicitly reported the regression coefficients of class/school average achievement on student ASC; (4) provided detailed information about class/school that was taken as the comparison target; (5) results derived from subjects with intellectual disability or learning disability were not considered here.

This preliminary selection procedure resulted in 39 studies. After excluding the studies using the same data resource, we got 33 studies in total with 56 effect sizes (N = 1,276,838) in the end. The whole process was based on PRISMA and detailed information about the process through literature search, study selection, and study inclusion for the meta-analysis was illustrated in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram showing the process through the literature search, study selection, and study inclusion for the meta-analysis.

Coding Procedures

Outcome Variable

We focus on the effect of class- or school-average achievement on student ASC, so the multilevel regression coefficients β and sample size of each study were recorded.

Regression coefficients were coded based on an independent sample, and separately coded if a study had several independent samples. Besides, if a study included repeated measurement experiments at different time, the result retrieved from the last measurement would be chosen.

Potential Moderating Variables

Six potential moderators would be examined in this meta-analysis: student age, comparison target, ASC domain, sample size, publication year and student location.

These 33 studies were carefully coded for the following variables.

1. Student Age. Student age was coded as “primary school,” “middle school,” “high school,” or “college.”

2. Comparison target. The comparison target was recorded as “school” or “class.”

3. ASC domain. The domain that student ASC was measured was recorded as “general,” “verbal,” or “STEM” (Science, Technology, Engineering, Mathematics). For example, studies using measuring scales that contain statements like “I am good at English/French/Verbal” would be codes as “verbal.”

4. Sample size.

5. Pub-year. The publication year was recorded.

6. Student location. The student location refers to the area where participants come from, it was coded as “Asia,” “Europe,” “North America,” “Oceania,” or “Mix.”

We didn't consider student gender because the BFLPE was tested to be robust over gender (Marsh and Hau, 2003). And the type of measuring tool was not considered because this variable can't be categorized that many researchers just reported the achievement measure as quote from some International Education Survey Project or offered vague information about item type, so we didn't examine its moderating effect here. The coding was conducted by two researchers twice with an interval of 2 months.

Statistical Analysis

Effect Size

Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software program version 3.0 was used to conduct the meta-analysis. Each regression coefficient was transformed into a Fisher's Z score as an effect size (ES), and all weighted mean ESs and corresponding confidence intervals were converted back at last for a better understanding.

Heterogeneity

Cochran's Q-Test and the I2 statistic were used for the homogeneity test. Moderator analyses were conducted after the homogeneity test. I2values of 0–25% were interpreted as no heterogeneity, 25–50% as low heterogeneity, 50–75% as moderate heterogeneity, and 75–100% as high heterogeneity among studies.

Publication Bias

The funnel plot and Egger regression test were used to test whether the results were biased due to different publication sources.

Results

Characteristic of the Studies Included

Study name (presented as “first author's last name & publication year”), regression coefficient, N (sample size), ES (effect size) and student age of each study included are reported in Table 1. Comparison target, ASC domain, and student location are reported in Table 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of studies included in the meta-analysis (1).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of studies included in the meta-analysis (2).

A total of N = 1,276,838 were involved in the included 33 studies, and 56 ESs were coded out of the studies.

Thirty-nine of the ESs were based on Large-scale assessments (7 for PISA, 6 for TIMSS, and 26 for other assessments like TOSCA), other 17 were retrieved from studies collecting data independently.

Seven of the ESs were based on students from Asia (4 for Singapore, 1 for United Arab Emirates, and 2 for Taiwan, China), 29 were based on Europe students (19 for Germany, 3 for Belgium, 2 for France, 1 for Netherlands, 1 for Norway, 2 for Poland, and 1 for UK), 10 were based on North America students, 1 was based on Oceanian students and 9 were Mix (e.g., from 27 countries).

Fourteen of the ESs were based on general ASC, 30 were based on STEM ASC (22 for mathematics ASC, 8 for science ASC), and 12 were based on verbal ASC (4 for French ASC, 6 for English ASC, 2 for general verbal ASC).

