Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 21 December 2018
Sec. Evolutionary Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Perceptions of People: Cues to underlying Physiology and Psychology View all 17 articles

Facial Adiposity, Attractiveness, and Health: A Review

\r\nStefan de Jager,*Stefan de Jager1,2*Nicoleen CoetzeeNicoleen Coetzee2Vinet CoetzeeVinet Coetzee3
  • 1Department of Psychology, Sefako Makgatho Health Sciences University, Ga-Rankuwa, South Africa
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa
  • 3Department of Genetics, Biochemistry and Microbiology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa

The relationship between facial cues and perceptions of health and attractiveness in others plays an influential role in our social interactions and mating behaviors. Several facial cues have historically been investigated in this regard, with facial adiposity being the newest addition. Evidence is mounting that a robust link exists between facial adiposity and attractiveness, as well as perceived health. Facial adiposity has also been linked to various health outcomes such as cardiovascular disease, respiratory disease, blood pressure, immune function, diabetes, arthritis, oxidative stress, hormones, and mental health. Though recent advances in the analysis of facial morphology has led to significant strides in the description and quantification of facial cues, it is becoming increasingly clear that there is a great deal of nuance in the way that humans use and integrate facial cues to form coherent social or health judgments of others. This paper serves as a review of the current literature on the relationship between facial adiposity, attractiveness, and health. A key component in utilizing facial adiposity as a cue to health and attractiveness perceptions is that people need to be able to estimate body mass from facial cues. To estimate the strength of the relationship between perceived facial adiposity and body mass, a meta-analysis was conducted on studies that quantified the relationship between perceived facial adiposity and BMI/percentage body fat. Summary effect size estimates indicate that participants could reliably estimate BMI from facial cues alone (r = 0.71, n = 458).

Introduction

Facial appearance in humans conveys a substantial amount of non-verbal information when it comes to our interactions with others. These include judgments of health (Rhodes et al., 2007; Coetzee et al., 2009; Stephen et al., 2009; Phalane et al., 2017), attractiveness (Rhodes, 2006; Coetzee et al., 2012; Foo et al., 2017), leadership ability (Little et al., 2007; Re and Perrett, 2014) and even academic ability (Zebrowitz et al., 2002; Talamas et al., 2016), to name but a few. Naturally, the evolutionary, social and behavioral implications of the way in which we perceive and react to other people's faces, has generated significant interested in the scientific community. Several facial cues have been identified as integral aspects of our judgments of others, including sexual dimorphism (Perrett et al., 1998; Thornhill and Gangestad, 2006), symmetry (Perrett et al., 1999; Scheib et al., 1999; Jones et al., 2001), averageness (Grammer and Thornhill, 1994; Rhodes et al., 2001), skin condition (Jones et al., 2004; Stephen et al., 2011), and facial adiposity, or perceived weight in the face (Coetzee et al., 2009; Tinlin et al., 2013). Of particular interest to scientists is the relationship between facial cues and perceptions of health and attractiveness in others, as these play a particularly influential role in our social interactions and mating behaviors. Since mate choice plays a central role in evolutionary psychology, researchers are eager to understand the relative contribution that various facial cues make in shaping our perceptions and judgments of health and attractiveness (Rhodes, 2006).

Attractiveness plays a prominent role in our everyday interactions with others and research has consistently shown that attractive people are judged more positively in general compared to unattractive people (see Rhodes, 2006; Little et al., 2011). Attractive people are also typically judged to be healthier compared to unattractive people (Kalick et al., 1998; Shackelford and Larsen, 1999; Boothroyd et al., 2013). One potential reason for this is that facial cues associated with attractiveness could also be linked to various health markers. To this end, humans evolved to be particularly attentive to facial cues associated with attractiveness, as these cues allow us to make inferences about other people's health and genetic fitness, or even behavioral tendencies (Rhodes et al., 2001). For example, fitness-related theories of human behavior suggest that key phenotypic cues influence our judgments of others because they evolved as cues to general health and mate quality (Langlois et al., 2000). One example of such a fitness-related theory is the “good genes” hypothesis, which postulates that female mate choice is heavily influenced by phenotypic cues, since they aid females in making snap judgments about men's current health, genetic quality and fertility, and thus suitability for mating (Hamilton and Zuk, 1982; Thornhill and Gangestad, 1993, 1999).

Choosing the right mate could provide substantial direct and indirect benefits to females and their offspring. For example, according to the immunocompetence handicap hypothesis (Folstad and Karter, 1992) males that display strong secondary male sexual traits are perceived as more attractive by females, as these secondary sexual traits may potentially serve as good indicators of immune function in males. The reason for this is that the testosterone that is responsible for the development of these secondary male sexual traits also has an immunosuppressive effect, and only good quality males can therefore afford to display them. By choosing a male with a strong immune system as a mating partner, a female can simultaneously lessen the risk of pathogen exposure, while also maximizing the robustness of her offspring in terms of immune function. It should be mentioned, however, that some studies have found very little to no evidence for a relationship between testosterone and immune suppression in mammals and humans, respectively, thus casting at least some doubt on the viability of the immunocompetence handicap hypothesis (Roberts et al., 2004; Nowak et al., 2018).

From an evolutionary point of view, preferences for certain facial cues as reliable indicators of health and genetic quality, can only be a coherent hypothesis if two conditions are met: (i) there should be agreement between people regarding the facial features that they find attractive (Langlois et al., 2000; Rhodes, 2006) and (ii) perceptions of attractiveness and health should be related to actual health outcomes or genetic quality (Coetzee et al., 2009; Rantala et al., 2013a). At present there is at least some empirical support for the notion that people can reliably detect and agree on facial cues that contribute to facial attractiveness and perceptions of health (Perrett et al., 1998; Langlois et al., 2000; Rhodes et al., 2001; Re and Rule, 2016a). This statement does come with a caveat though, as numerous studies have demonstrated that preferences for certain facial cues, such as facial adiposity for example, are likely mediated by environmental and cultural factors, including resource scarcity (Batres and Perrett, 2014, 2017), exposure to media beauty ideals (Batres and Perrett, 2014), and own ethnicity familiarity (Coetzee et al., 2014; Batres et al., 2017).

While there is strong support for the link between various facial cues and attractiveness, the link between facial attractiveness and actual health outcomes has been mixed (Weeden and Sabini, 2005; Rhodes et al., 2007; Foo et al., 2017; Cai et al., in press). For example, Kalick et al. (1998) found no reliable relationship between rated facial attractiveness and general health rating by physicians for people in adolescence, middle adulthood, or late adulthood. The researchers did, however, find that there was a correlation between rated facial attractiveness and perceived health for both males and females. After controlling for the mediating relationship that attractiveness may play between perceived health and actual health outcomes, a statistically significant correlation between perceived health and actual health was found. The researchers conclude that, paradoxically, attractiveness can cause a halo effect and serve to suppress accurate judgments of health, instead of enhancing them. In partial contrast to Kalick et al. (1998), Henderson and Anglin (2003) did find a significant correlation between rated attractiveness and longevity for 50 facial photographs taken from a high school yearbook, although it is not entirely clear how comparable longevity (Henderson and Anglin, 2003) and general health measures (Kalick et al., 1998) are within this context. For a review on the relationship between attractiveness and health see Weeden and Sabini (2005) and Rhodes (2006).

The relationship between individual facial cues associated with attractiveness and actual health outcomes has also been met with mixed results. For example, a study by Rhodes et al. (2001), using a very similar dataset to Kalick et al. (1998), found a relationship between facial averageness and perceived health, as well as facial symmetry and perceived health for both males and females. However, when the researchers investigated the potential relationship between actual health outcomes and facial averageness, a correlation was found only for male childhood health (r = 0.28), female adolescent health (r = 0.14) and the current health for females (r = 0.25). For rated asymmetry, no link was found between health outcomes and perceived symmetry of the participant's faces. One potential explanation for this inconsistent trend is that general health is a very broad concept that probably reflects a wide variety of health markers, which may, or may not, be linked to particular facial cues or even general facial attractiveness.

