Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 01 October 2018
Sec. Emotion Science

Competitive Intensity Modulates the Pain Empathy Response: An Event-Related Potentials Study

\r\nPinchao Luo&#x;Pinchao Luo1†Yu Pang,&#x;Yu Pang1,2†Beibei LiBeibei Li1Jing JieJing Jie1Mengdi ZhuangMengdi Zhuang1Shuting YangShuting Yang1Xifu Zheng*Xifu Zheng1*
  • 1School of Psychology, Center for Studies of Psychological Application, and Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
  • 2School of Education and Psychological Science, Sichuan University of Science and Engineering, Zigong, China

Previous studies have widely reported that competition modulates an individual’s ability to empathize with pain experienced by others. What remains to be clarified, however, is how modulations in the intensity of competition might affect this type of empathy. To investigate this, we first used a Eriksen Flanker task to set different competitive intensity context (high competitive intensity, HCI; medium competitive intensity, MCI; low competitive intensity, LCI). Then we used a recognition task as a competitive task, in which we recorded event-related potentials (ERP) while participants viewed static images of body parts in painful and non-painful situations. Participants were informed that both sets of images depicted an opponent that they were required to play against in the recognition task that varied in levels of competitive intensity according to condition (HCI, MCI, and LCI). We observed an early N2 differentiation between pain and no-pain stimuli over the frontal area under MCI and LCI conditions, but this was not detected under HCI condition. Moreover, we observed a pattern of pain and no-pain differentiation for the late LPP over the frontal and centro-parietal regions under HCI, MCI, and LCI condition. As the pain empathy response is indexed by pain and no-pain differentiation, these results indicate a down-regulation of pain empathy response attributable to a high level of competition. With its very early onset, this effect appears to inhibit bottom-up processing of the ability to perceive pain experienced by an opponent. Our results provide neuroscientific evidence for a deficit in early automatic arousal in response to the pain of the opponent under the influence of high competitive intensity.

Introduction

Pain empathy has been defined as the ability to understand and experience the painful feelings of another individual through cognitive/evaluative and affective processes (Jackson et al., 2006; Decety et al., 2010; Bernhardt and Singer, 2012; Gao et al., 2017). This ability enables us to understand the pain of others, to motivate prosocial behavior, and to reduce racial biases (Coke et al., 1978; Batson et al., 2002; Burgay et al., 2003; Todd et al., 2011; Graaff et al., 2018; Travis et al., 2018). Due to its significant role in social interaction, pain empathy has become an intense area of research in psychology and neuroscience (Sassenrath et al., 2016; Yang et al., 2017). Recently, a large body of studies have investigated whether and how pain empathy is affected by social factors such as sense of fairness (Singer et al., 2006), monetary reward (Guo et al., 2012; Goerlich et al., 2016), affective preference (Wang et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2014), social distance (Zaki, 2014; Wang et al., 2016), as well as competition and cooperation (Cui et al., 2016).

One interesting aspect of this research is that people would regulate their pain empathy response to others when operating within a context that is competitive. The effect of competition on empathy for pain remains a controversial issue. For instance, some studies have found that relative to in-group members, participants experience less empathy toward out-group members when the two groups are in a competitive relationship (Cikara et al., 2011, 2014). Other studies have observed that people are friendlier, more helpful, and more willing to respond to others in cooperation, but more aggressive, less helpful, and less willing to empathize with their opponents (Leach et al., 2003; Deutsch, 2015; Ouwerkerk et al., 2016; De Vos et al., 2016; Suleiman et al., 2018). However, affective responses to the opponent’s pain in competitive context were characterized not only by less empathy but also by increased counter-empathic responses: schadenfreude (Cikara et al., 2014). An ERP study conducted by Yamada et al. (2011) compared empathic responses to the cooperative coplayer and the competitive coplayer. The results showed that the affective expression of the coplayer presented to the participants would induced congruent empathic responses under cooperative condition, while incongruent counter-empathetic responses occurred under competitive condition (Yamada et al., 2011). Another recent ERP study conducted by Cui et al. (2016) compared brain response when participants passively viewed images depicting the hands or feet of anonymous individuals in painful and non-painful situations. Importantly, viewing took place within both competitive, and cooperative contexts. That is, alongside but unrelated to the images, participants played a game which they were instructed to play competitively or cooperatively with a partner. They found that viewing others in pain elicited significantly larger P3 amplitudes than the non-pain-related pictures, however this effect was only observed within the competitive context and not within the cooperative context. This result indicated that the participants were more responsive to other’s pain in a competitive context than in a cooperative context due to the threatening atmosphere induced by competitive context.

In these previous studies mentioned above, the influence of competitive social context on empathy was investigated by comparing empathy response in competitive and cooperative context (Koban et al., 2012; De Dreu and Kret, 2016; Lee et al., 2018) or by recording empathic response only in competitive context (Yamada et al., 2011). The intensity of competitive context was not taken into account. However, competition has a dynamic continuous structure ranging in intensity from weak to strong (Li et al., 2012) in real life. Previous studies have found that differences in competitive intensity represent different levels of threat (Ibáñez et al., 2011; Brankley and Rule, 2014). As a result, these variations in competitive intensity have differential effects, such that a higher competitive intensity context would create a more negative and threatening atmosphere. Accordingly, schadenfreude but not empathic response to the opponent’s pain is more likely to occur. What is currently unknown is whether the empathic response to pain and non-pain stimuli is modulated as a function of differing intensities of competition context.

