Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 March 2019
Sec. Organizational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Analysing Emotional Labor in the Service Industries: Consumer and Business Perspectives View all 11 articles

The Role of Emotional Service Expectation Toward Perceived Quality and Satisfaction: Moderating Effects of Deep Acting and Surface Acting

  • 1School of Ecological Environment and Eco-tourism, Kyungpook National University, Sangju, South Korea
  • 2School of Business, Hanyang University, Seoul, South Korea

A conceptual model articulating the nature of customer expectations and satisfaction over services was proposed with emotional factors. Five propositions about consumer emotional service expectations as a primary antecedent toward confirmation, perceived quality, and satisfaction were provided. As moderators, two dimensions of consumer detection of emotional labor (i.e., detecting deep acting and surface acting) were imposed on each of the relationships. Evidence demonstrated the roles of emotional service expectation in service confirmation and satisfaction. The moderating effects of consumer detections of employees’ emotional strategies were limited to the relationship between emotional service expectation and confirmation; its relationship was weakened by detections of surface acting while the other relationships were not moderated by detections of deep nor surface acting. Structural equation modeling analyses were conducted using online survey data targeting consumers in the hotel industry.

Introduction

Having motivated employees who proactively and professionally engage in customer interactions is undoubtedly critical to service firm’s success (Menguc et al., 2013; Schepers et al., 2016). In the hospitality industry, employees are deeply involved in everyday interactions with customers, such interactions being regarded as essential to the delivery of quality service experiences to customers (Farrell and Oczkowski, 2009). As a result, establishing effective strategies for employee–customer interactions, is recognized as the most important step in enhancing firms’ competitiveness (Lam et al., 2018). Indeed, huge resources are invested in training employees to enhance their interaction performance in the hospitality industry (Schepers et al., 2016).

Some researchers argue that service components are categorized into two attributes: (i) the core attributes that consist of functional quality factors; and (ii) the relational attributes that describe the interpersonally recognized factors during the service delivery (Babin et al., 2004). On the other hand, other researchers suggest that such a classification is artificial and fuzzy because the nature of services involves intangible and experiential consumption (Gaur et al., 2014; Koenig-Lewis and Palmer, 2014). They argue that factors perceived interpersonally by customers can be a core factor that significantly influences service quality, increasing consumer satisfaction and loyalty behaviors. These conflicting arguments lead to a main question of this study: does employees’ emotional service delivery during customer interactions, that is one of the relational attributes, significantly influence service outcomes, such as service confirmation, quality perceptions, and satisfaction? The second research question is as follows: if so, do different detections of employees’ emotional service strategies lead to different customer reactions? Typically, service employees are expected to express positive emotions and to suppress negative emotions in their interactions with customers (Diefendorff and Richard, 2003), but it is inherently impossible always to feel genuine positive emotions for customers (Groth et al., 2009). Consequently, employees attempt to utilize surface acting, faking a positive emotional display, to comply with their job requirements and meet customer needs for positive emotional services (Hochschild, 1983; Lam et al., 2018). However, customers may detect surface acting, and show less positive responses (i.e., service satisfaction and perceived quality) than when detecting deep acting (Lam et al., 2018).

To address these two research questions, we developed a theoretical model consisting of customer emotional expectations, overall confirmation, perceived quality, satisfaction, and two moderators of consumer detection of deep acting and surface acting. Affective elements have been well-reported in relation to the expectation – performance discrepancy link (Burns and Neisner, 2006) and customer satisfaction (Oliver, 1980). Nevertheless, the relationships between customer’s emotional service expectations and consumer satisfaction have not been fully addressed in the service literature. Furthermore, it has not yet been concluded how customer detections of employees’ emotional service strategies influence such relationships among emotional expectation, service quality perception, and satisfaction (Groth et al., 2009). Thus, this study would fill the void in the literature by addressing emotional service expectation and customer detection of the emotional display strategies in the hospitality service context.

Literature Review and Hypotheses

Emotional Service Expectation and Confirmation

Emotional labor can be defined as service employees’ efforts to demonstrate and express desired emotions at work by managing feelings (Hochschild, 1983; Ashforth and Humphrey, 1993; Grandey, 2000). For example, Hochschild (1983) defined emotional labor as “the management of feelings to create a publicly observable facial and bodily display” which is “sold for wage and therefore has exchange value” (p. 7). Similarly, Grandey (2000, p. 97) defined emotional labor as the “process of regulating both feelings and expressions for the organizational goals.” Previous literature has indicated critical roles of emotional labor in the service context. Tsai (2001) stated that employees’ emotional services are all part of the service itself. Hochschild (1983) also pointed out that “the emotional style of offering the service is part of the service itself” (p. 5). The importance of emotional services in confirming customer expectation is unanimously accepted among scholars and practitioners in the service field (Park et al., 2018).