Publication Bias

As we can see from Figure 2, the Funnel plot showed that all the 56 ESs are evenly distributed on both sides and gather at the top of the plot, and the Egger regression revealed no significant bias with t = 0.32 (df = 54, p > 0.05). Together, we can conclude that the results were not biased due to the publication sources.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Funnel plot.

Mean Effect Size

The homogeneity test results were Q = 25,478.88 (df = 55, p < 0.001), I2 = 99.78%, so the random effects model was chosen. The integrated results showed a significant negative effect of class/school average achievement on student ASC: β = −0.28 (Z = −13.84, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−0.32, −0.24]), which means that students in class/school with an average ability level one standard deviation above the mean have ASC that is 0.28 of a standard deviation below the average ASC level. These effect sizes were suitable for subsequent moderator analyses.

Moderator Analyses

Student Age

The mixed effects model was chosen here. As showed in Table 3, the main effect of student age was significant: Z = −17.56, p < 0.001, and the heterogeneity test was significant with Q = 7.86 (df = 3, p < 0.05), which meant that student age significantly moderates the BFLPE. From Table 3, we can also see that students in high school indicate the strongest effect (βhighschool = −0.32), while middle school and college students show a moderate effect (βmiddleschool = −0.28, βcollege = −0.23), and primary school students show the weakest effect (βprimaryschool = −0.21). These results indicated that the BFLPE is the strongest when students in high school, weaker in middle school and college, and shows the weakest in primary school.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Student age as moderator of the BFLPE.

Comparison Target

There was no significant influence of comparison target: Q = 0.01 (df = 1, p > 0.05), which meant that whether the study takes class-average achievement or school-average achievement as comparison target has little influence on the size of BFLPE.

Academic Self-Concept Domain

As showed in Table 4, the main effect of ASC domain was significant: Z = −15.62, p < 0.001, and the heterogeneity test was significant with Q = 7.23 (df = 2, p < 0.05), which meant that ASC domain significantly moderates the BFLPE. From Table 4, we can also see that verbal ASC indicates the strongest effect (βverbalASC = −0.31), while STEM ASC shows moderate effect (βSTEMASC = −0.30), and general ASC shows the weakest effect (βgeneralASC = −0.22). These results indicated that the BFLPE varies with the domain of ASC and indicates strongest when verbal ASC is considered.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. ASC domain as moderator of the BFLPE.

Sample Size

Meta-regression showed that there was no significant influence of sample size with Q = 0.00 (df = 1, p > 0.05).

Publication Year

Meta-regression showed that there was no significant influence of publication year with Q = 0.35 (df = 1, p > 0.05).

Student Location

As showed in Table 5, the main effect of student location was significant: Z = −14.56, p < 0.001, and the heterogeneity test was significant with Q = 11.07 (df = 4, p < 0.05), which meant that student location significantly moderates the BFLPE. From Table 5, we can also see that Asian students indicate the strongest effect (βAsia = −0.35), while North American students show the weakest effect (βNorthAmerica = −0.20), and students in Europe, Oceania and mixed countries show the moderate effect (βEurope = −0.30, β Oceania = −0.27, βMix = −0.26). These results indicated that the BFLPE varies with student location of participants and indicates strongest in Asia.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Student location as moderator of the BFLPE.

Discussion

The BFLPE

As the first meta-analysis of the BFLPE, this paper presents a new perspective into this theory and provides a reliable synthesized result of the effect size of the BFLPE based on empirical researches. More importantly, six potential moderators were examined and student age was found to significantly moderate the BFLPE.

The combined results show a significant negative effect of class/school average achievement on student ASC: β = −0.28 (Z = −13.84, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−0.32, −0.24]), which means that students in class/school with an average ability level one standard deviation above the mean have ASC that is 0.28 of a standard deviation below the average ASC level. The result confirms that the BFLPE is prevailing and robust in educational psychology, as supported by many other cross-culturable empirical studies (Marsh et al., 2014, 2015; Marsh, 2016).