Facial Adiposity as a Cue to Perceived Health and Attractiveness

One of the facial cues that has consistently been associated with both attractiveness (Coetzee, 2011; Coetzee et al., 2012; Rantala et al., 2013a; Foo et al., 2017; Phalane et al., 2017), perceived health (Coetzee et al., 2009; Fisher et al., 2014b; Han et al., 2016; Windhager et al., 2018) and actual health outcomes (Coetzee et al., 2009; Reither et al., 2009; Rantala et al., 2013a; Tinlin et al., 2013; Martinson and Vasunilashorn, 2016) is facial adiposity. Facial adiposity, or perceived weight in the face, was demonstrated to be a reliable cue to health in a study published by Coetzee et al. (2009) where higher facial adiposity ratings were linked to perceived health and attractiveness, as well as increased risk of infection and cardiovascular-illness. Studies also consistently report that people with lower perceived facial adiposity are rated as more attractive and healthier compared to people with higher perceived facial adiposity (Klaczynski et al., 2009; Rantala et al., 2013a; Han et al., 2016; Foo et al., 2017). A study by Foo et al. (2017) showed that facial adiposity was a better predictor of attractiveness compared to sexual dimorphism, averageness, and symmetry, for male faces. The researchers also found that, for females faces, facial adiposity squared and sexual dimorphism were the best predictors of female facial attractiveness, while facial adiposity was also the strongest predictor of perceived health for male faces, while sexual dimorphism was the strongest predictor of perceived health for female faces, with facial adiposity failing to reach statistical significance.

As mentioned previously, for a specific facial cue to serve as a valid cue to health, people need to reliably detect the cue and agree on its relationship to health or attractiveness. Some studies have reported that participants are able to reliably estimate a person's body fat percentage or body mass index (BMI) from facial cues alone (Fisher et al., 2013; Tinlin et al., 2013; Han et al., 2016; Phalane et al., 2017). The fact that participants are able to accurately detect changes in BMI likely stem from the fact that changes in BMI have been linked to a predictable set of morphological characteristics such as width-to-height ratio, perimeter-to-area ratio, cheek-to-jaw-width ratio (Coetzee et al., 2010), shape of lower face outline, nose width, and eyebrow position (Mayer et al., 2017). A study by Re et al. (2013) reported that people can detect changes in BMI as small as 1.3 kg/m2 in male faces and 1.6 kg/m2 in female faces from facial cues alone. Another study by Re and Rule (2016b) corroborated this finding by reporting that an average change in BMI of 1.33 kg/m2 was sufficient for participants to report a noticeable change in the appearance of faces. In recent years there has also been a concerted effort from researchers to develop computer vision methods (Wen and Guo, 2013; Kocabey et al., 2017; Barr et al., 2018) and statistical models (Wolffhechel et al., 2015; Stephen et al., 2017) to predict BMI from facial images.

Being able to draw inferences about a person's weight from facial cues of adiposity, provides a potentially robust perceptual link between facial cues and actual health. Body fat percentage and BMI has consistently been linked to various negative health outcomes. Overweight individuals are subject to unfavorable health outcomes including diabetes, mental health problems, impaired immune function, high and low blood pressure and heart disease (Kopelman, 2007; Dixon, 2010; Zaccardi et al., 2017); while being severely underweight could be an indication that a person is vulnerable to communicable diseases (Fisher et al., 2014b), since being underweight is linked to malnutrition and compromised immune function (Ritz and Gardner, 2006; Dobner and Kaser, 2018). Accurate estimation of overall body weight from facial cues can thus serve as an effective cue to health, as lower or higher than average body weight could be indicative of a person's past, current and future health problems (Coetzee et al., 2009; Henderson et al., 2016). In addition to the strong relationship between BMI and health outcomes, a study by Levine et al. (1998) has also established a strong link between cheek adipose tissue and visceral abdominal fat and a study by Lee and Kim (2014) established a correlation between the distances between the inferior ear lobes and visceral fat. Inferring visceral fat from facial cues is a potentially important predictor of health outcomes, as visceral fat has been connected to negative health outcomes due to its high metabolic activity (Després and Lemieux, 2006). BMI is also highly hereditable, as twin studies have estimated the hereditability of BMI to be as high as 0.57 in people between 20 and 29 years of age, even after environmental variation and cultural-geographic regions were accounted for (Silventoinen et al., 2017).

Tables 1, 2 present summaries of studies investigating the relationship between facial adiposity, attractiveness, and perceived health.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of studies that investigated the relationship between facial adiposity and attractiveness.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of studies that investigated the relationship between facial adiposity and perceived health.

While research into the link between facial adiposity and health and attractiveness has been gaining momentum over the last decade, there are some challenges faced by researchers. Investigating the relationship between facial adiposity, actual health, and attractiveness provides a series of methodological challenges. As both underweight and overweight individuals are likely to be judged less healthy and attractive, Coetzee et al. (2009) proposed that the relationship between perceived health, attractiveness and adiposity is best represented by a quadratic relationship. Seeing as both overweight and significantly underweight individuals are subject to negative health outcomes, any large deviations from average levels of adiposity is likely to lead to decreases in health and attractiveness ratings. Subsequent studies confirmed that curvilinear models are often the best fitting models for these data (Phalane et al., 2017; Windhager et al., 2018), though this is not always the case (Coetzee et al., 2012; see for example Foo et al., 2017). In studies where sampling range restrictions occur for BMI, it is also possible that the range restriction obscures the quadratic pattern of the results. One notable study by Windhager et al. (2013) created a set of female adolescent geometric morphs that allowed the researchers to produce a set of 5 images which ranged from −5 SD (19% body fat) to +5 SD (64% body fat). These facial morphs were then used in a recent study where 274 male and female participants had to rate each of the 5 images on maturity, dominance, masculinity, perceived health and attractiveness (Windhager et al., 2018). Analysis revealed that both health and attractiveness consistently produced distinct curvilinear shapes across all rater categories and genders.

In addition to the complex statistical relationship that exists between facial adiposity and preference ratings, there is also evidence to suggest that humans integrate information from multiple facial cues to evaluate faces. One of the facial cues that have been identified as playing a particularly important role in mediating the relationship between facial adiposity and health ratings is skin condition. According to Fisher et al. (2014b) judgments of health and attractiveness from facial adiposity in isolation produces a conundrums for raters, since it becomes very difficult to differentiate between individuals with low levels of facial adiposity due to health and those that have low facial adiposity levels due to ill-health. One way around this problem is to integrate information from skin condition with facial adiposity to provide more information on the potential risk associated with the facial adiposity levels being perceived. An experiment conducted by Fisher et al. (2014b) provided evidence that participants likely integrate both skin color and facial adiposity cues when judging faces for health and attractiveness. Redness and yellowness in the face has been associated with perceptions of health (Stephen et al., 2009, 2011) and in the study by Fisher et al. (2014b) there was evidence to suggest that participants relied more on color cues to decide if a face looked healthy or attractive when facial adiposity levels were relatively low.

According to the World Health Organization (WHO) classification, a healthy BMI for an average adult is between 18.5 and 24.9 kg/m2 (World Health Organization, 2018). Numerous studies have shown that people generally prefer facial adiposity levels that are associated with the normal range of the World Health Organization's classification for BMI and that lower levels of facial adiposity is typically rated as more attractive, even across cultures (Coetzee et al., 2011, 2012, 2014; Re et al., 2011; Re and Rule, 2016b). For example, a study by Coetzee et al. (2011) found that, female participants judging other female faces, indicated that facial adiposity levels associated with a BMI of 19.76 kg/m2 are optimally attractive, while male raters indicated that adiposity levels associated with a BMI of 20.01 kg/m2 are optimally attractive for female faces. A similar study by Re and Rule (2016b) found that participants preferred facial adiposity levels associated with a BMI of 19.11 kg/m2 for female faces and 23.79 kg/m2 for male faces. Coetzee (2011) compared facial adiposity preferences between a cohort of British and African participants and found that Caucasian males, African females and African males all preferred similar levels of facial adiposity for attractiveness and health. Despite the evidence that there is agreement across cultures on facial adiposity levels associated with health and attractiveness, there does appear to be some degree of divergence in BMI preference judged from facial adiposity and BMI preferences as judged from bodies across cultures. For example, a study by Tovée et al. (2006) found that ethnic South African Zulus preferred a BMI that is above the upper limit of the normal range for both health and attractiveness when judging female bodies. Since most studies on facial adiposity preferences utilize university students, Coetzee et al. (2012) argues that this discrepancy could potentially be attributed to repeated exposure to Western media ideals and improved access to resources that African university students enjoy. This line of reasoning is also supported by Tovée et al. (2006) who found that South African born Zulus who immigrated to the United Kingdom preferred a lower BMI for both attractiveness and health compared to Zulus still residing in South Africa (also see Tovée et al., 2007). At present, more research is needed to elucidate the cultural and environmental factors associated with facial adiposity preferences across cultures.