The notion of confirmation has been explained in relation to expectation, which is defined as belief probabilities of the consequences of an event (Roch and Poister, 2006). Burgoon and Walther (1990, p. 236) identified expectation as “what is predicated to occur rather than what is desired.” According to expectation violation theory (Burgoon, 1993), an individual interacts with others with expectancy that refers to what will happen in a given situation and so, the disconfirmation of such expectancy negatively influences outcomes of the interaction. In the hospitality service context, customers seem to have a certain level of emotional service expectation and want to confirm it during the interaction; they expect employees to express positive emotions, such as friendliness, positivity, compassion, and/or warmth, while suppressing negative emotions, such as anger, indifference, or frustration (Beal et al., 2006; Grandey and Gabriel, 2015).

The literature has suggested that customer expectation positively influences confirmation. For example, Park et al. (2018) argued that high expectation increases customers’ involvement levels, leading to easy confirmation. Groth et al. (2009) also empirically demonstrated that positive attitude with high expectation results in high confirmation. Consistent with the results in the existing studies, this study hypothesized that customers’ expectation of emotional services will positively influence overall confirmation of a service outcome.

H1: Emotional service expectation has a positive effect on overall confirmation.

Emotional Service Expectation Toward Perceived Quality and Customer Satisfaction

In their seminal work on SERVQUAL, Parasuraman et al. (1985) indicated that service quality is determined by the evaluation of a service provider by comparing the service provider’s performance with the customer’s expectations of how similar service providers should perform. In other words, customers set the range of expected outcomes of emotional services as a quality standard in which a given service quality is likely to be evaluated (Tsai, 2001; Humphrey et al., 2007). Therefore, quality of services depends on customers’ expectation of how effectively and positively employees express positive emotions. In addition to service expectation, overall confirmation is likely to directly influence quality perception. Parasuraman et al. (1988) stated that service quality results from overall assessments in an integral dimension of expectations. Grönroos (1984) addressed service quality can be measured in the two dimensions of process quality and technical quality, and these two dimensions are intricately connected, indicating service quality is related to overall confirmation. Confirming this argument, Homburg and Stock (2004) argued that quality perception is related to customers’ confirmation of cognitive and affective factors of services. In a hotel setting, satisfying interaction experiences with employees confirmed customer expectation of emotional services and so, enhanced overall service quality perception (Ladhari, 2009). Further, Brady and Cronin (2001) demonstrated quality perception is determined by overall impression of a given service. Since the nature of service involves intangibility and inseparability, service quality seems to be influenced by overall confirmation of expectations. In summary, the literature suggested that perceived quality is influenced by customer expectation and overall confirmation, so we hypothesized as follows:

H2: Emotional service expectations have a positive effect on perceived quality.

H3: Overall confirmation has a positive effect on perceived quality.

Gracia et al. (2011) found that five service quality components, including reliability, assurance, responsiveness, empathy, and tangibles, have positive effects on customer emotions and, as a result, increase satisfaction in the hotel and restaurant service context. Han and Jeong (2013) also empirically demonstrated that customers’ service quality perception significantly influences service experiences and emotional satisfaction. Due to their close relationship, quality perception is often used as an indicator of measures of customer satisfaction (Homburg and Stock, 2004; Wong, 2004).

Ladhari (2009) claimed that satisfaction consists of cognitive (i.e., customer’s judgment) and affective components (i.e., happy, pleasant, and joyful), both of which are evaluated to confirm service expectation. In addition, customer satisfaction seems to be influenced by various external attributes, such as in-store environment and feelings (Han, 2013). Therefore, this study hypothesized that both perceived quality and overall confirmation influence customer satisfaction.

H4: Perceived quality has a positive effect on customer satisfaction.

H5: Overall confirmation has a positive effect on customer satisfaction.