It is suggested that empathy involves both an early automatic component characterized by emotional sharing (bottom-up processing) and a late controlled component characterized by cognitive evaluation (top-down processing) (Decety and Lamm, 2006; Xiang et al., 2018). Evidences from ERP studies have shown that the temporal dynamics of empathy for pain consists of an early affective arousal component (N1/N2) followed by a late cognitive reappraisal and regulation component (P3/LPP) (Luck and Hillyard, 2000; Fan and Han, 2008; Han et al., 2008; Cheng et al., 2017; Decety et al., 2017). Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have also demonstrated engagement of the anterior insula (AI), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), brain stem, and cerebellum during observation of other people in painful situations (Singer et al., 2004; Jackson et al., 2005; Cheng et al., 2007; Gu and Han, 2007; Lamm et al., 2007, 2011; Walter et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2018). It also remains to be clarified whether bottom-up and top-down information processes of pain empathy are associated with these differences in competitive intensity.

To investigate this, we first used a Eriksen Flanker task to set different competitive intensity context. Given that competition is rooted in evaluation, reward, wins and losses (Amabile, 1996), different competitive intensity contexts could be implemented by manipulating the possibility of winning (Li et al., 2012). Consistent with the research paradigm of Li et al. (2012), we manipulated the variable of competitive intensity by varying the probabilities of winning as a function of condition (HCI condition, 10% probability; MCI condition, 50% probability; and LCI condition, 90% probability). Then we used a recognition task as competitive task, in which we recorded event-related potentials (ERP) while participants viewed static images of body parts in painful and non-painful situations. Participants were informed that both sets of images depicted an opponent that they were required to play against in the judgment task that varied in levels of competitive intensity according to condition (HCI, MCI, and LCI).

According to previous studies (Decety et al., 2010; Luo et al., 2015), the pain empathy response is indexed by pain and no-pain differentiation. The early empathic N2 component is supposed to be automatic, bottom-up driven and more dependent on the context or characteristic of stimulus (Fan and Han, 2008). In addition, compared with low competitive intensity, high competitive intensity represented a greater threat to the possibility of winning (Diehl and Stroebe, 1991), because the success of participants’ opponent would reduce the possibility of their own success when competing for the same goal. Therefore, we expected that N2 differences between pain/no-pain stimuli in the early empathic response could not be found only under HCI condition, as high competitive context would hinder automatic empathic response. The late empathic LPP component is supposed to be controlled-top-down driven. As we know, empathy induces prosocial behavior and is widely appreciated by the society (Coke et al., 1978; Batson et al., 2002). Thus, we anticipated that the three conditions would show LPP differences between pain/no-pain stimuli, as top down mechanisms would reappraise the stimuli and regulate the empathic response to conform to social expectations.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Forty-seven college students (23 females, 24 males) aged 19–24 years (mean age, 21.3 years; SD 2.3 years) were enrolled in the study. Two participants (1 female, 1 male) were excluded from data analysis because of intensive head movements during electroencephalographic (EEG) recording (over 15% bad epochs). All participants were right-handed, with normal or corrected to normal vision, and reported no history of neurological, brain injuries, or developmental disabilities. Each participant signed an informed consent form and received monetary compensation for the experiment. The study was approved by the Academic Committee of South China Normal University. The experimental procedure met the standard of ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki (British Medical Journal Publishing Group, 1996).

Visual Stimuli

Similar to those in previous ERP studies (Fan and Han, 2008; Decety et al., 2010), visual stimuli of judgment task in ERP session consisted of 60 digital color pictures showing a person’s hand or foot in painful or non-painful situations (30 each). The accidents in the pictures depicted everyday life scenarios. Pain pictures included situations such as a hand trapped in a door or cut by scissors. Each pain picture was matched with a non-pain picture that showed similar events in the same contexts, but without the nociceptive component. All of them had the same size of 9 × 6.76 cm (width × height) and were of 100 pixels per inch. Each picture was presented at the center of a 17-in. color monitor against a white background, subtending a visual angle of 2.86 × 2.29 at a viewing distance of 100 cm. Besides that, the visual stimuli used in the Eriksen Flanker task were four strings (“<<<<<”, “>>>>>”, “<<><<”, and “>><>>”), which were similar to those in previous studies (Cui et al., 2016).

Experimental Procedure

Once consent forms were signed, one experimenter pretended to be the opponent was introduced to the participant. All participants were informed that the opponent would sit in another room and play the competitive game with him/her. The experiment consisted of two parts. One was a Eriksen Flanker task and the other was an ERP session. The former was to set different competitive intensity context, while the latter was to record empathy brain response to the opponent’s pain within different competitive intensity context. The experimental procedure was shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Illustration of the stimuli and experimental procedure used in the current study. (A) stimuli used and timing of events in one trial in the Eriksen Flanker task. (B) stimuli used and timing of events in one trial in the ERP experiment procedure.