There also appears to be relative differences between males and females when it comes to judgments of facial adiposity levels required for a healthy appearance for female faces. Coetzee et al. (2011) report that females preferred a slightly lower BMI (19.76 kg/m2) for attractiveness compared to optimum health (20.84 kg/m2) when rating other females, while no such difference was found for males. One hypothesis that has been put forth to account for this discrepancy, is that socio-cultural factors related to judgments of body fat influences and shapes female body weight ideals, especially via media exposure to a thin ideal (Groesz et al., 2002; Harrison and Hefner, 2006; Coetzee et al., 2011). Though the link between facial adiposity and perceived health and attractiveness appears to be remarkably stable, facial adiposity likely interacts in complex ways with various biological, environmental and socio-cultural factors, as well as other facial cues such symmetry, skin color, skin texture, sexual dimorphism and averageness in shaping social and health perceptions derived from faces.

Facial Adiposity and Health Outcomes

One of the primary reasons why researchers are interested in facial adiposity is its potential to act as a robust cue to health outcomes. Given the strong relationship that exists between facial adiposity and BMI, negative health outcomes related to BMI should therefore also be linked to facial adiposity. To date, numerous studies have found a relationship between facial adiposity and actual health outcomes, although the results are not always consistent.

Cardiovascular Health

A study by Coetzee et al. (2009) found that facial adiposity was associated with systolic blood pressure (r = 0.28) and diastolic blood pressure (r = 0.43), as well as a cardiovascular-illness component (r = 0.41). In addition to the results reported by Coetzee et al. (2009) a study by Reither et al. (2009) also found a link between perceived facial adiposity and cardiovascular health. Reither et al. (2009) used 3,027 yearbook photographs of teenagers from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS), which also captured basic health data from 1957 to 2004. The researchers found that increased facial adiposity ratings in adolescent faces were associated with high blood pressure (OR = 1.25), heart trouble (OR = 1.20) and heart disease (OR = 1.69) later in life. Recently a study by Stephen et al. (2017) also reported that a geometric morphometric model that captured aspects of facial shape from images, could be used to predict 21% of the variance in blood pressure in their sample. These results indicate that there is a robust link between cardiovascular health and facial adiposity.

Immunity

Coetzee et al. (2009) reported that higher levels of rated facial adiposity was associated with an increase in the number of colds and flu's participants reported (r = 0.209), bout length of those reported colds and flu's (r = 0.24), the frequency of antibiotics use reported by participants (χ2 = 11.706) and a respiratory-illness component (r = 0.29). These relationships remained significant, even after the researchers controlled for parental income and age. A study on male participants by Rantala et al. (2013a) found an association between perceived facial adiposity and immunity (β = −0.38) using a direct measure of immune function (antibody response to a Hepatitis B vaccination). The researchers also reported that perceived facial adiposity mediated the relationship between antibody response and facial attractiveness. However, a similar study using female participants, found no link between the antibody response produced by a Hepatitis B vaccination and percentage body fat, though it should be mentioned that no direct measures of facial adiposity were included in the study (Rantala et al., 2013b). Although the studies reported above found indications that immune function may be linked to facial adiposity, especially in males, other studies have not produced the same pattern of results when using different measures of immune function. For example, a study by Phalane et al. (2017) using African participants found no significant relationship between adiposity and immune responsiveness as measured by functional cytokine profile and C-reactive proteins in African men. It is worth mentioning, however, that the sample size for the cytokine profile was relatively low (n = 41) and could thus reflect a potential type II error due to a lack of statistical power. A recent study by Foo et al. (2017) also found no relationship between facial adiposity and immune function (bacterial killing capacity, overall bacterial immunity, bacterial suppression capacity, and lysozyme activity) in either males or females. To date, there is some evidence to suggest that facial adiposity and immune function are linked, especially in men, although more research is needed to clarify the potential relationship between different aspects of immune function and facial adiposity.

Mental Health

Relatively few studies have investigated the link between facial adiposity and mental health. A study by Martinson and Vasunilashorn (2016) that was conducted using the same basic methodology applied to the WLS data used by Reither et al. (2009), found that females who were rated as being overweight (M = 8.79) in 1957 scored significantly higher on the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) later in life compared to normal weight females (M = 6.94). No statistically significant difference was found between overweight (M = 6.40) and normal weight (M = 5.97) CES-D scores for male students. Additionally, a study conducted by Tinlin et al. (2013) using female participants, also found that rated facial adiposity was negatively correlated with a psychological condition factor (r = 0.29). These findings suggest that facial adiposity may also serve as a valid cue to mental health in females, although more research is required to draw any conclusions about the link between mental health and facial adiposity in males.

Hormones

The relationship between facial adiposity and various hormone levels has also produced largely mixed results. For example, a study by Tinlin et al. (2013) conducted using female participants reported a significant negative correlation between salivary progesterone levels (rs = −0.30) and facial adiposity, but found no relationship between facial adiposity and estradiol levels, though the results should be interpreted with caution due to the small sample size (n = 49). Rantala et al. (2013a) found a significant relationship between circulating testosterone and facial adiposity in men (r = −0.52). Han et al. (2016) found no relationship between facial adiposity and cortisol levels in females, while Rantala et al., 2013b also found no relationship between cortisol levels and percentage body fat in females. To date, most of the research done on the relationship between hormones and facial adiposity has been done using female participants.

Other Health Outcomes

In addition to cardiovascular health, immune function, mental health and hormone levels, some studies also investigated potential links between facial adiposity and other health outcomes such as diabetes and arthritis or indicators of health such as oxidative stress and sperm health. For example, Reither et al. (2009) found that adolescents who were rated as having higher levels of facial adiposity were also more likely to experience muscle aches (OR = 1.13), shortness of breath (OR = 1.10) and chest pains (OR = 1.21) during the course of their lives, but also showed an increased risk of developing arthritis (OR = 1.19), diabetes (OR = 1.44), and die of non-accidental causes (OR = 1.32). A study by Foo et al. (2017) investigated the relationship between facial adiposity and oxidative DNA damage, as well as level of lipid peroxidation. The researchers found a significant relationship (r = −0.22) between facial adiposity and urinary 8-hydroxy-2′ –deoxyguanosine (8-OHdG), a biomarker for oxidative stress and carcinogenesis (Valavanidis et al., 2009) for males, but not for females. However, Foo et al. (2017) also reported that no relationship existed between facial adiposity and a measure of lipid peroxidation (isoprostane levels) in either males or females. Lastly, Foo et al. (2017) found no relationship between facial adiposity and sperm health (rapid progressive motility, linearity of sperm movement, sperm concentration and percentage motile sperm) for males. A study by Tinlin et al. (2013) investigated the relationship between facial adiposity and general health markers in female participants. The researchers found that rated facial adiposity was not correlated with a physical condition factor, although the researcher did note that there was evidence for high collinearity between the physical condition and psychological condition factors in their study. A combination of these factors (labeled general condition factor), displayed a moderate association with facial adiposity (r = 0.41).

When combined, the results from these studies indicate that facial adiposity can serve as a reliable indicator of actual health outcomes including immune function, cardiovascular health, respiratory health, mental health and oxidative stress, although more research is needed to elucidate the link between these health outcomes and facial adiposity. The next section briefly touches upon factors that can potentially influence or mediate the relationship between facial adiposity and judgments of attractiveness and health.