Two Emotional Strategies: Deep and Surface Acting

Emotional services, such as displays of enthusiasm, friendless, and warmth, are identified as the important services of worth in service delivery (Diefendorff et al., 2006; Sutton et al., 2013). The literature has confirmed that emotional services have positive effects on customer perceptions and satisfaction (Groth et al., 2009; Grandey and Gabriel, 2015). Lin and Liang (2011) study also confirmed that displays of positive emotions increase customers’ positive responses, such as customer delight, repurchase intent, and positive word-of-mouth. Similarly, Johanson and Woods (2008) argued that emotional services help achieve organizational goals, such as customer satisfaction and firms’ long-term profitability.

Service firms, in general, require employees to regulate their emotions to express only positive emotions to customers. To comply with the job requirement, employees tend to adopt two main strategies of emotional displays: deep acting and surface acting. Deep acting refers to “good faith” by “putting one’s self in another’s shoes” (Diefendorff et al., 2006) and it is a genuine feeling created within themselves (Hennig-Thurau et al., 2006). Deep acting is regarded as sincerely showing the emotions that match their genuine feelings and organization desires by feeling customers’ feelings and having empathy (Grandey, 2003). In turn, employees who use deep acting are likely to be customer-oriented and so, provide their customers sincere service (Allen et al., 2010). Deep acting has been reported to increase job performance and, as a result, lead to positive feedback from customers (Hatfield et al., 1994). In contrast, surface acting involves “simulating emotions that are not actually felt” (Ashforth and Humphrey, 1993, p. 92). Surface acting occurs when service employees modify only their visible emotions and deceive customers by “putting on a mask,” without actually changing how they feel. Surface acting can bring out an emotional discrepancy between true feelings and expressions because employees just pretend to feel positive to meet the organization’s requirements (Johnson and Spector, 2007).

The Moderating Role of Employee Deep/Surface Acting

Mattila and Enz (2002) stated that customers’ evaluation of service consumption experience depends on how effectively employees display positive emotions during customer interactions. Deep acting strategy allows employees to express their genuine feelings in line with the desired emotions that service firms require, and customers expect (Diefendorff and Greguras, 2009). Deep acting is related to trustworthiness and authenticity that have been known as the main components of service performance to enhance quality perceptions and satisfaction (Krumhuber et al., 2007). Indeed, employees who use deep acting are likely to understand customers and respond to their needs well (Sandström et al., 2008).

When employees cannot modify their inner feelings, employees may use the surface acting strategy (Diefendorff and Greguras, 2009), but this may result in negative outcomes (Hatfield et al., 1994). Grandey et al. (2005) stated “when service providers do not seem sincere in their expressions… it is less likely to create a positive impression in the customer; instead, a false smile may seem manipulative and the employee’s impression management attempt fails” (p. 52). Since people tend to prefer honesty and authenticity in social interactions, employees’ fake emotional displays are unlikely to meet customers’ emotional expectation (Grandey, 2003; Hennig-Thurau et al., 2006). From the employees’ perspective, surface acting may also impair their service performance in that expressing a fake emotion involves additional waste of cognitive resources, affecting job satisfaction (Johnson and Spector, 2007). According to fit theory, when there is wide discrepancy between outward and inward feelings at work, employees are likely to be frustrated and dissatisfied with their jobs, decreasing work performance (Gabriel et al., 2015) which is a primary reason of negative feedback from customers (Heskett et al., 1994).

All things considered, it was anticipated detecting deep acting is likely to strengthen the link of expectation-confirmation toward satisfaction, while detections of surface acting may counteract it. In this study, Hypotheses 6 through 10 were proposed with regard to the positive moderating effects of customer detection of deep acting while Hypotheses 11 through 15 were about the negative moderating effects of customer detection of surface acting. All hypothesized paths in the study model are shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Theoretical model paths.

H6–10: Customer detection of deep acting positively moderates the relationship among the study constructs (i.e., emotional service expectation, overall confirmation, perceived quality, and satisfaction).

H11–15: Customer detection of surface acting negatively moderates the relationship among the study constructs (i.e., emotional service expectation, overall confirmation, perceived quality, and satisfaction).