The results of the meta-analysis contribute to the BFLPE realm both theoretically and practically. First of all, confirmation of the persistence of the BFLPE demonstrates the point that students' perception of oneself can be understood in consideration of social comparison theory, which argues that unpleasant social comparison experienced in higher ability educating environment may induce lower ASC (Marsh et al., 1995; Huguet et al., 2009). Since there lacks less able students to make favorable comparison with and overflows with more able students in a highly capable group, it is possible for students to experience uncertainty about one's own ability and ambiguity in verifying their own competence, which may induce lower ASC. Second, the BFLPE could give explanations for educational phenomena. For example, average students in general classes or schools always have more positive ASC than those abler ones attending advanced placement, which can be interpreted by the BFLPE that the former usually rank favorably in their local environment, while the latter frequently rank unfavorably with much more high-quality peers in their surroundings. Last but not least, negative consequences of being in a more competitive educational setting should not be ignored. From the perspective of parents who consider sending their children to high-achieving schools or transferring children to advanced classes, they should be informed of the potential negative consequence on ASC; as for educators, understanding how ASC might be influenced by the BFLPE can facilitate application of appropriate teaching practices, so that they can help students develop proper ASC, which is necessary for fine academic development. It has been demonstrated that differentiated instruction strategies can be used to attenuate the BFLPE (Roy et al., 2015); besides, it reveals the necessity of special education classes or schools: when disadvantaged students are put in regular schools/classes, they are very likely to suffer from low ASC for being small fishes in the big pond.

Moderating Role of Student Age

The BFLPE was found significant in all age groups in this study, from primary school to college, which coincides with the point that the BFLPE is more likely to occur in elementary (primary) school, during when children are in the formatting self-concepts (Marsh, 1987).

Moreover, this meta-analysis found that student age significantly moderates the BFLPE, that is, the BFLPE is the strongest when students in high school, weaker in middle school and college, and shows the weakest in primary school. It coincides with past assumptions that inferring a person's ability is a process underlying ASC, and only those who have developed the most differentiated conceptions of ability are able to infer other's ability based on their achievement and efforts (Marsh, 1984). Besides, social comparison that plays an important role in the BFLPE largely correlates with cognitive development.

Early adolescents, as primary school students in this study, begin to master social comparison, but still lack the ability to integrate different information about themselves (Harter, 2003), so they show a significant BFLPE but very small in size. As their cognitive skills and academic pressure grow, the effect size increases a bit in middle school. Students in college are old and experienced enough to get rid of relying too much on others, which means that they are capable to assess their own academic skills independent of the performance of their classmates (Marsh, 1987; Becker and Neumann, 2016), so the decline happens in the BFLPE. As for high school students' strongest effect, we can explain it in two ways. First, the tracking effect. Academic tracking system has been the most-implemented curriculum delivery model in almost all schools, which mostly happens during high school (Lüdtke et al., 2006; Falkenstein, 2007; Liu and Wang, 2008; Wouters and Fraine, 2010; Houtte and Stevens, 2015; Salchegger, 2016; Dumont et al., 2017). The academic tracking system divides students into class/school levels for low, medium, and high achievers in each grade based on past performance, which may increase the chances of experiencing unpleasant comparison for students in intermediate-track or high-track schools; second, high school students are experiencing a period of life characterized by increased self-consciousness, and they always face more academic pressure. So synthetically considering, students in this age group would be much more influenced by the class/school-average ability.

These results suggest that the BFLPE is an age-based process, which occurs at primary school age and reaches peak value during high school. Considered that ASC in high school has been found to be more salient than actual academic achievement in predicting learning effort, educational and occupational aspirations, and subsequent university course selection (Guay et al., 2004; Marsh et al., 2008a), special caution from teachers and parents should be paid for high school students, who are at risk of suffering the strongest BFLPE.

Moderating Role of Academic Self-Concept Domain

The BFLPE was found significant in all three domains of ASC and the size of the BFLPE was found to vary by different ASC domains: general ASC resulted the lowest effect, verbal ASC showed the strongest effect, and STEM ASC indicated medium effect.