Table 3 presents a summary of studies investigating the relationship between facial adiposity and health outcomes.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of studies that investigated the relationship between facial adiposity and health outcomes.

Factors That Potentially Contribute to Adiposity Preferences

While numerous studies have found a robust link between facial adiposity and perceptions of health and attractiveness, a number of potential mediating factors have been identified that could serve to modify our facial adiposity preferences. A study by Re et al. (2011) demonstrated that our preferences for particular adiposity levels can be altered, at least in the short term. First, the researchers used a pre-test to establish a baseline for participants' facial adiposity preferences. By exposing participants to either “plus-sized” female bodies or regular female bodies, the researchers could induce an after-effect where participants who viewed the “plus-size” bodies increased their adiposity preference by an average of 0.5 kg/m2. Participants thus preferred faces higher in adiposity after viewing the “plus-size” bodies. This finding illustrates an adaptation effect, whereby exposure to certain stimuli can create a preference shift toward that particular type of stimulus. According to the authors, this after-effect could be an indication that overall attractiveness judgments require integration of multiple cues, though these cues are often investigated independently.

The idea of an adaptation effect is also related to repeated exposure to facial cues found in our own ethnicity, or ethnicities that we are regularly exposed to. In a study by Schneider et al. (2013) Japanese and German participants were asked to provide body weight estimates for both German and Japanese individuals based on facial photographs alone. The researchers found that German observers tended to slightly overestimate the body weight of the Japanese faces, while the Japanese participants significantly underestimated the body weight of the German faces. The researchers argue that one potential explanation for the gross underestimation of body weight for German faces by Japanese participants, is that Japanese participants utilized an inappropriate reference point regarding the link between facial shape and body weight for German faces. Since Japanese faces tend to display more rounded and broad (brachycephalic) head proportions, German faces associated with an average body weight appeared more slender, compared to a Japanese face associated with and average body weight, which Japanese participants would have been more familiar with. These findings imply that accurate weight perceptions derived from facial cues, is also contingent upon reasonable exposure and familiarity with the association between facial phenotypic variation and body weight of a particular ethnic group.

Another factor that can play a significant role in influencing people's adiposity preferences, is potential pathogen exposure and how it relates to our behavioral immune system. Studies have shown that levels of pathogen disgust sensitivity can have a dramatic influence on individuals' perceptions of facial cues associated with attractiveness or perceived health (Park et al., 2012; Jones et al., 2013). These result suggest that a pathogen disgust reaction may indeed play a role in our relative tolerance for facial cues that may signal ill-health. A study by Fisher et al. (2013) found that men with higher pathogen disgust responses showed preferred facial cues associated with lower weight, indicating that individual differences in pathogen disgust also play a role in relative levels of facial adiposity that people find attractive or perceive to be healthy. These result are also supported by Rantala et al. (2013a) who found that facial adiposity mediated the association between facial attractiveness and immune response in men. However, a study by Dixson et al. (2017) found no relationship between facial adiposity preferences and malarial prevalence for people living in urban or rural areas in Vanuatu, a series of small Pacific islands. These results suggest that the relationship between facial adiposity preferences and pathogen exposure or disgust, while theoretically elegant, have to be interpreted with caution.

Environmental pressures also appear to play a role in shaping our preferences for facial or body adiposity. For example, it has been demonstrated that resource scarcity could play a role in males' preference of breast size, with men from lower socio-economic regions preferring larger breasts, due to breast size being a good indicator of adipose tissue reserves (Dixson et al., 2011; Swami and Tovée, 2013). A study by Batres and Perrett (2014) found that people in rural areas of El Salvador tend to prefer female faces with higher levels of adiposity, compared to people from urban areas. One hypothesis that has been proposed to account for this difference is the fact that rural areas, especially in developing nations, tend to be poorer and people from these areas tend to have less access to resources such as food or medicine. A similar study conducted by Swami and Tovée (2007) also provide evidence that people from resource-poor areas of Thailand preferred female bodies that were higher in BMI compared to people from more industrialized areas of Thailand. A more recent study by Batres et al. (2017) replicated the results of Batres and Perrett (2014) in another cohort of people from El Salvador, as well as Malaysia. What is especially interesting about these results is the fact that a difference in preference for facial adiposity levels between more developed and poorer areas was only found for females faces. One possible reason for this is that a trade-off exists in poorer areas, where males have to weigh the potential negative long-term health outcomes associated with higher levels of adiposity and more immediate concerns regarding survival and reproductive fitness of females (Batres et al., 2017). Another possible contributor to this phenomenon could be increased media exposure to thin ideals in more developed areas, which then serves to alter people's baseline expectations of attractiveness or health, via and adaptation effect as described by Re et al. (2011) and Batres and Perrett (2014). It should be pointed out, however, that there are studies that problematize the view that people in resource-poor areas prefer bodies or faces associated with a slightly higher BMI. For example, a study by Dixson et al. (2017) found no evidence to support the hypothesis that males from rural area would prefer females faces associated with higher levels of facial adiposity. A series of studies conducted in China, Papua New Guinea, Cameroon, Indonesia, Samoa and New Zealand, also found a high degree of cross-cultural consensus when participants were asked to judge female body attractiveness, with both males and females preferring a lower waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) regardless of BMI (Dixson et al., 2010a,b; Singh et al., 2010). These results suggest that sexually dimorphic fat distribution could in fact be more important than overall BMI when judging female body attractiveness, even across ethnicities or socio-economic regions.

A study by Weston et al. (2015) revealed that judgments of weight in faces can even be influenced by the expression of the facial stimuli. The researchers observed that participants tended to rate male faces containing sad expressions as more overweight compared male faces containing a neutral facial expression. A study by Henderson et al. (2016) also found that downward mouth curvature was negatively correlated with perceived health (r = −0.20) and upward mouth curvature was positively correlated with perceived health (r = 0.51), presumably because a frown is associated with a sad facial expression that reflect a negative mood or mental state. It is thus likely that facial cues are also evaluated within the context of emotional attributions, adding yet another layer of complexity to the way in which we judge physical and emotional health from facial cues in others.

Methodological Considerations in the Study of Facial Adiposity

In the laboratory there are several factors related to the facial stimuli that can influence the outcome of a study. For example, a study by Jones et al. (2012) found that perceptions and the derived judgments of facial stimuli can be altered by changing the viewing angle of the face. According to the authors, this effect is likely attributable to directional asymmetries in the perception of facial cues related to shape. A study by Russell et al. (2016) found that facial contrast can also influence perceptions of health within the face, with faces low in contrast being judged as less healthier. These two studies highlight the importance of controlling for subtle factors such as viewing angle and facial contrast in the generation of facial stimuli, as they could potentially confound the results of the study.

In addition to the way in which stimuli are generated, statistical and research design considerations also need to be taken into account when conducting facial morphology research. For example, Windhager et al. (2018) proposes that a major drawback of the traditional reliance on p-values in the analysis of data derived from facial stimuli, is the fact that facial morphology research often involves complex nested designs. Most designs in the field use human raters to rate multiple facial stimuli on one or more dimensions. Researchers then often calculate a mean rating for a particular face and although inter-rater reliability is mostly high (α > 0.80), the variance associated with rater responses is usually lost when rater responses are collapsed to a single mean. Many studies also use suboptimal sample sizes (n > 100) due to the complexity of the designs, which can lead to a lack of statistical power in detecting smaller effect sizes (see for example Tinlin et al., 2013; Phalane et al., 2017). In reality, collinearity concerns are likely always going to be a potential problem researchers will have to deal with, due to the complex facial cue integration that happens within an organic judgment faces. However, recent advances in the creation of facial morphs can potentially allow researchers to manipulate facial cue dimensions with increasing precision, while also theoretically isolating specific facial cues within a panel of facial stimuli (Windhager et al., 2013, 2018; Re and Perrett, 2014). Bottom-up or data-driven methodologies can also offer researchers a way to increase the precision with which facial cues are specified by reducing shape coordinates to a set of principle components that can be used in further analysis (Wolffhechel et al., 2015; Henderson et al., 2016).