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

A web-based online survey using email invitation was conducted using a nationally recognized consumer research panel service. A sample of 220 individuals from across the United States over 20 years old was used with screening questions of their hotel service experience with scenario explanation. All participants who satisfied the sampling conditions, opted in to the survey in exchange for a credit of $1. Participants answered the questions to examine the role of emotional service expectation toward customer satisfaction and the moderating roles of detections of deep acting and surface acting on their relationships in the service industry. The measurement items were adopted based on the literature review. Table 1 shows demographic results, including gender, age, income, marital status, education level, and annual income. Males made up 51.4% and 48.6% was female. Participants were categorized by age into young adults (ages 20–39 years; n = 142) and older adults (ages 40–50 and over+ years, n = 78). More than half of the sample had less than $50,000 in annual income.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Respondents’ socio-demographic characteristics.

Measures

The questionnaires contained question items focusing on the emotional service expectation, overall confirmation, perceived quality, customer satisfaction, and customer detection of deep and surface acting. Latent variables are not directly observable, so compound multi-item scale measures, including at least two items for each variable, were used (Kenny, 2014). Based on the existing literature, well-validated measurement items for study constructs were adopted and included in the questionnaire. Specifically, emotional service expectation measures were adopted from Davis et al. (1999) three-item scale using sympathy, enjoyment, and compassion dimensions. Overall confirmation was assessed using a two-item scale developed by Oliver (1980). For perceived service quality, three items were adopted from Fornell and Larcker (1981) study. A 5-point Likert-type scale was used, with response options ranging from 1 to 5 in three different dimension standards (e.g., 1 = “poor,” 5 = “excellent”; 1 = “inferior,” 5 = “superior”; and 1 = “low,” 5 = “high”). For customer satisfaction, two items were borrowed from Westbrook and Oliver (1991) study, designed to measure customer satisfaction and emotions with services. To assess customer detections of employees’ deep and surface acting, three-item measures were derived from previous study (Groth et al., 2009). These measures were developed “from Grandey (2003), originally developed by Brotheridge and Lee (2003)” (Groth et al., 2009, p. 964). Given the fact that most recent studies still adopt the measures of Brotheridge and Lee (2003) or Grandey (2003), Groth et al. (2009) measures are relatively updated (see Baranik et al., 2017; Uy et al., 2017; Lam et al., 2018; Moin, 2018). Except for quality dimensions, all other variables consistently used the same semantic measurement tool (e.g., 1 = “I would not feel this way at all,” 5 = “I would feel this way very much”). All items included in each variable are reported in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Items, standardized factor loadings and Cronbach’s α.

Results

Measurement Model Tests

Before analyzing the structural model, the measurement model was assessed with Confirmatory Factor Analyses (CFA) using AMOS 22 and SPSS. CFA evaluated goodness of fit with the six key variables, including emotional expectations, conformation, quality perception, satisfaction, detections of deep acting and surface acting. The results confirmed good fit indices (χ286 = 175.408, p < 0.000; RMSEA = 0.063; CFI = 0.978; NFI = 0.959; GFI = 0.925; RFI = 0.942; IFI = 0.978; TLI = 0.970) of the measurement model. Factor loadings for the indicators for each variable were all significant and sufficiently higher than the recommended value of 0.50 (Table 2), indicating convergent validity (Grayson and Marsh, 1994). All Cronbach’s alpha values for each variable were above the minimum threshold of 0.70 (Hair et al., 1998), suggesting internal consistency in measurement items. As shown in Table 3, composite reliability values for each construct ranged from 0.815 to 0.975, greater than 0.70, indicating good reliability (Hair et al., 2011). Average value extracted (AVE) values ranged from 0.609 to 0.929, all above 0.50, confirming convergent validity (Hair et al., 2011). Also, these AVE values were all greater than the square of correlation between pairs of constructs, achieving discriminant validity (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). All constructs’ correlations were less than the threshold of 0.85, confirming further discriminant validity (Kline, 2005). The means, standard deviations, composite reliability, average variance extracted, and Pearson’s correlations of variables are reported in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Means, standard deviations, reliabilities, AVE, and correlations.

Structural Model Tests

The results of the structural equation modeling (SEM) with maximum likelihood estimation procedure showed a good model fit (χ225= 53.801, p < 0.000; χ2/df = 2.151; GFI = 0.961; NFI = 0.980; CFI = 0.989; RFI = 0.964; IFI = 0.989; TLI = 0.981; RMSEA = 0.066). Table 4 presents empirical findings of the hypothesized relationships within the original model. Emotional service expectations were found to significantly and positively influence the overall confirmation (β = 0.582, p < 0.001, H1), but not significantly influence perceived quality (β = -0.054, p = 0.401, H2). Overall confirmation significantly and positively linked to perceived quality (β = 0.899, p < 0.001, H3) and customer satisfaction (β = 0.930, p < 0.001, H4). The hypothesized path from perceived quality to customer satisfaction was found to be insignificant (β = 0.004, p = 0.973, H5). Overall, the findings supported Hypotheses 1, 3, and 4, but provided no support for Hypotheses 2 and 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of the tests of path coefficients.