The Eriksen Flanker task contained 30 flanker task trials. In each of the flanker trials, a fixation was presented on a white screen for 500 ms, followed by one of the four strings: “<<<<<”, “>>>>>”, “<<><<”, and “>><>>”. Participants were instructed to respond to the arrow in the middle of the string by pressing “F” or “J” buttons (“<” corresponded to “F” and “>” corresponded to “J”). String presentation lasted for a maximum 3000 ms until a response was given. Participants were informed that the computer would compare their results with the opponents’ results according to the reaction time and accuracy, and predict their probability of winning in the subsequent recognition task in ERP session. In actual fact, participants were randomly assigned to one of three different competitive intensity groups: HCI condition (10% probability of winning), MCI condition (50% probability of winning) and LCI condition (90% probability of winning). To ensure that the competitive intensity manipulation was successful, participants were asked to rate the competitive intensity (3-point scale: 1 = LCI, 2 = MCI, 3 = HCI) and threat level (10-point scale: 1 = no threat, 10 = big threat) that they felt once the probability of winning was announced.

In the ERP session, participants were told that the reward would depend on whether they could win in the following recognition task, in which they were asked to imagine the pictures they watched belonged to their opponents and recognize the affective response (painful or non-painful) of their opponents. If the participant had a higher accuracy and faster reaction time than the opponent, he/she could win 50 RMB. Otherwise, he/she got 0 RMB. Participants were also told that their opponents would finish the same task, with one difference: the opponents imagined the pictures they watched belonged to the participants. Participants in different competitive intensity conditions performed the same ERP procedure. ERP recordings were made up of four sessions with each containing 60 trials. The order of the trial condition (pain, non-pain) was randomized. In each trial, a black fixation against a white screen was presented for 500 ms, followed by a picture for 1000 ms. Then a question mark would remain for a maximum 3000 ms until a response was given. Here, the participants were asked to recognize the valence of the pictures (pain or non-pain) and press “F” or “J” buttons (“F” corresponded to “pain”, “J” corresponded to “no-pain”) as quickly and accurately as possible. The trial ended with a blank screen varying in duration from 500 ms to 800 ms randomly. At the end of the ERP session, participants were asked to answer the questions “The hand in the picture belongs to whom? (1 = the opponent, 2 = a stranger)” and “What are your feelings when you see the opponent in pain? (1 = unpleasant, 2 = pleasant, 3 = no feeling)”.

After ERP recording, to measure individual differences of empathy, participants were instructed to fill in the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) (Davis, 1983) including four subscales: perspective taking (PT), fantasy (FS), empathic concern (EC), and personal distress (PD).

ERP Recording and Analysis

Electroencephalogram (EEG) data were recorded from 64 scalp electrodes mounted on an elastic cap according to the extended 10–20 system (Brain Products, Germany), with references on the left and right mastoids and a ground electrode on the medial frontal aspect. Eye blinks and vertical eye movements were monitored with electrodes located above and below the left eye. The horizontal EOG was recorded from electrodes positioned 1.5 cm lateral to the left and the right external canthi. The EEG activity was amplified at 0.01–100 Hz band-passes and sampled at 500 Hz. All electrode impedances were kept below 5 kΩ. ERPs under each condition were computed separately off-line using Brain Vision Analyzer 2.0 software (Brain Products, Germany) (Fritsch and Kuchinke, 2013). ERP waveforms were time-locked to the onset of stimuli. The average epoch was 1200 ms, including a 200 ms pre-stimulus baseline. Trials contaminated by eye movements and muscle potentials exceeding ± 100 μV at any electrode or response errors were excluded from the average.

Previous studies have indicated that empathy for pain include an early emotional sharing component (N2) and a late cognitive evaluation component (LPP) (Luo et al., 2015; Cheadle, 2017). Moreover, grand averaged waveforms and topographical map of ERPs (see Figure 2) showed that those elicited by pain pictures and non-pain pictures in different competitive intensities were different and these differences were largest at frontal, central, and parietal sites. Thus, nine electrodes were selected for the following statistical analysis: F3, F4, Fz (frontal sites), C3, C4, Cz (central sites), P3, P4, Pz (parietal sites). Three-way ANOVA was conducted for N2 (220–250 ms) and LPP (350–600 ms) components. There was one between-group factor (competitive intensity: HCI, MCI, and LCI), and two within-group factors (stimulus: pain and non-pain pictures; electrode distribution: frontal, central, and parietal sites). The dependent variable was the mean amplitude for each component calculated from the frontal, central, parietal areas. Degrees of freedom for F-ratios were corrected according to the Greenhouse-Geisser method.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Average ERPs at Fz, Cz, and Pz for pain pictures and non-pain pictures under high competitive intensity (HCI), medium competitive intensity (MCI), and low competitive intensity (LCI) conditions, and the topographical map during 220–250 ms time windows (N2) and 350–600 ms time windows (LPP).