Estimating BMI From Facial Adiposity

One of the key components of utilizing weight related facial cues to make inferences about people's health is that people should be able to accurately judge body mass from facial cues alone. To our knowledge no study has provided a quantitative synthesis of the link between facial adiposity and judgments of body weight across different studies published in the field. A meta-analysis was therefore conducted to evaluate the strength of the relationship between judgments of facial adiposity and BMI/ percentage body fat.

Methods

Inclusion Criteria

All peer-reviewed journal articles that provided an effect size that quantifies the relationship between perceived facial adiposity and estimations of BMI or percentage body fat were included in the meta-analysis. Studies that reported more than one effect size based upon the same facial stimuli were excluded from the analysis due to potential biases in heterogeneity estimates. For example, a study by Coetzee et al. (2012) calculated a correlation coefficient for the relationship between perceived facial adiposity and BMI, as well as percentage body fat for the same participants. Although two effect sizes can be extracted from this study, the two effect sizes obtained from the same group would not be statistically independent. Similarly, Fisher et al. (2014a) used the same facial stimuli as Fisher et al. (2013), thus the effect sizes obtained from these two studies would also not be statistically independent. The effect size for the relationship between facial adiposity and percentage body fat from Coetzee et al. (2012) and the study by Fisher et al. (2014a) were thus excluded from the analysis. No restriction criteria were placed on the age, gender or ethnicity of either the facial stimuli or raters in studies to be included in the meta-analysis.

Search Strategy

A systematic review protocol was developed in accordance with the Preferred Reporting for Items for Systematic Reviews and MetaAnalyses-Protocols (PRISMA) (Moher et al., 2009). The PRISMA protocols can easily be reproduced by other researchers thereby ensuring transparency and reliability of meta-analyses (Gurevitch et al., 2018). The use of PRISMA protocols is becoming increasingly popular within the field of evolutionary psychology (Geniole et al., 2015; see for example Gouda-Vossos et al., 2018). In line with the protocol developed for this study, all studies that reported an effect size for the relationship between perceived facial adiposity and BMI or percentage body fat were identified by searching relevant databases for peer-reviewed articles that contained the phrase “facial adiposity”: Pubmed (9), EbscoHost (27), Science Direct (66), ProQuest (66), and Web of Science (21). Once databases were searched, reference lists of prominent published articles were also searched and any additional relevant abstracts were added. Results were entered into the open-source software management package Zotero Version 5.0. Duplicate entries were removed, after which abstracts were screened for relevance in line with the inclusion criteria by the authors. After the initial screening procedure, the full-text articles were downloaded for the remaining list and re-screened for relevance. See Figure 1 for the PRISMA flow diagram.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart for selection of studies.

Data Extraction

A data extraction template was used to capture all the relevant data from the articles selected for inclusion in the meta-analysis. The data extraction sheets included detailed information on the types of stimuli including gender, age, BMI/percentage body fat, ethnicity, as well as the rating scale used and if the facial stimuli were captured in 2D or 3D. Demographic information on the raters was included as well, including gender, age and ethnicity. Finally, effect sizes were captured for all relevant studies.

Data Synthesis

All extracted effect sizes were converted to Pearson's r before any analyses were conducted. As discussed previously, studies often use the same facial stimuli or raters multiple times during a study, thus creating a complex nested design structure. This nested structure means that it is not unusual for a particular study to produce two or more analysis outcomes that were derived from highly correlated or identical sources within the study. If this statistical dependency is not adequately accounted for in the analysis procedures used by the researchers, they risk introducing bias in the variance estimation during the analysis (Hunter and Schmidt, 2004). In order to account for the within-study statistical dependence, we aggregated all effect sizes within each study to produce a single estimated effect size for that study using the “Agg” function contained in the “Mac” package for R (Del Re and Hoyt, 2012; R Core Team, 2013). As information on the true correlations of within-study effect sizes were unavailable, all within-study effect size correlations were fixed at 0.50 (Wampold et al., 1997). This aggregation procedure produced seven effect size estimates, one corresponding to each study. Due to key differences between studies regarding selection and presentation of facial stimuli, as well as sample composition, meta-analyses were conducted using random-effect models (Borenstein et al., 2010; Quintana, 2015). The aggregated Pearson's r for each study was transformed to a Fisher's z scale, as r is not normally distributed. These effect size estimates were back transformed where appropriate in the presentation of the results. See Table 4 for a summary of studies included in the meta-analysis.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Summary of studies that investigated the relationship between facial adiposity and BMI/Percentage body fat.

Results

A random effects model weighted by sample size was performed using the “metaphor” package in R (Viechtbauer, 2010). The analysis revealed a strong positive overall correlation between perceived facial adiposity ratings and BMI/percentage body fat [r = 0.71; 95% CI (0.66, 0.76), p < 0.001]. Heterogeneity statistics revealed no statistically significant between-study heterogeneity [Q = 7.12 (df = 6), p = 0.31; I2 = 19.36%; τ2 0.004; τ = 0.06]. According to guidelines published by Higgins et al. (2003), an I2 proportion below 25% is a good indicator of low between-studies variability and can serve as a more robust indicator of between-study variance in small sample sizes.

Sensitivity analysis was conducted to investigate if any of the studies included in the analysis contributed disproportionately to heterogeneity. Outlier and influential case diagnostics were conducted, which indicate that only Coetzee et al. (2012) (Study 6 in Figure 2) displayed signs of being a potential outlier or influential case (Viechtbauer and Cheung, 2010). See Figure 3 for influential case diagnostics. To test if the removal of Coetzee et al. (2012) from the analysis would influence the overall model fit, the study was excluded and the model re-fitted. The re-fitted model did not make a significant impact on the interpretation of the overall results however [r = 0.70; 95% CI (0.65, 0.76), p < 0.001]. Moderator analysis also revealed that the ethnicity of the facial stimuli (African or Caucasian) did not have a moderating effect on the relationship between perceived facial adiposity ratings and BMI/ percentage body fat [Q (1), 0.50, p = 0.48].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Forest plot of relationship between perceived facial adiposity and BMI/ percentage body fat.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Plot of the (A) studentized deleted residuals; (B) DFFITS values; (C) Cook's distances; (D) covariance ratios; (E) estimates of τ2; (F) test statistics for (residual) heterogeneity; (G) hat values; and (H) weight for the 7 studies included in the analysis.

Risk of Bias

Figure 4 shows a funnel plot of correlation coefficients plotted against standard errors for each study. Egger's regression test showed no evidence of any asymmetry in the funnel plot. Due to the relatively low sample size (n = 7) of the study, visual symmetry was hard to estimate and therefore no trim-and-fill methods were used.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Funnel plot of standard errors by correlation coefficient.

Discussion

Facial adiposity has consistently been linked to perceptions of attractiveness and health, with heavier faces being judged to be more unattractive and unhealthier. To date, facial adiposity has also been linked to a number of actual health outcomes including: cold and flu number, duration of colds and flu, frequency of antibiotic use, respiratory illness, blood pressure, cardiovascular illness, salivary progesterone, psychological well-being, arthritis, diabetes, circulating testosterone, immune function, and oxidative stress. While a strong relationship between facial adiposity, attractiveness, perceived health and actual health outcomes has been reported, there are a few limitations to the current evidence presented in favor of facial adiposity as an important contributor to health and attractiveness judgments. It is noteworthy that the majority of the studies published on facial adiposity used Caucasian students in their early to mid-twenties as both stimuli and raters, with isolated studies being done on Asian, Hispanic and African faces. A study by Batres and Perrett (2014) also found that people without internet access preferred female faces with higher levels of adiposity compared to people with internet access. This finding highlights a key shortcoming in the field, as most current published studies may provide a biased or incomplete picture of the way in which attractiveness and health judgments are made from facial cues related to body weight. It is therefore important for researchers to expand the diversity of sampled populations in future studies, as environmental pressures, and media exposure to Western weight ideals may differ substantially from region to region or culture to culture. It is also recommended that studies take socio-economic status of participants into account if possible.