Tests of Measurement Invariance

Prior to testing moderating effects, a measurement invariance test across two groups of moderators was conducted (Steenkamp and Baumgartner, 1998). In this study, respondents were divided into two groups of each of moderators of deep and surface acting by a median-split method: high (n = 130) and low (n = 90) groups of deep acting; and high (n = 113) and low (n = 107) groups of surface acting. For each moderator, an unconstrained model (baseline model) and a constrained model (invariance model) were generated and tested. In the invariance model, factor loading, factor variances, and covariances were constrained to be equivalent across the two groups from the baseline model. As indicated in Table 5, Fit indices of Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) and Comparative Fit Index (CFI) were checked because they are relatively less sensitive to sample size (Fan et al., 1999), while the Chi-square index is relatively sensitive to sample size (Byrne, 2001).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of measurement invariance test.

With regard to deep acting, the overall model fits were excellent both for the baseline model (χ252= 125.213, CFI = 0.966; RMSEA = 0.073) and the invariance model (χ258= 134.694, CFI = 0.965; RMSEA = 0.071) (Table 5). The difference in χ2 between those two models was insignificant (Δχ26 = 1.580, p > 0.05), supporting the invariance model. The results confirmed that the measurement model was equivalent across high and low groups, so the invariance model was employed for the subsequent analyses of moderating effects of deep acting.

The structural invariance test for surface acting were conducted in the same way; the baseline (unconstrained) model (χ252 = 118.909, CFI = 0.976; RMSEA = 0.070) and the invariance (constrained) model (χ258 = 123.271, CFI = 0.976; RMSEA = 0.066) showed excellent fits to the data. Again, there was no significant difference in χ2 between the baseline and invariance models (Δχ26 = 0.702, p > 0.05), supporting the invariance model. Therefore, the invariance model was adopted for subsequent analysis of moderating effects of surface acting.

Tests of Moderating Effect of Deep and Surface Acting

For the moderating effect tests of deep acting, a baseline model was generated by adding the hypothesized links among the variables based on the invariance model in both high and low deep acting groups. As shown in Table 6, the baseline model satisfactorily fit to the data (χ250 = 94.997, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.979; RMSEA = 0.059). In the nested models, the hypothesized paths were constrained to be equal for high and low groups. The baseline model and a series of the nested models (equal path model) were compared in pairs to analyze moderating effects of deep acting. Contrary to our expectations, the differences in χ2 between high and low deep acting groups on each of the hypothesized paths were all found to be insignificant, rejecting Hypotheses 6, 7, 8, 9, and 10 (Figure 2 and Table 6). In other words, the original relationships among the variables were not moderated by deep acting. The findings related to deep acting were reported in Figure 2 and Table 6.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Results of path and model comparison of deep acting.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results of the structural invariance model with deep acting. Dash lines indicate hypothesized paths of moderating impact of deep acting. ∗∗∗p < 0.001, ∗∗p < 0.01, p < 0.05.

In the same way, a baseline model and a series of nested models (equal path model) for surface acting were compared in pairs (Table 7), and the baseline model provided an excellent fit to the data (χ250 = 96.225, CFI = 0.983; RMSEA = 0.059). On the path from emotional service expectation to overall confirmation, there was a significant difference in Chi-square between the baseline model and the nested model (χ251 = 112.931, CFI = 0.978; RMSEA = 0.068, Δχ21 = 16.706, p < 0.001), in support of Hypothesis 11. The positive effect of emotional service expectation on overall confirmation was significant when surface acting was less detected (β = 0.909, t-value = 5.677, p < 0.001), while such a positive effect turned to be insignificant when surface acting was strongly detected (β = 0.197, t-value = 1.258, p = 0.208). That means that surface acting offsets the positive influence of emotional service expectation on overall confirmation. The other nested models were not significantly different from the baseline model, rejecting Hypotheses 12, 13, 14, and 15. The findings of path model comparisons for surface acting were reported in Figure 3 and Table 7.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Results of path and model comparison of surface acting.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Results of the structural invariance model with surface acting. Dash lines indicate hypothesized paths of moderating impact of surface acting. ∗∗∗p < 0.001, ∗∗p < 0.01, p < 0.05.