In 1976, Shavelson, Hubner, and Stanton presented the Shavelson model (cf. Byrne and Worth Gavin, 1996), which posited ASC to be hierarchically organized, with general ASC at the apex of the hierarchy. Empirical researches strongly support the hypotheses of the hierarchical organization (Marsh et al., 1988; Marsh, 1990; Martin et al., 2010). General ASC is regarded as relatively stable competence beliefs that is independent of the situation (e.g., Scherbaum et al., 2006). Besides, general ASC is found to directly influences domain-general and subject-specific measures of ASC. Hence, general ASC directly accounts for a substantial amount of variance in all measures of ASC (Martin et al., 2010). Summing up the above, general ASC has the ability to maintain relative stability, so it may suffer less from the negative effect of class/school average achievement.

There exists clear distinction between verbal ASC and STEM ASC (Marsh, 1986). Compared with STEM ASC, verbal ASC exposes more to external comparison. Generally speaking, various language activities will be held in class or school, which will bring rich success-failure experience, so that students more frequently compare their own verbal abilities with the perceived abilities of other students in their frame of reference and use this external impression as one basis of their self-perceptions of verbal ASC. Besides, external observers usually form the evaluation of one's verbal ability based on their speaking skills, which in turns lead to change in verbal ASC. Thus, verbal ASC may be more easily influenced by the average ability of classmates or schoolmates, which will show the strongest BFLPE.

Moderating Role of Student Location

The BFLPE was found significant in all student locations here, which verifies the BFLPE is intercultural and stable (Marsh and Hau, 2003). The result also reveals that learning to avoid the negative effect of the BFLPE is necessary for educators from all countries.

Besides, the size of BFLPE was found to be strongest for Asian students and weakest in North America. Asian participants here were most from Taiwan, China and Singapore, which are highly industrialized and always perform outstandingly in international large-scale assessments (Liou, 2014). The possible reason for the strongest relation between class/school-average achievement and ASC may be the cultural difference. Seaton et al. (2009) put forward that the size of BFLPE varies across countries and the different population may lead to different patterns between student ASC and achievement (Liou, 2014). Most Asian students are raised up in surroundings highly value academic achievement while students from other student locations face less academic stress than Asian ones, and Asian schools always emphasize the competition with their peers, so they may compare with classmates and schoolmates more frequently, besides, Asian students are found to have a high level of test anxiety and self-doubt compared with their counterparts (Stankov, 2010), which result in the strongest BFLPE in Asian students.

The non-significant moderating effect of sample size, and publication year reveal that the size of the BFLPE doesn't vary as sample size or publication year changes, which confirm the BFLPE's universality and robustness (Marsh et al., 2014, 2015; Marsh, 2016).

Limitations

There exists an apparent gap between the number of different comparison targets (39 for school-average achievement and 17 for class-average achievement). This may result in the insignificant result in the moderation analyses, so future research can broaden the scope of literature search to obtain enough studies. Furthermore, the dependence of ESs caused by deriving more than one ES from a study or from studies conducted by the same research team was not examined here, which can be further discussed using a multilevel model.

Future Research

Regarding the direction of future research, the possible moderating role of student ability can be taken into consideration. Although the BFLPE was found in students across different level of ability (Marsh and Hau, 2003), some researches (Marsh and Rowe, 1996; Trautwein et al., 2009) found that the ASC of relatively high-achieving students appear to be less affected by BFLPE than those of relatively low-achieving students. Roy et al. (2015) also found that significant BFLPE was only for students with low individual achievement and for whom teachers reported less frequent use of differentiated instruction strategies. So, it is worth exploring whether the BFLPE is moderated by students' ability level.

Conclusion

This research made these main contributions: (1) presents a new perspective of the BFLPE by conducting a meta-analysis, which goes beyond prior work by providing a reliable quantitative conclusion of the BFLPE; (2) examines six potential moderating variables and identifies three moderators of the BFLPE: student age, student location and ASC domain. The findings help further the understanding of the BFLPE and make it clear that BFLPE is an age-based process, which occurs at primary school age and reaches peak value during high school. Besides, the BFLPE varies with student location and ASC domain, indicating strongest when verbal ASC is considered and for Asian students. Furthermore, these findings have utility for educators. A better understanding of these processes may enable teachers to better motivate students and provides credible reinforcement to seek measures to reduce the negative BFLPE.