There also appears to be important differences in judgments made by males and females regarding adiposity as a cue to health and attractiveness. For example, a link has been found between adiposity and immune responsiveness in male Rantala et al., 2013a, but not female faces Rantala et al., 2013b. There is also at least some evidence to suggest that Western body ideals may have a differential effect on males and females, where ideal attractiveness is associated with a lower BMI compared to ideal weight for females (Coetzee et al., 2011). Due to the fact that research in the field is highly reliant on cross-gender judgments of health and attractiveness, most studies would be well-advised to be sensitive to the nuanced differences in how people of opposite genders judge one another based on facial cues related to adiposity, especially across socio-economic or cultural boundaries.

In addition to the socio-economic, cultural and gender factors, researchers also need to pay close attention to stimuli generation and study designs when conducting research. The recent development of 3D facial imaging technology allows researchers to produce 3D facial images that more closely approximate the way people would be able to view another person's face in reality. This is especially important when participants are required to make judgments about facial shape, as 2D facial images contain less overall facial shape information compared to 3D images. Researcher also need to be mindful of the potential drawbacks of using traditional statistical procedures and inadequate sample sizes when the research designs make use of nested structures where multiple participants judge multiple facial stimuli along multiple dimensions.

Despite the limitations presented in this paper, the field of facial morphometrics is a burgeoning field that holds the potential for the development of reliable, inexpensive and non-invasive methods for disease detection and monitoring. For example, the Andreu et al., 2016) is a personal health monitor that integrates a 3D optical scanner, multispectral cameras and gas detection sensor for collecting data of individuals who stand in front of the mirror. One of the key features of the Wize Mirror is the use of facial morphometric analysis to predict cardio-metabolic risk. Studies by Kocabey et al. (2017) and Barr et al. (2018) also reported on the development of Face-to-BMI- Systems that predicts BMI from facial images found on social media platforms, for example. The results reported by these studies indicate that these computer models could predict actual BMI from facial cues alone to a degree of accuracy very similar to that of human observers. With rapid advancement of these types of technologies, exciting possibilities begin to open up for continuous monitoring of health risk associated with BMI.

As mentioned throughout this paper, one of the key conditions that need to be met for facial adiposity to serve as a valid cue to attractiveness and health, is that people need to be able to accurately judge body mass from facial cues. The meta-analysis presented in this review found that participants can reliably estimate BMI from facial cues alone (r = 0.71, n = 458) and that ethnicity of the rated faces did not mediate the relationship between perceived facial adiposity and estimates of body mass. This is an important finding as it demonstrates that cues related to facial adiposity can be reliably detected by participants and also reliably used to make inferences about another person's body weight. Due to the strong relationship between body weight and negative health outcomes, accurate judgment of facial cues related to body weight, is a key factor in allowing us to make inferences about another person's health.

Author Contributions

SdJ and VC contributed conception and design of the study; SdJ and VC organized the database; SdJ performed the statistical analysis; SdJ and VC wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The preparation of this paper was financially supported by a Department of Higher Education and Training (DHET) Research Development Grant and a National Research Foundation (NRF) Competitive Programme for Rated Researchers (99078).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Andreu, Y., Chiarugi, F., Colantonio, S., Giannakakis, G., Giorgi, D., Henriquez, P., et al. (2016). Wize Mirror - a smart, multisensory cardio-metabolic risk monitoring system. Comput. Vis. Image Underst. 148, 3–22. doi: 10.1016/j.cviu.2016.03.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axelsson, J., Sundelin, T., Olsson, M. J., Sorjonen, K., Axelsson, C., Lasselin, J., et al. (2018). Identification of acutely sick people and facial cues of sickness. Proc. Biol. Sci. 285:20172430. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.2430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, M., Guo, G., Colby, S., Olfert, M., Barr, M. L., Guo, G., et al. (2018). Detecting body mass index from a facial photograph in lifestyle intervention. Technologies 6:83. doi: 10.3390/technologies6030083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batres, C., Kannan, M., and Perrett, D. I. (2017). Familiarity with own population's appearance influences facial preferences. Hum. Nat. Interdiscip. Biosoc. Perspect. 28, 344–354. doi: 10.1007/s12110-017-9289-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batres, C., and Perrett, D. I. (2014). The influence of the digital divide on face preferences in el salvador: people without internet access prefer more feminine men, more masculine women, and women with higher adiposity. PLoS ONE 9:e100966. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0100966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batres, C., and Perrett, D. I. (2017). How the harsh environment of an army training camp changes human (Homo sapiens) facial preferences. Ethology 123, 61–68. doi: 10.1111/eth.12571

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boothroyd, L. G., Scott, I., Gray, A. W., Coombes, C. I., and Pound, N. (2013). Male facial masculinity as a cue to health outcomes. Evol. Psychol. 11, 1044–1058. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., and Rothstein, H. R. (2010). A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 1, 97–111. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, Z., Hahn, A. C., Zhang, W., Holzleitner, I. J., Lee, A. J., DeBruine, L. M., et al. (in press). No evidence that facial attractiveness femininity, averageness, or coloration are cues to susceptibility to infectious illnesses in a university sample of young adult women. Evol. Hum. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.10.002.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coetzee, V. (2011). The Effect of Weight on Health and Face Perception: A Cross-Cultural Perspective. Available online at: https://search.proquest.com/docview/1512387626?accountid=14717

Coetzee, V., Chen, J., Perrett, D. I., and Stephen, I. D. (2010). Deciphering faces: quantifiable visual cues to weight. Perception 39, 51–61. doi: 10.1068/p6560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coetzee, V., Faerber, S. J., Greeff, J. M., Lefevre, C. E., Re, D. E., and Perrett, D. I. (2012). African perceptions of female attractiveness. PLoS ONE 7:e48116. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coetzee, V., Greeff, J. M., Stephen, I. D., and Perrett, D. I. (2014). Cross-cultural agreement in facial attractiveness preferences: the role of ethnicity and gender. PLoS ONE 9:e99629. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coetzee, V., Perrett, D. I., and Stephen, I. D. (2009). Facial adiposity: a cue to health? Perception 38, 1700–1711. doi: 10.1068/p6423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coetzee, V., Re, D., Perrett, D. I., Tiddeman, B. P., and Xiao, D. (2011). Judging the health and attractiveness of female faces: is the most attractive level of facial adiposity also considered the healthiest? Body Image 8, 190–193. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Re, A., and Hoyt, W. (2012). MAc: Meta-Analysis With Correlations. R package version 1.1. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MAc

Després, J. P., and Lemieux, I. (2006). Abdominal obesity and metabolic syndrome. Nature. 444, 881–887. doi: 10.1038/nature05488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, J. B. (2010). The effect of obesity on health outcomes. Mol. Cell. Endocrinol. 316, 104–108. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2009.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J., Li, B., and Dixson, A. F. (2010a). Female waist-to-hip ratio, body mass index and sexual attractiveness in China. Curr. Zool. 56, 175–181.

Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J., Little, A. C., Dixson, H. G., and Brooks, R. C. (2017). Do prevailing environmental factors influence human preferences for facial morphology? Behav. Ecol. 28, 1217–1227. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arx067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J., Sagata, K., Linklater, W. L., and Dixson, A. F. (2010b). Male preferences for female waist-to-hip ratio and body mass index in the highlands of Papua New Guinea. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 141, 620–625. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J., Vasey, P. L., Sagata, K., Sibanda, N., Linklater, W. L., and Dixson, A. F. (2011). Men's preferences for women's breast morphology in New Zealand, Samoa, and Papua New Guinea. Arch. Sex. Behav. 40, 1271–1279. doi: 10.1007/s10508-010-9680-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobner, J., and Kaser, S. (2018). Body mass index and the risk of infection - from underweight to obesity. Clin. Microbiol. Infect. 24, 24–28. doi: 10.1016/j.cmi.2017.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, C. I., Fincher, C. L., Hahn, A. C., DeBruine, L. M., and Jones, B. C. (2013). Individual differences in pathogen disgust predict men's, but not women's, preferences for facial cues of weight. Personal. Individ. Differ. 55, 860–863. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, C. I., Fincher, C. L., Hahn, A. C., Little, A. C., DeBruine, L. M., and Jones, B. C. (2014a). Do assortative preferences contribute to assortative mating for adiposity? Br. J. Psychol. 105, 474–485. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, C. I., Hahn, A. C., DeBruine, L. M., and Jones, B. C. (2014b). Integrating shape cues of adiposity and color information when judging facial health and attractiveness. Perception 43, 499–508. doi: 10.1068/p7728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstad, I., and Karter, A. (1992). Parasites, bright males, and the immunocompetence handicap. Am. Nat. 139, 603–622. doi: 10.1086/285346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foo, Y. Z., Simmons, L. W., and Rhodes, G. (2017). Predictors of facial attractiveness and health in humans. Sci. Rep. Nat. Publ. Group 7:39731. doi: 10.1038/srep39731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geniole, S. N., Denson, T. F., Dixson, B. J., Carré, J. M., and McCormick, C. M. (2015). Evidence from Meta-analyses of the facial width-to-height ratio as an evolved cue of threat. PLoS ONE 10:e0132726. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gouda-Vossos, A., Nakagawa, S., Dixson, B. J., and Brooks, R. C. (2018). Mate choice copying in humans: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 4, 364–386. doi: 10.1007/s40750-018-0099-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grammer, K., and Thornhill, R. (1994). Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual selection: the role of symmetry and averageness. J. Comp. Psychol. 108, 233–242. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.108.3.233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., and Murnen, S. K. (2002). The effect of experimental presentation of thin media images on body satisfaction: a meta-analytic review. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 31, 1–16. doi: 10.1002/eat.10005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gurevitch, J., Koricheva, J., Nakagawa, S., and Stewart, G. (2018). Meta-analysis and the science of research synthesis. Nature 555, 175–182. doi: 10.1038/nature25753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, W. D., and Zuk, M. (1982). Heritable true fitness and bright birds: a role for parasites? Science 218, 384–387. doi: 10.1126/science.7123238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, C., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., Debruine, L. M., and Jones, B. C. (2016). Women's facial attractiveness is related to their body mass index but not their salivary cortisol. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 28, 352–355. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22792

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, K., and Hefner, V. (2006). Media exposure, current and future body ideals, and disordered eating among preadolescent girls: a longitudinal panel study. J. Youth Adolesc. 35, 146–156. doi: 10.1007/s10964-005-9008-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, A. J., Holzleitner, I. J., Talamas, S. N., and Perrett, D. I. (2016). Perception of health from facial cues. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 371:20150380. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, J. J. A., and Anglin, J. M. (2003). Facial attractiveness predicts longevity. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 351–356. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00036-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., and Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327, 557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, J., and Schmidt, F. (2004). Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings. 2nd Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Jones, A. L., Kramer, R. S. S., and Ward, R. (2012). Signals of personality and health: the contributions of facial shape, skin texture, and viewing angle. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 38, 1353–1361. doi: 10.1037/a0027078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, B. C., Fincher, C. L., Welling, L. L. M., Little, A. C., Feinberg, D. R., Watkins, C. D., et al. (2013). Salivary cortisol and pathogen disgust predict men's preferences for feminine shape cues in women's faces. Biol. Psychol. 92, 233–240. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., and Perrett, D. I. (2004). When facial attractiveness is only skin deep. Perception 33, 569–576. doi: 10.1068/p3463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Tiddeman, B. P., Burt, D. M., and Perrett, D. I. (2001). Facial symmetry and judgements of apparent health: Support for a “good genes” explanation of the attractiveness–symmetry relationship. Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 417–429. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00083-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalick, S. M., Zebrowitz, L. A., Langlois, J. H., and Johnson, R. M. (1998). Does human facial attractiveness honestly advertise health? longitudinal data on an evolutionary question. Psychol. Sci. 9, 8–13. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klaczynski, P., Daniel, D. B., and Keller, P. S. (2009). Appearance idealization, body esteem, causal attributions, and ethnic variations in the development of obesity stereotypes. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 30, 537–551. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kocabey, E., Camurcu, M., Ofli, F., Aytar, Y., Marin, J., Torralba, A., et al. (2017). Face-to-BMI: using computer vision to infer body mass index on social media. arXiv:1703.03156v1 [cs.HC].

Google Scholar

Kopelman, P. (2007). Health risks associated with overweight and obesity. Obes. Rev. 8, 13–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2007.00311.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., and Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol. Bull. 126, 390–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, B. J., and Kim, J. Y. (2014). Predicting visceral obesity based on facial characteristics. BMC Complement. Altern. Med. 14:248. doi: 10.1186/1472-6882-14-248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, J. A., Ray, A., and Jensen, M. D. (1998). Relation between chubby cheeks and visceral fat. N. Engl. J. Med. 339, 1946–1947. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199812243392619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, A. C., Burriss, R. P., Jones, B. C., and Roberts, S. C. (2007). Facial appearance affects voting decisions. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., and DeBruine, L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366, 1638–1659. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinson, M. L., and Vasunilashorn, S. M. (2016). The long-arm of adolescent weight status on later life depressive symptoms. Age Ageing 45, 389–395. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afw020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, C., Windhager, S., Schaefer, K., and Mitteroecker, P. (2017). BMI and WHR are reflected in female facial shape and texture: a geometric morphometric image analysis. PLoS ONE 12:e0169336. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0169336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G., and PRISMA Group (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowak, J., Pawłowski, B., Borkowska, B., Augustyniak, D., and Drulis-Kawa, Z. (2018). No evidence for the immunocompetence handicap hypothesis in male humans. Sci. Rep. 8:7392. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25694-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, J. H., van Leeuwen, F., and Stephen, I. D. (2012). Homeliness is in the disgust sensitivity of the beholder: relatively unattractive faces appear especially unattractive to individuals higher in pathogen disgust. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 569–577. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrett, D. I., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., Lee, K. J., Rowland, D. A., and Edwards, R. (1999). Symmetry and Human Facial Attractiveness. Evol. Hum. Behav. 20, 295–307. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00014-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., et al. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature 394, 884–887. doi: 10.1038/29772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phalane, K. G., Tribe, C., Steel, H. C., Cholo, M. C., and Coetzee, V. (2017). Facial appearance reveals immunity in African men. Sci. Rep. Nat. Publ. Group 7, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-08015-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quintana, D. S. (2015). From pre-registration to publication: a non-technical primer for conducting a meta-analysis to synthesize correlational data. Front. Psychol. 6:1549. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2013). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Rantala, M. J., Coetzee, V., Moore, F. R., Skrinda, I., Kecko, S., Krama, T., et al. (2013a). Adiposity, compared with masculinity, serves as a more valid cue to immunocompetence in human mate choice. Proc. Biol. Sci. 280:20122495. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rantala, M. J., Coetzee, V., Moore, F. R., Skrinda, I., Kecko, S., Krama, T., et al. (2013b). Facial attractiveness is related to women's cortisol and body fat, but not with immune responsiveness. Biol. Lett. 9:20130255. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Re, D. E., Coetzee, V., Xiao, D., Buls, D., Tiddeman, B. P., Boothroyd, L. G., et al. (2011). Viewing heavy bodies enhances preferences for facial adiposity. J. Evol. Psychol. 9, 295–308. doi: 10.1556/JEP.9.2011.4.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Re, D. E., Holzleitner, I. J., Coetzee, V., Tiddeman, B. P., Xiao, D. K., and Perrett, D. I. (2013). Wie viel gewicht liegt im gesicht? J. Für Ästhetische Chir. 6, 141–146. doi: 10.1007/s12631-013-0240-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Re, D. E., and Perrett, D. I. (2014). The effects of facial adiposity on attractiveness and perceived leadership ability. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2006 67, 676–686. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.825635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Re, D. E., and Rule, N. O. (2016a). “Appearance and physiognamy,” in APA Handbook of Nonverbal Communication, eds. D. Matsumoto, H. Hwang, and M. Frank (Washington DC: American Psychological Association), 221–256.