Results

Behavioral Performance

Table 1 shows the mean scores and standard errors for each subscale of the IRI. One-way ANOVA showed that there was no difference between the three competitive intensity groups in PT, FS, EC, and PD subscales [PT: F(2,42) = 0.612, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.028; PD: F(2,42) = 1.085, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.049; EC: F(2,42) = 0.200, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.009; FS: F(2,42) = 0.767, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.035]. All participants correctly identified the pain cues on the recognition task when watching pain and non-pain stimuli during ERP recording sessions. All the participants’ answers to the questions “The hand in the picture belongs to whom?” and “What are your feelings when you see the opponent in pain?” were “the opponent” and “unpleasant”, respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Mean scores and standard error for the subscales of the IRI.

Subjective competitive intensity ratings were analyzed by one-way ANOVA and showed a significant difference in the three groups [F(2,41) = 16.646, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.448] (see Figure 3). Further analysis showed that the scores of the HCI group were significantly higher than that of the MCI group (p < 0.05) and the LCI group (p < 0.05), and the scores of the MCI group were significantly higher than that of the LCI group (p < 0.01). Moreover, one-way ANOVA revealed that the main effect of the threat level felt by the three groups was significant [F(2,41) = 13.127, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.390] (see Figure 3). The amount of threat experienced by the HCI group was significantly higher than that of both the MCI group (p < 0.01) and the LCI group (p < 0.01). However, there was no significant difference between the MCI and the LCI group (p > 0.05). Results thus indicate that the setting of the competitive intensity was effective. Moreover, the threat of HCI was found to be significantly stronger than that of MCI and LCI.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. The rating scores of subjective competitive intensity (SCI) and subjective threat (ST). p < 0.05 and ∗∗p < 0.01.

ERP Results

For the N2 component (see Tables 2, 3), ANOVA produced a significant main effect of electrode distribution [F(2,41) = 103.815, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.712]. The main effect of competitive intensity [F(2,42) = 2.805, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.118] and stimulus [F(1,42) = 1.196, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.028] were not significant. In addition, a two-way interaction between stimulus and competitive intensity was significant [F(2,42) = 5.536, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.209]. The simple effect analysis revealed that pain pictures elicited a more positive shift than non-pain pictures under the MCI (t = -5.118, p < 0.05) and LCI (t = 1.944, p < 0.05) conditions, but not under the HCI condition (t = 0.022, p > 0.05). A two-way interaction between competitive intensity context and electrode distribution was significant [F(4,84) = 4.110, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.164]. The simple effect of competitive intensity was significant at frontal site [F(2, 87) = 11.095, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.203] and central site [F(2,87) = 4.416, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.093], but not at parietal site [F(2,87) = 2.565, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.056]. In the frontal region and central region, a subsequent pairwise comparison showed that the LCI context elicited a smaller negative deflection than the MCI context (p < 0.01), and the MCI context elicited a smaller negative deflection than the HCI context (p < 0.05). No other interaction was found to be significant.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Summary of ANOVA results of N2 (220–240 ms) and LPP (350–600 ms) with the electrode distribution (frontal, central, and parietal), stimulus (pain, non-pain) as the within-subject factors, and competitive intensity (HCI, MCI, and LCI) as the between-subject factor.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Mean amplitudes (μV) and standard error at N2 (220–240 ms) and LPP (350–600 ms) shown by the three-way interaction of electrode distribution, stimulus, and competitive intensity.

Repeated-measures ANOVA on the LPP component produced a significant main effect of electrode distribution [F(2,41) = 8.593, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.1701], stimulus [F(1,42) = 89.104, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.680] and a marginally significant main effect of competitive intensity [F(2,42) = 3.155, p = 0.053, η2 = 0.131] (see Tables 2, 3). A two-way interaction between electrode distribution and competitive intensity [F(4,84) = 4.075, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.163] and a three-way interaction between electrode distribution, competitive intensity and stimulus [F(4, 84) = 2.576, p < 0.05, η2 = 0109] were found to be significant. Beyond that, no other significant interactions were observed. Focusing on the three-way interaction, significant two-way interaction was observed between competitive intensity and stimulus at frontal site [F(2,42) = 3.509, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.143], but not at central site [F(2,42) = 1.735, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.076] and parietal site [F(2,42) = 0.159, p > 0.05, η2 = 0.007]. In frontal region, simple effect of stimulus was significant under HCI (t = 2.790, p < 0.05), MCI (t = 4.339, p < 0.01), and LCI (t = 7.951, p < 0.01) conditions. Pairwise comparisons showed that pain pictures elicited larger LPP amplitudes than those elicited by non-pain pictures (p < 0.01) under HCI, MCI, and LCI condition, respectively. Moreover, simple effects of competitive intensity were found under both the pain condition [F(2, 42) = 6.885, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.247] and non-pain condition [F(2,42) = 5.330, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.202]. Subsequent multiple comparisons showed that the amplitudes of LPP elicited under HCI condition were significantly smaller than those elicited under MCI (p < 0.01) and LCI (p < 0.01) conditions. However, no significant differences between the LPP amplitudes elicited in MCI and LCI condition were found.