Author Contributions

JF, XH, and MZ came up with the experiment ideas. JF and FH did literature research. JF, XH, and ZL analyzed experimental results. JF and QY wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the South China Normal University (The growth model of students in Guangdong Province, grant number 538/339124).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Albert, M. A., and Dahling, J. J. (2016). Learning goal orientation and locus of control interact to predict academic self-concept and academic performance in college students. Pers. Indiv. Diff. 97, 245–248. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Areepattamannil, S., Khine, M. S., and Al Nuaimi, S. (2017). The big-fish-little-pond effect on mathematics self-concept: evidence from the United Arab Emirates. J. Adolesc. 59, 148–154. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arens, A. K., and Watermann, R. (2015). How an early transition to high-ability secondary schools affects students' academic self-concept: contrast effects, assimilation effects, and differential stability. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 37, 64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.11.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, M., and Neumann, M. (2016). Context-related changes in academic self-concept development: on the long-term persistence of big-fish-little-pond effects. Learn. Instruct. 45, 31–39. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M., and Worth Gavin, D. A. (1996). The Shavelson model revisited: testing for the structure of academic self-concept across pre-, early, and late adolescents. J. Educ. Psychol. 88, 215–228. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.88.2.215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Card, N. A. (2012). Applied Meta-Analysis for Social Science Research. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Chiu, M. S. (2012). The internal/external frame of reference model, big-fish-little-pond effect, and combined model for mathematics and science. J. Educ. Psychol. 104, 87–107. doi: 10.1037/a0025734

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dumont, H., Protsch, P., Jansen, M., and Becker, M. (2017). Fish swimming into the ocean: how tracking relates to students' self-beliefs and school disengagement at the end of schooling. J. Educ. Psychol. 109, 855–870. doi: 10.1037/edu0000175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkenstein, R. (2007). Student Experiences of Participation in Tracked Classes Throughout High School: The Ethic of Justice, School Leadership, and Curriculum Design. Electronic Thesis or Dissertation. Available online at: https://etd.ohiolink.edu/

Guay, F., Larose, S., and Boivin, M. (2004). Academic self-concept and educational attainment level: a ten-year longitudinal study. Self Identity, 3, 53–68. doi: 10.1080/13576500342000040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guay, F., Marsh, H. W., and Boivin, M. (2003). Academic self-concept and academic achievement: a developmental perspective on their causal ordering. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 124–136. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harter, S. (2003). “The development of self-representations during childhood and adolescence,” in Handbook of Self and Identity, eds M. R. Leary, Tangney, and J. Price (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 610–642.

Houtte, M. V., and Stevens, P. (2015). Tracking and sense of futility: the impact of between-school tracking versus within-school tracking in secondary education in flanders (belgium). Br. Educ. Res. J. 41, 782–800. doi: 10.1002/berj.3172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huguet, P., Dumas, F., Marsh, H., Wheeler, L., Seaton, M., Nezlek, J., et al. (2009). Clarifying the role of social comparison in the big-fish-little-pond effect (BFLPE): an integrative study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 156. doi: 10.1037/a0015558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jansen, M., Schroeders, U., and Lüdtke, O. (2014). Academic self-concept in science: multidimensionality, relations to achievement measures, and gender differences. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 30, 11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jansen, M., Schroeders, U., Lüdtke, O., and Marsh, H. W. (2015). Contrast and assimilation effects of dimensional comparisons in five subjects: an extension of the I/E model. J. Educ. Psychol. 107, 1086–1101. doi: 10.1037/edu0000021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liem, G. A. D., and Yeung, A. S. (2013). The big-fish-little-pond effect and a national policy of within-school ability streaming alternative frames of reference. Am. Educ. Res. J. 50, 326–370. doi: 10.3102/0002831212464511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liou, P. (2014). Investigation of the big-fish-little-pond effect on students' self-concept of learning mathematics and science in Taiwan: Results from TIMSS 2011. Asia Pacific Educ. Res. 23, 769–778. doi: 10.1007/s40299-013-0152-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, W. C., and Wang, C. K. J. (2008). Home environment and classroom climate: an investigation of their relation to students' academic self-concept in a streamed setting. Curr. Psychol. 27, 242–256. doi: 10.1007/s12144-008-9037-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohbeck, A., and Moller, J. (2017). Social and dimensional comparison effects on math and reading self-concepts of elementary school children. Learn Individ. Diff. 54, 73–81. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.01.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lüdtke, O., Marsh, H. W., Köller, O., and Baumert, J. (2006). Tracking, grading, and student motivation: using group composition and status to predict self-concept and interest in ninth-grade mathematics. J. Educ. Psychol. 98, 788–806. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.4.788