Re, D. E., and Rule, N. O. (2016b). Heavy matters: the relationship between just noticeable differences in perceptions of facial adiposity and facial attractiveness. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 7, 69–76. doi: 10.1177/1948550615599829

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reither, E. N., Hauser, R. M., and Swallen, K. C. (2009). Predicting adult health and mortality from adilescent facial characteristics in yearbook photographs*. Demography 46, 27–41. doi: 10.1353/dem.0.0037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 199–226. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G., Yoshikawa, S., Palermo, R., Simmons, L. W., Peters, M., Lee, K., et al. (2007). Perceived health contributes to the attractiveness of facial symmetry, averageness, and sexual dimorphism. Perception 36, 1244–1252. doi: 10.1068/p5712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, G., Zebrowitz, L. A., Clark, A., Kalick, S. M., Hightower, A., and McKay, R. (2001). Do facial averageness and symmetry signal health? Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 31–46. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00060-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritz, B. W., and Gardner, E. M. (2006). Malnutrition and energy restriction differentially affect viral immunity. J. Nutr. 136, 1141–1144. doi: 10.1093/jn/136.5.1141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, M. L., Buchanan, K. L., and Evans, M. R. (2004). Testing the immunocompetence handicap hypothesis: a review of the evidence. Anim. Behav. 68, 227–239. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, R., Porcheron, A., Sweda, J. R., Jones, A. L., Mauger, E., and Morizot, F. (2016). Facial contrast is a cue for perceiving health from the face. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 42:1354. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheib, J. E., Gangestad, S. W., and Thornhill, R. (1999). Facial attractiveness, symmetry and cues of good genes. Proc. Biol. Sci. 266, 1913–1917. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, T. M., Hecht, H., Stevanov, J., and Carbon, C.-C. (2013). Cross-ethnic assessment of body weight and height on the basis of faces. Personal. Individ. Differ. 55, 356–360. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shackelford, T. K., and Larsen, R. J. (1999). Facial attractiveness and physical health. Evol. Hum. Behav. 20, 71–76. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00036-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silventoinen, K., Jelenkovic, A., Sund, R., Yokoyama, Y., Hur, Y.-M., Cozen, W., et al. (2017). Differences in genetic and environmental variation in adult BMI by sex, age, time period, and region: an individual-based pooled analysis of 40 twin cohorts. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 106, 457–466. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.117.153643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, D., Dixson, B. J., Jessop, T. S., Morgan, B., and Dixson, A. F. (2010). Cross-cultural consensus for waist–hip ratio and women's attractiveness. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 176–181.

Google Scholar

Stephen, I. D., Coetzee, V., and Perrett, D. I. (2011). Carotenoid and melanin pigment coloration affect perceived human health. Evol. Hum. Behav. 32, 216–227. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephen, I. D., Hiew, V., Coetzee, V., Tiddeman, B. P., and Perrett, D. I. (2017). Facial shape analysis identifies valid cues to aspects of physiological health in Caucasian, Asian, and African populations. Front. Psychol. 8:1883. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephen, I. D., Law Smith, M. J., Stirrat, M. R., and Perrett, D. I. (2009). Facial skin coloration affects perceived health of human faces. Int. J. Primatol. 30, 845–857. doi: 10.1007/s10764-009-9380-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swami, V., and Tovée, M. J. (2007). Differences in attractiveness preferences between observers in low- and high-resource environments in Thailand. J. Evol. Psychol. 5, 149–160. doi: 10.1556/JEP.2007.1005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swami, V., and Tovée, M. J. (2013). Resource security impacts men's female breast size preferences. PLoS ONE 8:e57623. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talamas, S. N., Mavor, K. I., and Perrett, D. I. (2016). Blinded by beauty: attractiveness bias and accurate perceptions of academic performance. PLoS ONE 11:e0148284. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0148284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thornhill, R., and Gangestad, S. W. (1993). Human facial beauty : Averageness, symmetry, and parasite resistance. Hum. Nat. Hawthorne N 4, 237–269. doi: 10.1007/BF02692201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thornhill, R., and Gangestad, S. W. (1999). Facial attractiveness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 452–460. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01403-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thornhill, R., and Gangestad, S. W. (2006). Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 131–144. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tinlin, R. M., Watkins, C. D., Welling, L. L. M., DeBruine, L. M., Al-Dujaili, E. A. S., and Jones, B. C. (2013). Perceived facial adiposity conveys information about women's health. Br. J. Psychol. Lond. Engl. 104, 235–248. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2012.02117.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tovée, M. J., Furnham, A., and Swami, V. (2007). “Healthy body equals beautiful body? changing perceptions of health and attractiveness with shifting socioeconomic status,” in The Body Beautiful: Evolutionary and Sociocultural Perspectives, eds. V. Swami and A. Furnham (London: Palgrave Macmillan UK), 108–128.

Google Scholar

Tovée, M. J., Swami, V., Furnham, A., and Mangalparsad, R. (2006). Changing perceptions of attractiveness as observers are exposed to a different culture. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 443–456. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valavanidis, A., Vlachogianni, T., and Fiotakis, C. (2009). 8-hydroxy-2' -deoxyguanosine (8-OHdG): a critical biomarker of oxidative stress and carcinogenesis. J. Environ. Sci. Health Part C Environ. Carcinog. Ecotoxicol. Rev. 27, 120–139.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw. 36, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v036.i03

CrossRef Full Text

Viechtbauer, W., and Cheung, M. W. L. (2010). Outlier and influence diagnostics for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 1, 112–125. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wampold, B. E., Mondin, G. W., Moody, M., Stich, F., Benson, K., and Ahn, H. (1997). A meta-analysis of outcome studies comparing bona fide psychotherapies: empiricially,“ all must have prizes.” Psychol. Bull. 122:203.

Google Scholar

Weeden, J., and Sabini, J. (2005). Physical attractiveness and health in Western societies: a review. Psychol. Bull. 131, 635–653. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wen, L., and Guo, G. (2013). A computational approach to body mass index prediction from face images. Image Vis. Comput. 31, 392–400. doi: 10.1016/j.imavis.2013.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weston, T. D., Hass, N. C., and Lim, S.-L. (2015). The effect of sad facial expressions on weight judgment. Front. Psychol. 6. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Windhager, S., Bookstein, F. L., Mueller, H., Zunner, E., Kirchengast, S., and Schaefer, K. (2018). Calibrating facial morphs for use as stimuli in biological studies of social perception. Sci. Rep. Nat. Publ. Group 8, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24911-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Windhager, S., Patocka, K., and Schaefer, K. (2013). Body fat and facial shape are correlated in female adolescents. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 25, 847–850. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolffhechel, K., Hahn, A. C., Jarmer, H., Fisher, C. I., Jones, B. C., and DeBruine, L. M. (2015). Testing the utility of a data-driven approach for assessing BMI from face images. PLoS ONE 10:e0140347. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2018). Body Mass Index - BMI. Available online at: http://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/disease-prevention/nutrition/a-healthy-lifestyle/body-mass-index-bmi

Zaccardi, F., Dhalwani, N. N., Papamargaritis, D., Webb, D. R., Murphy, G. J., Davies, M. J., et al. (2017). Nonlinear association of BMI with all-cause and cardiovascular mortality in type 2 diabetes mellitus: a systematic review and meta-analysis of 414,587 participants in prospective studies. Diabetologia 60, 240–248. doi: 10.1007/s00125-016-4162-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Hall, J. A., Murphy, N. A., and Rhodes, G. (2002). Looking smart and looking good: facial cues to intelligence and their origins. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 238–249. doi: 10.1177/0146167202282009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: facial adiposity, attractiveness, perceived health, health outcomes, BMI, percentage body fat, meta-analysis

Citation: de Jager S, Coetzee N and Coetzee V (2018) Facial Adiposity, Attractiveness, and Health: A Review. Front. Psychol. 9:2562. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02562

Received: 12 September 2018; Accepted: 29 November 2018;
Published: 21 December 2018.

Edited by:

Kok Wei Tan, University of Reading Malaysia, Malaysia

Reviewed by:

Barnaby James Wyld Dixson, The University of Queensland, Australia
Danielle Leigh Wagstaff, Federation University, Australia
Shen Liu, University of Science and Technology of China, China

Copyright © 2018 de Jager, Coetzee and Coetzee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stefan de Jager, gsdejager@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.