Discussion

Previous studies have investigated empathic response to other’s pain within a competitive context (Koban et al., 2012; Cui et al., 2016). The present work extends previous research by examining the neural processes underlying responses to viewing others in painful and non-painful situations within different kinds of competitive (HCI, MCI, and LCI) context. We found that empathic responses were modulated by competitive intensity. Specifically, we observed early N2 differentiation between painful and no-painful situations over the frontal area under MCI and LCI conditions. In contrast, no such early ERP response was detected under HCI condition. Moreover, we observed a pattern of pain and no-pain differentiation for the late LPP over the frontal and centro-parietal regions under HCI, MCI, and LCI condition. These results indicated that pain empathic response is down-regulated under HCI condition at early N2 stage.

Our behavioral results showed that participants identified the painful situation of the opponents during ERP recording sessions and felt unpleasant when watching the opponents in pain. This suggests that empathic response but not schadenfreude was induced by the opponents’ pain in our study. Societal expectation and the amount of reward may contribute to the affective response. First, participants try to fit a role based on society expectation because kindheartedness is advocated by intellectuals in Chinese culture. Second, the amount of reward is too little to induce schadenfreude. Although subjective competitive intensity ratings showed a significant difference in the three groups (HCI group > MCI group > LCI group), there was no significant difference between the threat level felt by the MCI and the LCI group. However, the threat of HCI was significantly stronger than that of MCI and LCI. The results indicate that high competitive intensity context is more likely to modulate the neural underpinnings of pain empathy response due to its bigger threat.

According to previous research results, the frontocentral N2 component is thought to reflect aspects of response conflict and response inhibition (Botvinick et al., 2004; Kerns et al., 2004; Luo et al., 2013). The larger N2 amplitudes, the higher level of conflict and inhibition will be. People tend to vicariously resonate with the pain of others (Lamm et al., 2011). However, pain empathy is not obligatory (Cameron et al., 2017). The “threat value of pain” hypothesis demonstrates that processing of another’s pain also may be associated with a threat, which informs us of potential harm and promotes self-protective response (Williams, 2002; Yamada and Decety, 2009; Ibáñez et al., 2011). Empathic response is other-oriented while protective response is self-oriented. Thus, these two responses are conflicting and the inhibition of self-protective response is important during the empathic task. In our study, N2 amplitudes in HCI condition were significantly larger than that of in MCI condition, and N2 amplitudes in MCI condition were significantly larger than that of in LCI context. The results show that as competitive intensity increase, the conflict and inhibition is getting stronger and stronger. More importantly, we found neutral pictures elicited more negative deflections than painful pictures only in MCI and LCI condition, but not in HCI condition. A highly competitive intensity context creates a negative and threatening atmosphere, which in turn triggers a system of threat-detection and induces negative emotion. Individuals who are in bad moods have difficulty in focusing on others’ painful situations (Baron-Cohen et al., 2004). One possible explanation is that the HCI context may induce a greater sense of threat and lead to a negative emotion, which forces people to ignore other’s pain. Therefore, the bottom-up processing of participants’ perception of pain experienced by their opponent is absent. Our results are consistent with previous studies which have demonstrated that the automatic process of empathy (N2) can be affected by competitive context (Cikara et al., 2014; Cui et al., 2016).

The LPP component is considered to reflect a facilitated process attention to emotionally relevant or motivationally salient stimuli (Schupp et al., 2000, 2003; Hajcak et al., 2010; Kiat and Cheadle, 2017). We found that LPP amplitudes elicited in HCI condition were significantly smaller than in MCI and LCI conditions. Our behavioral data showed that the subjective threat level felt by the HCI group was significantly stronger than by the MCI group and the LCI group. One possible explanation is that people feel more threats in HCI condition, which in turn leads to less attention paid to painful stimuli. In this case, painful stimuli elicited the smallest LPP amplitudes in HCI context. In addition, our LPP results showed that pain stimuli elicited larger amplitudes than non-pain stimuli under all three competitive contexts. Previous studies suggest that, compared with neutral stimuli, negative stimuli would recruit more physiological and psychological resources due to the evaluation of evolutionary importance (Yuan et al., 2007). Thus, negative stimuli elicit larger LPP amplitudes than neutral stimuli (Luo et al., 2013, 2015). Our results are consistent with these previous studies and indicate that empathic response could be found within three competitive intensity contexts at late cognitive controlled stage.