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W. (1984). Self-concept: the application of a frame of reference model to explain paradoxical results. Aust. J. Educ. 28, 165–181. doi: 10.1177/000494418402800207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W. (1986). Verbal and math self-concepts: an internal/external frame of reference model. Am. Educ. Res. J. 23, 129–149.

Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W. (1987). The big-fish-little-pond effect on academic self-concept. J. Educ. Psychol. 79, 280–295.doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.79.3.280

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W. (1988). Influences of internal and external frames of reference on the formation of math and English self-concepts. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 107–116. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W. (1990). The structure of academic self-concept: the Marsh/Shavelson model. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 623–636. doi: 10.1037//0022-0663.82.4.623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W. (1994). Using the national longitudinal study of 1988 to evaluate theoretical models of self-concept: the self-description questionnaire. J. Educ. Psychol. 86, 439–456. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.86.3.439

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W. (2016). Cross-cultural generalizability of year in school effects: negative effects of acceleration and positive effects of retention on academic self-concept. J. Educ. Psychol. 108, 256–273. doi: 10.1037/edu0000059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Abduljabbar, A. S., Morin, A. J. S., Parker, P., Abdelfattah, F., and Nagengast, B., et al. (2015). The big-fish-little-pond effect: generalizability of social comparison processes over two age cohorts from Western, Asian, and middle Eastern Islamic countries. J. Educ. Psychol. 107, 258–271. doi: 10.1037/a0037485

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Byrne, B. M., and Shavelson, R. J. (1988). A multifaceted academic self-concept: its hierarchical structure and its relation to academic achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 80, 366–380. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.366

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Chessor, D., Craven, R., and Roche, L. (1995). The effects of gifted and talented programs on academic self-concept: the big fish strikes again. Am. Educ. Res. J. 32, 285–319. doi: 10.3102/00028312032002285

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., and Craven, R. G. (2006). Reciprocal effects of self-concept and performance from a multidimensional perspective: beyond seductive pleasure and unidimensional perspectives. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 133–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., and Hau, K. T. (2003). Big-fish-little-pond effect on academic self-concept. a cross-cultural (26-country) test of the negative effects of academically selective schools. Am. Psychol. 58, 364–376. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.5.364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Koller, O., and Baumert, J. (2001). Reunification of East and West German school systems: longitudinal multilevel modelling study of the big-fish-little-pond effect on academic self-concept. Am. Educ. Res. J. 38, 321–350. doi: 10.3102/00028312038002321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Kong, C. K., and Hau, K. T. (2000). Longitudinal multilevel models of the big-fish-little-pond effect on academic self-concept: counterbalancing contrast and reflected-glory effects in Hong Kong schools. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 337–349.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Kuyper, H., Morin, A. J. S., Parker, P. D., and Seaton, M. (2014). Big-fish-little-pond social comparison and local dominance effects: integrating new statistical models, methodology, design, theory and substantive implications. Learn. Instruct. 33, 50–66. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Lüdtke, O., Robitzsch, A., Traütwein, U., Asparouhov, T., Muthén, B. O., et al. (2009). Doubly-latent models of school contextual effects: Integrating multilevel and structural equation approaches to control measurement and sampling error. Multivariate Behav. Res. 44, 764–802. doi: 10.1080/00273170903333665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., and O'Mara, A. J. (2010). Long-term total negative effects of school-average ability on diverse educational outcomes: direct and indirect effects of the big-fish-little-pond effect. German J. Educ. Psychol. 24, 51–72. doi: 10.1024/1010-0652.a000004

