Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 21 November 2018
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic On the Development of Space-Number Relations: Linguistic and Cognitive Determinants, Influences, and Associations View all 28 articles

Commentary : The Developmental Trajectory of the Operational Momentum Effect

  • 1Psychology Department, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
  • 2The Department of Behavioral Sciences, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel

A Commentary on
The Developmental Trajectory of the Operational Momentum Effect

by Pinheiro-Chagas, P., Didino, D., Haase, V. G., Wood, G., and Knops, A. (2018). Front. Psychol. 9:1062 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01062

Recently, Pinheiro-Chagas et al. (2018) studied the development of the operational momentum (OM) which denotes a tendency to accept larger than correct outcomes in addition and smaller than correct outcomes in subtraction. The authors reviewed some theories of OM and derived two competing predictions. First, they described the attentional account, according to which OM results from an overshoot of an attentional spotlight when moving along the spatially oriented mental number line (MNL) in accordance with the magnitude of the second operand. Given that “formal schooling …might consolidate a systematic movement direction during the acquisition of arithmetical skills” (p.3), older children should show more OM. Secondly, they described the compression account of OM according to which linear operations (addition, subtraction) are performed on logarithmically compressed operand representations. Referring to a log-to-linear developmental shift in the placement of numbers on visually presented number lines, they predicted that older children should show less (un-) compression and thus less OM. Their results from 8 to 12-year olds showed a gradual increase of OM starting at 9 years and thus supported the attentional account.

The clear performance pattern reported by Pinheiro-Chagas and colleagues makes a useful contribution to the literature on OM development but their report also misrepresents the state of knowledge about OM. It might leave readers unnecessarily misinformed about the multi-faceted origin of this bias generally, and more specifically about the status of reverse OM for our understanding of cognitive biases in formal reasoning. We draw attention to these points below.

First, the authors acknowledged early OM in 9-month-olds (McCrink and Wynn, 2009) as well as reverse OM in 6-year-olds (Knops et al., 2013), thus recognizing a potential problem with their conclusion of late-emerging and gradually increasing OM. While the authors mentioned the work of Pinhas and colleagues they did not convey its full impact with regard to this point. First, Pinhas and Fischer (2008; see also Shaki et al., 2018) observed larger OM in zero problems (such as 4+0) compared to non-zero problems (e.g., 3+1). This alone could suffice to discredit the compression account because the logarithm of zero is not defined. Thus, the compression account was arguably a mere strawman pitted against the attentional account, although other methodological differences, such as the number format, remain. But if attention shift magnitude is “… a distance corresponding to the magnitude of the second operand” (p. 2), how does this account explain larger OM with zero problems?

Further inconsistencies are reflected in the methods: From an attentional perspective, repeated downward movements of both addends, as well as upward movements of the subtrahend, constitute inconsistencies with the vertical MNL that maps small quantities below larger quantities. Experience with vertical mappings will change over age and might increase the performance consequences of such inconsistencies. More generally, why were operations along a horizontal MNL primed with vertical movements? The fact that subtrahends moved away from the area of interest in the display center removed attention from the place of mentally simulating the outcome, thus impeding subtraction.

Secondly, Pinhas and Fischer (2008) proposed multiple sources of OM, including the operands, the operator, and the result. Taking into consideration evidence from biased quantitative reasoning, estimation heuristics and spatial-numerical associations, we have since developed this proposal into a comprehensive model of arithmetic heuristics and biases (AHAB; see also Shaki and Fischer, 2017; Fischer and Shaki, 2018; Shaki et al., 2018). This model can explain the complete range of findings reported in the literature, including reverse OM, as a weighted contribution from an anchoring effect, an estimation heuristic, and spatial associations of operands and operators. Pinheiro-Chagas et al.'s report created the false impression that reverse OM is an anomaly. Instead it was found repeatedly (Charras et al., 2012, 2014; Knops et al., 2013; Pinhas et al., 2015; Blini et al., 2018) and can be understood as reflecting anchoring bias in non-zero problems. However, anchoring bias increases from fourth to eight grade (Smith, 1999) and this should gradually reduce OM unless other factors compensate for this bias.

One further strength of AHAB is its ability to account for both spatial and non-spatial biases in mental arithmetic, regardless of whether computational uncertainty originated from encoding non-symbolic operators or results, as in studies by Knops and colleagues, or from mapping of perfectly identifiable operators and results onto a continuous response dimension, such as horizontal lines or time intervals (Shaki et al., 2015). It would be interesting to learn whether Pinheiro-Chagas and colleagues replicated the spatial bias in response selection previously observed in this paradigm by Knops et al. (2009).