In conclusion, the current ERP study provides new neuroscientific insights into how differing levels of competitive intensity affect the ability to experience empathy for pain. Previous studies have suggested that empathy involves both bottom-up and top-down information processing (Decety and Lamm, 2006). The former is automatic and allows individuals to experience similar emotional states to others, whereas the latter is equivalent to an elaborative process that reflects the integration of cognitive control and reappraisal (Dennis and Hajcak, 2009; DeCicco et al., 2012). Our results suggest that, under condition of high intensity competition, sensory processing elicited by the perception of pain during the automatic emotional sharing stage (N2) is absent and pain empathy response at the late cognitive evaluation stage (LPP) is less obvious. Our results allow a better understanding of the mechanism underlying the effect of competition on pain empathy. One limitation is that we have no control group, which can be compared with three groups to observe the empathy for others’ pain between different competitive intensity contexts and control context, and then to control the group as a reference to understand the empathy of pain in different competitive intensity contexts separately. Another limitation is that the present research only focuses on empathy in relation to physical pain, it overlooks other ways in which people readily empathize with others in everyday life such as social pain and emotional suffering (Zaki et al., 2009; Masten et al., 2011; Rameson et al., 2012). Thus, for the purposes of ecological validity, future studies would benefit from examining how these other forms of empathy might be influenced by modulating levels of competitive context.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Academic Committee of South China Normal University with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Academic Committee of South China Normal University.

Author Contributions

PL designed the experiments, analyzed the data, and wrote the paper. YP analyzed the data and wrote the paper. BL, JJ, MZ, and SY collected the data. XZ designed the experiments.

Funding

The present study was funded by the Natural Science Foundation of China (31771218) and the Project of Key Institute of Humanities and Social Sciences, MOE (17JJD190001).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the support from the China Scholarship Council, the Research Center for Crisis Intervention and Psychological Service of Guangdong Province and the Psychological Services and Counseling Base for the Happy Guangzhou Project.

References

Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in Context: Update to the Social Psychology of Creativity. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., and Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 163–175. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D., Chang, J., Orr, R., and Rowland, J. (2002). Empathy, attitudes, and action: can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group motivate one to help the group. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 1656–1666. doi: 10.1177/014616702237647

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernhardt, B. C., and Singer, T. (2012). The neural basis of empathy. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 1–23.

Google Scholar

Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., and Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 539–546. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brankley, A. E., and Rule, N. O. (2014). Threat perception: how psychopathy and machiavellianism relate to social perceptions during competition. Pers. Individ. Dif. 71, 103–107. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

British Medical Journal Publishing Group (1996). Declaration of Helsinki (1964). Br. Med. J. 313, 1448–1449. doi: 10.1136/bmj.313.7070.1448a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgay, M., D’Amico, N., Possenti, A., Manchester, R. N., Lyne, A. G., Joshi, B. C., et al. (2003). An increased estimate of the merger rate of double neutron stars from observations of a highly relativistic system. Nature 426, 531–533. doi: 10.1038/nature02124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, C. D., Spring, V. L., and Todd, A. R. (2017). The empathy impulse: a multinomial model of intentional and unintentional empathy for pain. Emotion 17, 395–411. doi: 10.1037/emo0000266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, J., Luo, Y., and Fang, C. (2017). Empathy for pain influenced by cognitive load: evidence from an ERP study. Acta Psychol. Sin. 49, 622–630. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.00622

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, Y., Lin, C. P., Liu, H. L., Hsu, Y. Y., Lim, K. E., Hung, D., et al. (2007). Expertise modulates the perception of pain in others. Curr. Biol. 17, 1708–1713. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cikara, M., Bruneau, E., and Saxe, R. (2011). Us and them: intergroup failures of empathy. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 149–153. doi: 10.1177/0963721411408713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cikara, M., Bruneau, E., Van Bavel, J. J., and Saxe, R. (2014). Their pain gives us pleasure: how intergroup dynamics shape empathic failures and counter-empathic responses. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 55, 110–125. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coke, J. S., Batson, C. D., and Mcdavis, K. (1978). Empathic mediation of helping: a two-stage model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 36, 752–766. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.36.7.752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, F., Zhu, X., Duan, F., and Luo, Y. (2016). Instructions of cooperation and competition influence the neural responses to others’ pain: an ERP study. Soc. Neurosci. 11, 289–296. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1078258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. (1983). Interpersonal reactivity index. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Dreu, C. K., and Kret, M. E. (2016). Oxytocin conditions intergroup relations through upregulated in-group empathy, cooperation, conformity, and defense. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 165–173. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Vos, B., Zomeren, M. V., Gordijn, E. H., and Postmes, T. (2016). When does the communication of group-based anger increase outgroup empathy in intergroup conflict? the role of perceived procedural unfairness and outgroup consensus. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 21, 533–548. doi: 10.1177/1368430216674340

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. Sci. World J. 6, 1146–1163. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2006.221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., Meidenbauer, K. L., and Cowell, J. M. (2017). The development of cognitive empathy and concern in preschool children: a behavioral neuroscience investigation. Dev. Sci. 21:e12570. doi: 10.1111/desc.12570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., Yang, C. Y., and Cheng, Y. (2010). Physicians down-regulate their pain empathy response: an event-related brain potential study. Neuroimage 50, 1676–1682. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeCicco, J. M., Solomon, B., and Dennis, T. A. (2012). Neural correlates of cognitive reappraisal in children: An ERP study. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2, 70–80. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dennis, T. A., and Hajcak, G. (2009). The late positive potential: a neurophysiological marker for emotion regulation in children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 1373–1383. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02168.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deutsch, M. (2015). Cooperation, Competition, and Conflict. Morton Deutsch: A Pioneer in Developing Peace Psychology. New York, NY: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Diehl, M., and Stroebe, W. (1991). Productivity loss in idea-generating groups: tracking down the blocking effect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 392–403. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.392