CrossRef Full Text

Marsh, H. W., and Parker, J. W. (1984). Determinants of student self-concept: is it better to be a relatively large fish in a small pond even if you don't learn to swim as well? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 47, 213–231. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.1.213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., and Rowe, K. J. (1996). The negative effects of school-average ability on academic self-concept: an application of multilevel modelling the negative effects of school-average ability on academic self-concept: an application of multilevel modelling. Aust. J. Educ. 40, 65–87. doi: 10.1177/000494419604000105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Seaton, M., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Hau, K. T., O'Mara, A. J., et al. (2008a). The big-fish-little-pond-effect stands up to critical scrutiny: implications for theory, methodology, and future research. Educ. Psychol. Rev., 20, 319–350. doi: 10.1007/s10648-008-9075-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Baumert, J., and Köller, O. (2007). The big-fish-little-pond effect: persistent negative effects of selective high schools on self-concept after graduation. Am. Educ. Res. J. 44, 631–669. doi: 10.3102/0002831207306728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., and Köller, O. (2008b). Social comparison and big-fish-little-pond effects on self-concept and other self-belief constructs: role of generalized and specific others. J. Educ. Psychol. 100, 510–524. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.510

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Koller, O., and Baumert, J. (2005). Academic self-concept, interest, grades, and standardized test scores: Reciprocal effects models of causal ordering. Child Dev. 76, 397–416. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00853.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, B., Ulrich, K., Christophe, D., Monique, R., Sonja, U., Antoine, F., et al. (2010). The structure of academic self-concepts revisited: the nested Marsh/Shavelson model. J. Educ. Psychol. 102, 964–981. doi: 10.1037/a0019644

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Möller, J., Pohlmann, B., Köller, O., and Marsh, H. W. (2009). A meta-analytic path analysis of the internal/external frame of reference model of academic achievement and academic self-concept. Rev. Educ. Res. 79, 1129–1167. doi: 10.3102/0034654309337522

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagengast, B., and Marsh, H. W. (2011). The negative effect of school-average ability on science self-concept in the UK, the UK countries and the world: the Big-Fish-Little-Pond-Effect for PISA 2006. Educ. Psychol. 31, 629–656. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2012.696353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagengast, B., and Marsh, H. W. (2012). Big fish in little ponds aspire more: mediation and cross-cultural generalizability of school-average ability effects on self-concept and career aspirations in science. J. Educ. Psychol. 104, 1033–1053. doi: 10.1037/a0027697

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niepel, C., Brunner, M., and Preckel, F. (2014). The longitudinal interplay of students' academic self-concepts and achievements within and across domains: replicating and extending the reciprocal internal/external frame of reference model. J. Educ. Psychol. 106, 1170–1191. doi: 10.1037/a0036307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, P. D., Marsh, H. W., Lüdtke, O., and Trautwein, U. (2013). Differential school contextual effects for math and english: integrating the big-fish-little-pond effect and the internal/external frame of reference. Learn. Instruct. 23, 78–89. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinxten, M., De Fraine, B., Van Damme, J., and D'Haenens, E. (2010). Causal ordering of academic self-concept and achievement: effects of type of achievement measure. Brit. J. Educ. Psychol. 80, 689–709. doi: 10.1348/000709910X493071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plieninger, H., and Dickhäuser, O. (2015). The female fish is more responsive: gender moderates the bflpe in the domain of science. Educ. Psychol. 35, 213–227. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2013.814197