Finally, the authors mention also an heuristic account of OM: a tendency to accept more than the correct outcome for additions and less than the correct outcome for subtractions because addition leads to “more” and subtraction to “less” (McCrink and Wynn, 2009). They compare it to the attentional account and state that “… the two accounts provide equivalent predictions” (p. 3). This is in conflict with the recent analysis offered in McCrink and Hubbard(2018, p. 240) that “… the use of heuristics is generally increased when attention is decreased”. We think that heuristics are triggered by operators. Yet, OM only emerges late, i.e., when both operator and second operand have been processed (Liu et al., 2017; Masson et al., 2017; Blini et al., 2018). Results obtained from procedures where operators even preceded the first operand (cf. Knops et al., 2009) or multiple quantities are presented during responding (cf. Pinheiro-Chagas et al., 2018) must be interpreted cautiously because the normal ingredients of OM are dis-ordered or diluted.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

Supported by DFG FI 1915/8-1 Competing heuristics and biases in mental arithmetic. We acknowledge the support of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and Open Access Publishing Fund of University of Potsdam.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Blini, E., Pitteri, M., and Zorzi, M. (2018). Spatial grounding of symbolic arithmetic: an investigation with optokinetic stimulation. Psychol. Res. 1–20. doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-1053-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charras, P., Brod, G., and Lupianez, J. (2012). Is 26 + 26 smaller than 24 + 28? Estimating the approximate magnitude of repeated vs. different numbers. Attention Percept. Psychophys. 74, 163–173. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0217-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charras, P., Molina, E., and Lupiáñez, J (2014). Additions are biased by operands: evidence from repeated versus different operands. Psychol. Res. 78, 248–265. doi: 10.1007/s00426-013-0491-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, M. H., and Shaki, S. (2018). Number concepts: abstract and embodied. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 373:20170125. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knops, A., Viarouge, A., and Dehaene, S. (2009). Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: evidence from the operational momentum effect. Attention Percept. Psychophys. 71, 803–821. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.4.803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knops, A., Zitzmann, S., and McCrink, K. (2013). Examining the presence and determinants of operational momentum in childhood. Front. Psychol. 4:325. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, D., Cai, D., Verguts, T., and Chen, Q. (2017). The time course of spatial attention shifts in elementary arithmetic. Sci. Rep. 7:921. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-01037-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masson, N., Letesson, C., and Pesenti, M. (2017). Time course of overt attentional shifts in mental arithmetic: evidence from gaze metrics. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 71, 1009–1019. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2017.1318931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrink, K., and Hubbard, T. (2018). Dividing attention increases operational momentum. J. Numerical Cogn. 382, 230–245. doi: 10.5964/jnc.v3i2.34

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrink, K., and Wynn, K. (2009). Operational momentum in large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 400–408. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinhas, M., and Fischer, M. H. (2008). Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic. Cognition 109, 408–415. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinhas, M., Shaki, S., and Fischer, M. H. (2015). Addition goes where the big numbers are: evidence for a reversed operational momentum effect. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 22, 993–1000. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0786-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinheiro-Chagas, P., Didino, D., Haase, V. G., Wood, G., and Knops, A. (2018). The developmental trajectory of the operational momentum effect. Front. Psychol. 9:1062. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaki, S., and Fischer, M. H. (2017). Competing biases in mental arithmetic: when division is more and multiplication is less. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:37. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaki, S., Pinhas, M., and Fischer, M. H. (2018). Heuristics and biases in mental arithmetic: revisiting and reversing operational momentum. Think. Reason. 24, 138–156. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2017.1348987

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaki, S., Sery, N., and Fischer, M. H. (2015). 1 + 2 is more than 2 + 1: violations of commutativity and identity axioms in mental arithmetic. J. Cogn. Psychol. 27:4. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.973414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, H. D. (1999). Use of anchoring and adjustment heuristic by children. Curr. Psychol. 18, 294–300.

Google Scholar

Keywords: embodied cognition, operational momentum, SNARC effect, mental arithmetic, numerical cognition

Citation: Fischer MH, Miklashevsky A and Shaki S (2018) Commentary : The Developmental Trajectory of the Operational Momentum Effect. Front. Psychol. 9:2259. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02259

Received: 27 August 2018; Accepted: 30 October 2018;
Published: 21 November 2018.

Edited by:

Krzysztof Cipora, Universität Tübingen, Germany

Reviewed by:

Catherine Thevenot, Université de Lausanne, Switzerland
Jérôme Prado, Centre national de la recherche scientifique (CNRS), France

Copyright © 2018 Fischer, Miklashevsky and Shaki. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Martin H. Fischer, martinf@uni-potsdam.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.