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, Y., and Han, S. (2008). Temporal dynamic of neural mechanisms involved in empathy for pain: an event-related brain potential study. Neuropsychologia 46, 160–173. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritsch, N., and Kuchinke, L. (2013). Acquired affective associations induce emotion effects in word recognition: an ERP study. Brain Lang. 124, 75–83. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, X., Pan, W., Li, C., Weng, L., Yao, M., and Chen, A. (2017). Long-Time exposure to violent video games does not show desensitization on empathy for pain: an fMRI study. Front. Psychol. 8:650. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goerlich, K. S., Votinov, M., Lammertz, S. E., Winkler, L., Spreckelmeyer, K. N., and Habel, U., et al. (2016). Effects of alexithymia and empathy on the neural processing of social and monetary rewards. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 2235–2250. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1339-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graaff, J. V. D., Carlo, G., Crocetti, E., Koot, H. M., and Branje, S. (2018). Prosocial behavior in adolescence: gender differences in development and links with empathy. J. Youth Adolesc. 47, 1–14.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gu, X., and Han, S. (2007). Attention and reality constraints on the neural processes of empathy for pain. Neuroimage 36, 256–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, X., Zheng, L., Zhang, W., Zhu, L., Li, J., Wang, Q., et al. (2012). Empathic neural responses to others’ pain depend on monetary reward. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 535–541. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hajcak, G., Anderson, B. S., Arana, A., Borckardt, J., Takacs, I., George, M. S., et al. (2010). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex stimulation modulates electrocortical measures of visual attention: evidence from direct bilateral epidural cortical stimulation in treatment-resistant mood disorder. Neuroscience 170, 281–288. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.04.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, S., Fan, Y., and Mao, L. (2008). Gender difference in empathy for pain: an electrophysiological investigation. Brain Res. 1196, 85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.12.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ibáñez, A., Hurtado, E., Lobos, A., Escobar, J., Trujillo, N., Baez, S., et al. (2011). Subliminal presentation of other faces (but not own face) primes behavioral and evoked cortical processing of empathy for pain. Brain Res. 1398, 72–85. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, P. L., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A. N., and Decety, J. (2006). Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain. Neuropsychologia 44, 752–761. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, P. L., Meltzoff, A. N., and Decety, J. (2005). How do we perceive the pain of others? a window into the neural processes involved in empathy. Neuroimage 24, 771–779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., and Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulated conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303, 1023–1026. doi: 10.1126/science.1089910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiat, J. E., and Cheadle, J. E. (2017). The impact of individuation on the bases of human empathic responding. Neuroimage 155, 312–321. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koban, L., Pourtois, G., Bediou, B., and Vuilleumier, P. (2012). Effects of social context and predictive relevance on action outcome monitoring. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 460–478. doi: 10.3758/s13415-012-0091-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamm, C., Decety, J., and Singer, T. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54, 2492–2502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamm, C., Nusbaum, H. C., Meltzoff, A. N., and Decety, J. (2007). What are you feeling? using functional magnetic resonance imaging to assess the modulation of sensory and affective responses during empathy for pain. PLoS One 2:e1292. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leach, C. W., Spears, R., Branscombe, N. R., and Doosje, B. (2003). Malicious pleasure: schadenfreude at the suffering of another group. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 932–943. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M., Ahn, H. S., Kwon, S. K., and Kim, S. I. (2018). Cooperative and competitive contextual effects on social cognitive and empathic neural responses. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:218. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y. W., Ma, W., Luo, J., and Zhang, Q. (2012). Competition and emotion impact on effect of prototype elicitation during insight problem solving. Acta Psychol. Sin. 44, 1–13. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2012.00001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., and Hillyard, S. A. (2000). “The operation of selective attention at multiple stages of processing: evidence from human and monkey electrophysiology,” in The New Cognitive Neuroscience, ed. M. S. Gazzaniga (New York, NY: Free Press), 687–700.