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preckel, F., and Brull, M. (2010). The benefit of being a big fish in a big pond: contrast and assimilation effects on academic self-concept. Learn. Individ. Differ. 20, 522–531. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preckel, F., Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., and Kleine, M. (2008). Gender differences in gifted and average-ability students. Gifted Child Quart. 52, 146–159. doi: 10.1177/0016986208315834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roy, A., Guay, F., and Valois, P. (2015). The big-fish–little-pond effect on academic self-concept: the moderating role of differentiated instruction and individual achievement. Learn. Individ. Differ. 42, 110–116. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salchegger, S. (2016). Selective school systems and academic self-concept: how explicit and implicit school-level tracking relate to the big-fish—little-pond effect across cultures. J. Educ. Psychol. 108, 405–423. doi: 10.1037/edu0000063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherbaum, C. A., Cohen-Charash, Y., and Kern, M. J. (2006). Measuring general self-efficacy: a comparison of three measures using item response theory. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 66, 1047–1063. doi: 10.1177/0013164406288171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherer, R., and Siddiq, F. (2015). The big-fish–little-pond-effect revisited: do different types of assessments matter? Comput. Educ. 80, 198–210. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2014.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seaton, M., Marsh, H. W., and Craven, R. G. (2009). Earning its place as a pan-human theory: universality of the big-fish-little-pond effect across 41 culturally and economically diverse countries. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 403–419. doi: 10.1037/a0013838

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stäbler, F., Dumont, H., Becker, M., and Baumert, J. (2017). What happens to the fish's achievement in a little pond? A simultaneous analysis of class-average achievement effects on achievement and academic self-concept. J. Educ. Psychol. 109, 191–207. doi: 10.1037/edu0000135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stankov, L. (2010). Unforgiving Confucian culture: a breeding ground for high academic achievement, test anxiety and self-doubt? Learn. Individ. Differ. 20, 555–563. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sung, Y. T., Huang, L. Y., Tseng, F. L., and Chang, K. E. (2014). The aspects and ability groups in which little fish perform worse than big fish: examining the big-fish-little-pond effect in the context of school tracking. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 39, 220–232. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szumski, G., and Karwowski, M. (2015). Emotional and social integration and the big-fish-little-pond effect among students with and without disabilities. Learn. Individ. Differ. 43, 63–74. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.08.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thijs, J., Verkuyten, M., and Helmond, P. (2010). A further examination of the big-fish-little-pond effect: perceived position in class, class size, and gender comparisons. Sociol. Educ. 83, 333–345. doi: 10.1177/0038040710383521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Traütwein, U., Ludtke, O., Koller, O., and Baumert, J. (2006). Self-esteem, academic self-concept, and achievement: how the learning environment moderates the dynamics of self-concept. J. Personality Soc. Psychol. 90, 334–349. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Marsh, H. W., and Nagy, G. (2009). Within-school social comparison: how students perceive the standing of their class predicts academic self-concept. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 853–866. doi: 10.1037/a0016306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valentine, J. C., and Dubois, D. L. (2005). “Effects of self-beliefs on academic achievement and vice versa. Separating the chicken from the egg,” in International Advances in self Research: New Frontiers for Self Research. Vol.2, eds H. W. Marsh, R. G. Craven, and D. M. McInerney ( Greenwich, CT: Information Age), 53–78.

Google Scholar

Wouters, S., and Fraine, B. D. (2010). The effect of track changes on the development of academic self-concept in high school: a dynamic test of the big-fish-little-pond effect. J. Educ. Psychol. 104, 793–805. doi: 10.1037/a0027732

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wouters, S., Germeijs, V., Colpin, H., and Verschueren, K. (2011). Academic self-concept in high school: predictors and effects on adjustment in higher education. Scand. J. Psychol. 52, 586–594. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2011.00905.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: big-fish-little-pond effect, student, academic self-concept, age, meta-analysis

Citation: Fang J, Huang X, Zhang M, Huang F, Li Z and Yuan Q (2018) The Big-Fish-Little-Pond Effect on Academic Self-Concept: A Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 9:1569. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01569

Received: 18 March 2018; Accepted: 07 August 2018;
Published: 29 August 2018.

Edited by:

Courtney McKim, University of Wyoming, United States

Reviewed by:

Malte Jansen, Institute for Educational Quality Improvement (IQB), Germany
Dimitrios Zbainos, Harokopio University, Greece

Copyright © 2018 Fang, Huang, Zhang, Huang, Li and Yuan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Minqiang Zhang, 2640726401@qq.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.