Discussion

This study aimed to develop an alternative conceptual model articulating the role of emotional service expectation toward customer satisfaction and the moderating roles of customer detections of deep acting and surface acting on their relationships; the five paths were proposed among the variables, including emotional service expectation, overall confirmation, perceived quality, and customer satisfaction, and then detections of deep and surface acting were imposed on each of the paths to verify the moderating effects. We hypothesized that the detection of deep acting would cement each of the relationships while the detection of surface acting would weaken those relationships.

As expected, the role of customer’s emotional service expectation was significant in overall service confirmation (Hypothesis 1), which further influenced perceived quality (Hypothesis 3) and satisfaction (Hypothesis 4). The results are consistent with expectation violation theory (Burgoon, 1993) positing that an individual interacts with others with expectancy in which interaction outcomes are determined. Also, the study’s results supported Farrell and Oczkowski (2009) arguments that service outcomes are likely to be confirmed by employee–customer interactions that take place for service delivery. However, this study did not find the significant relationship between perceived quality and customer satisfaction (Hypothesis 5); this is not congruent with the dominant belief in existing studies. We postulate, overall confirmation sufficiently strongly influences customer satisfaction (Hypothesis 4), so the influence of perceived quality on customer satisfaction becomes relatively weak in this study construct.

A growing body of research has demonstrated the roles of emotional strategies in customer judgments and perceptions (Groth et al., 2009; Hülsheger et al., 2010; Grandey and Gabriel, 2015). Hülsheger et al. (2010) suggested that influences of emotional strategies on the customer’s decisions are significant and universal. However, in this study, the moderating effects of customer detection of emotional strategies were limited to the influence of emotional service expectation on overall confirmation; Customer detection of employees’ surface acting negatively influences service confirmation in the beginning stage of interaction (Hypothesis 11) and, as a result, indirectly decreases perceived quality and satisfaction. Whereas all the other hypothesized moderating effects were found to be insignificant. Especially, detections of deep acting hardly influence customer perception and evaluation of services. These results are somewhat consistent with Hur et al. (2015), indicating there were no direct impacts of employees’ deep acting strategies on service assessments.

Theoretical and Practical Implications

Despite a growing awareness of the critical roles of employees’ emotional services, little has been known about how customers’ emotional service expectations and employees’ emotional displays influence service assessments in the hospitality service context. The major theoretical contribution of this study demonstrates the role of employees’ emotional display strategies in the relationship between customer emotional service expectation and overall confirmation that further influenced satisfaction and perceived quality. This study extends the well-known body of literature articulating the roles of expectation in customer satisfaction (Oliver, 1980; Teas, 1993) by integrating emotional factors in a manner in which customer emotional expectation-confirmation influences perceived quality and satisfaction that are moderated by emotional display strategies. This study provides confirmative evidence that emotional service expectation and emotional display strategies play a critical role in service confirmation, suggesting that comprehensive understating of customer behaviors in the hotel service context is critical.

Also, this study successfully extends the traditional model framework consisting of direct and mediating effects based on expectancy-disconfirmation theory by incorporating emotional components as moderators. There are two contrasting theories explaining the effects of expectations. Direct effect hypothesis claims that expectations have a direct, positive impact on customer satisfaction (Park et al., 2018) while Affective expectation model emphasizes roles of discrepancy between actual and expected service outcomes in satisfaction (Wilson and Klaaren, 1992). The results of this study confirmed both of the expectation theories. Emotional service expectation has direct, positive effects on service outcomes (H1) while its influences on overall confirmation differ by detections of high and low surface acting (H11).