Google Scholar

Luo, P., Qu, C., Chen, X., Zheng, X., Jiang, Y., and Zheng, X. (2013). A comparison of counselors and matched controls in maintaining different brain responses to the same stimulus under the self-perspective and the other-perspective. Brain Image. Behav. 7, 188–195. doi: 10.1007/s11682-012-9214-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, P., Wang, J., Yan, J., Huang, S., Xie, M., Lin, D., et al. (2015). Gender differences in affective sharing and self–other distinction during empathic neural responses to others’ sadness. Brain Image. Behav. 9, 312–322. doi: 10.1007/s11682-014-9308-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, C. L., Morelli, S. A., and Eisenberger, N. I. (2011). An fMRI investigation of empathy for ‘social pain’ and subsequent prosocial behavior. Neuroimage 55, 381–388. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouwerkerk, J. W., van Dijk, W. W., Vonkeman, C. C., and Spears, R. (2016). When we enjoy bad news about other groups: a social identity approach to out-group schadenfreude. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 21, 214–232. doi: 10.1177/1368430216663018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rameson, L. T., Morelli, S. A., and Lieberman, M. D. (2012). The neural correlates of empathy: experience, automaticity, and prosocial behavior. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 235–245. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sassenrath, C., Pfattheicher, S., and Keller, J. (2016). I might ease your pain, but only if you’re sad: the impact of the empathized emotion in the empathy-helping association. Motiv. Emot. 41, 96–106. doi: 10.1007/s11031-016-9586-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Cacioppo, J. T., Ito, T., and Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology 37, 257–261. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720257

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schupp, H. T., Junghofer, M., Weike, A. I., and Hamm, A. O. (2003). Emotional facilitation of sensory processing in the visual cortex. Psychol. Sci. 14, 4–13. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 439, 466–469. doi: 10.1038/nature04271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suleiman, R., Yahya, R., Decety, J., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2018). The impact of implicitly and explicitly primed ingroup–outgroup categorization on the evaluation of others pain: the case of the Jewish–Arab conflict. Motiv. Emot. 42, 438–445. doi: 10.1007/s11031-018-9677-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, A. R., Bodenhausen, G. V., Richeson, J. A., and Galinsky, A. D. (2011). Perspective taking combats automatic expressions of racial bias. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 1027–1042. doi: 10.1037/a0022308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Travis, J., Pashak McCall, A., Conley Drew, J., Whitney Samuel, R., Oswald Stephanie, G., and Heckroth Evan, M. (2018). Empathy diminishes prejudice: active perspective-taking, regardless of target and mortality salience, decreases implicit racial Bias. Sci. Res. Publ. 9, 1340–1356.

Google Scholar

Walter, C., Oertel, B. G., Felden, L., Kell, C. A., Nöth, U., Vermehren, J., et al. (2016). Brain mapping-based model of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol effects on connectivity in the pain matrix. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 1659–1669. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Qu, C., Luo, Q., Qu, L., and Li, X. (2014). Like or dislike? affective preference modulates neural response to others’ gains and losses. PLoS One 9:e105694. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105694

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Song, J., Guo, F., Zhang, Z., Yuan, S., and Cacioppo, S. (2016). Spatiotemporal brain dynamics of empathy for pain and happiness in friendship. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:45. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, A. C. (2002). Facial expression of pain: an evolutionary account. Behav. Brain Sci. 25, 439–455. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X02000080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiang, Y., Wang, Y., Gao, S., Zhang, X., and Cui, R. (2018). Neural mechanisms with respect to different paradigms and relevant regulatory factors in empathy for pain. Front. Neurosci. 12:507. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamada, M., and Decety, J. (2009). Unconscious affective processing and empathy: an investigation of subliminal priming on the detection of painful facial expressions. Pain 143, 71–75. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.01.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamada, M., Lamm, C., and Decety, J. (2011). Pleasing frowns, disappointing smiles: an ERP investigation of counter-empathy. Emotion 11, 1336–1345. doi: 10.1037/a0023854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., Hu, X., Li, X., Lei, Z., Yi, D., Xiang, L., et al. (2017). Decreased empathy response to other people’s pain in bipolar disorder: evidence from an event-related potential study. Sci. Rep. 7:39903. doi: 10.1038/srep39903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Liu, X. X., Fang, Y., and Hong, Y. Y. (2014). Unresolved world war ii animosity dampens empathy toward 2011 Japanese earthquake and tsunami. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 45, 171–191. doi: 10.1177/0022022113509118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, J., Zhang, Q., Chen, A., Li, H., Wang, Q., Zhuang, Z., et al. (2007). Are we sensitive to valence differences in emotionally negative stimulus? electrophysiological evidence from an ERP study. Neuropsychologia 45, 2764–2771. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaki, J. (2014). Empathy: a motivated account. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1608–1647. doi: 10.1037/a0037679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaki, J., Weber, J., Bolger, N., and Ochsner, K. (2009). The neural bases of empathic accuracy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 11382–11387. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902666106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pain, empathy, competitive intensity, ERP, N2, LPP

Citation: Luo P, Pang Y, Li B, Jie J, Zhuang M, Yang S and Zheng X (2018) Competitive Intensity Modulates the Pain Empathy Response: An Event-Related Potentials Study. Front. Psychol. 9:1854. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01854

Received: 11 January 2018; Accepted: 11 September 2018;
Published: 01 October 2018.

Edited by:

Marco Tamietto, Tilburg University, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Fei Luo, Institute of Psychology (CAS), China
Massimiliano Aragona, Società italiana di medicina migratoria, Italy

Copyright © 2018 Luo, Pang, Li, Jie, Zhuang, Yang and Zheng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xifu Zheng, 20001016@m.scnu.edu.cn; zhengxf@scnu.edu.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.