Customers have typical expectancies about employees’ emotional services, such as “service with a smile” (Parasuraman et al., 1985; Schminke et al., 2014). Burgoon and Walther (1990) claimed that expectancy is associated with predictions rather than desires. Consistent with their arguments, customers seem to predict authentic emotional services from employees; surface acting such as faking smiles appears not enough to enhance positive outcomes and even worse, has negative impacts on overall confirmation. Service managers are “wise to want workers to be sincere, to go well beyond the smile that’s just painted on” (Hochschild, 1983, p. 33). Even though some researchers argued that there is no difference in customer reactions and service outcomes between displays of authentic and fake emotions (Beal et al., 2006; Chi et al., 2011; Wang and Groth, 2014), the study’s results suggest that customer decisions can differ by customers’ detections of employees’ emotional displays, whether they are real or fake. Thus, service organizations need to intervene to help their employees adopt deep acting strategies to meet customer emotional service expectations. Possible training may include perspective taking and empathy training to help employees understand customer’s perspectives and display genuine emotions. In summary, managerial emphasis should be on hiring those who can genuinely express their emotions and training them to understand customer’s needs sincerely (Parker and Axtell, 2001).

Limitation and Future Research

As is the case with any research, there are limitations that can be suggested for future research. First, this study explored the influence of customers’ predictive expectations as an independent variable, so the other dimensions of expectations need to be investigated. Expectations can be classified into normative (i.e., what should be), predictive (i.e., what customers really expect), and equitable expectations (i.e., what customers should receive, taking into account the expenses borne) (Medrano et al., 2016). For the future research, it seems worthwhile comparing influences of different dimensions of customers’ expectations. Second, this study used an online panel in which respondents were asked questions recalling their most recent service experiences. While the use of an online panel is suitable to collect a large sample and the use of experience and memory based measures of emotions allows researchers to investigate real service users, these methods are subject to several response biases, such as selection bias, non-response bias, recall bias, and memory bias as questionnaires are not distributed perfectly randomly and/or participants have difficulty in retrieving the detailed information of the past experience. Another limit of this study is that the questionnaires were collected in the United States, and there may be different findings across cultures and nations. Norms and interpretations about emotion-communication may be specific to cultures, so customer responses to employees’ emotional displays are likely to vary across cultures (Triandis, 1989). For example, compared with individualist cultures, collectivist cultures tend to emphasize connectedness to other people, conformity, and social contribution (Ariely, 2008), so more severe emotional duties and obligations are likely to be implicitly imposed on employees in these cultures. In turn, customers in collectivist cultures may strongly predict employees’ deep acting strategies, while being more sensitive to surface acting. Fourth, factors unrelated services, such as weather, personality and customer emotions per se, may influence customers’ assessment of employees’ emotion displays (Penz and Hogg, 2011; Pelegrín-Borondo et al., 2015). Therefore, more properly controlled experiments seem to be necessary for future research to clearly determine causality among the variables. Lastly, it is necessary to explore which categorical emotional displays are responsible for the degree of customers’ detections of deep and surface action (Pelegrín-Borondo et al., 2015). Then, future research may give even greater insights into human resource management practically and academically in the hospitality industry.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by the research fund of Hanyang University (HY-2018).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Allen, J. A., Pugh, S. D., Grandey, A. A., and Groth, M. (2010). Following display rules in good or bad faith? Customer orientation as a moderator of the display rule-emotional labor relationship. Hum. Perform. 23, 101–115. doi: 10.1021/ja800916d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ariely, D. (2008). Predictably Irrational. New York, NY: HarperCollins.

Google Scholar

Ashforth, B. E., and Humphrey, R. H. (1993). Emotional labor in service roles: the influence of identity. Acad. Manag. Rev. 18, 88–115. doi: 10.3109/09638237.2012.734656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babin, B. J., Chebat, J. C., and Michon, R. (2004). Perceived appropriateness and its effect on quality, affect and behavior. J. Retail. Cons. Serv. 11, 287–298. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2003.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baranik, L. E., Wang, M., Gong, Y., and Shi, J. (2017). Customer mistreatment, employee health, and job performance: cognitive rumination and social sharing as mediating mechanisms. J. Manag. 43, 1261–1282. doi: 10.1177/0149206314550995

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beal, D. J., Trougakos, J. P., Weiss, H. M., and Green, S. G. (2006). Episodic processes in emotional labor: perceptions of affective delivery and regulation strategies. J. Appl. Psychol. 91:1053.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Brady, M. K., and Cronin J. J. Jr., (2001). Some new thoughts on conceptualizing perceived service quality: a hierarchical approach. J. Mark. 65, 34–49. doi: 10.1509/jmkg.65.3.34.18334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brotheridge, C. M., and Lee, R. T. (2003). Development and validation of the emotional labour scale. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 76, 365–379. doi: 10.1348/096317903769647229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgoon, J. K. (1993). Interpersonal expectations, expectancy violations, and emotional communication. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 12, 30–48. doi: 10.1177/0261927X93121003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgoon, J. K., and Walther, J. B. (1990). Nonverbal expectancies and the evaluative consequences of violations. Hum. Commun. Res. 17, 232–265. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1990.tb00232.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burns, D. J., and Neisner, L. (2006). Customer satisfaction in a retail setting: the contribution of emotion. Int. J. Retail Distri. Manag. 34, 49–66. doi: 10.1108/09590550610642819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS, EQS, and LISREL: comparative approaches to testing for the factorial validity of a measuring instrument. Int. J. Test. 1, 55–86. doi: 10.1207/S15327574IJT0101_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chi, N. W., Grandey, A. A., Diamond, J. A., and Krimmel, K. R. (2011). Want a tip? Service performance as a function of emotion regulation and extraversion. J. Appl. Psychol. 96:1337. doi: 10.1037/a0022884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. H., Mitchell, K. V., Hall, J. A., Lothert, J., Snapp, T., and Meyer, M. (1999). Empathy, expectations, and situational preferences: personality influences on the decision to participate in volunteer helping behaviors. J. Pers. 67, 469–503. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diefendorff, J. M., and Greguras, G. J. (2009). Contextualizing emotional display rules: examining the roles of targets and discrete emotions in shaping display rule perceptions. J. Manag. 35, 880–898. doi: 10.1177/0149206308321548

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diefendorff, J. M., and Richard, E. M. (2003). Antecedents and consequences of emotional display rule perceptions. J. Appl. Psychol. 88:284.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Diefendorff, J. M., Richard, E. M., and Croyle, M. H. (2006). Are emotional display rules formal job requirements? Examination of employee and supervisor perceptions. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 79, 273–298. doi: 10.1348/096317905X68484

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, X., Thompson, B., and Wang, L. (1999). Effects of sample size, estimation methods, and model specification on structural equation modeling fit indexes. Struct. Equ. Model 6, 56–83. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrell, M. A., and Oczkowski, E. (2009). Service worker customer orientation, organisation/job fit and perceived organisational support. J. Strat. Mark. 17, 149–167. doi: 10.1016/j.apnr.2016.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 18, 39–50. doi: 10.1177/002224378101800104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabriel, A. S., Daniels, M. A., Diefendorff, J. M., and Greguras, G. J. (2015). Emotional labor actors: a latent profile analysis of emotional labor strategies. J. Appl. Psychol. 100:863. doi: 10.1037/a0037408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaur, S. S., Herjanto, H., and Makkar, M. (2014). Review of emotions research in marketing, 2002–2013. J. Retail. Cons. Serv. 21, 917–923. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2014.08.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gracia, E., Bakker, A. B., and Grau, R. M. (2011). Positive emotions: the connection between customer quality evaluations and loyalty. Cornell Hosp. Quart. 52, 458–465. doi: 10.1177/1938965510395379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotional regulation in the workplace: a new way to conceptualize emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 5:95. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A. (2003). When “the show must go on”: surface acting and deep acting as determinants of emotional exhaustion and peer-rated service delivery. Acad. Manag. J. 46, 86–96.

Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A., Fisk, G. M., Mattila, A. S., Jansen, K. J., and Sideman, L. A. (2005). Is “service with a smile” enough? Authenticity of positive displays during service encounters. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 96, 38–55. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A., and Gabriel, A. S. (2015). Emotional labor at a crossroads: where do we go from here? Ann. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 2, 323–349. doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111400

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grayson, D., and Marsh, H. W. (1994). Identification with deficient rank loading matrices in confirmatory factor analysis: multitrait-multimethod models. Psychometrika 59, 121–134. doi: 10.1007/BF02294271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grönroos, C. (1984). A service quality model and its marketing implications. Eur. J. Mark. 18, 36–44. doi: 10.1108/EUM0000000004784

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groth, M., Hennig-Thurau, T., and Walsh, G. (2009). Customer reactions to emotional labor: the roles of employee acting strategies and customer detection accuracy. Acad. Manag. J. 52, 958–974. doi: 10.5465/amj.2009.44634116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., and Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate Data Analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2011). PLS-SEM: Indeed a silver bullet. J. Mark. Theor. Pract. 19, 139–152. doi: 10.2753/MTP1069-6679190202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, H. (2013). Effects of in-flight ambience and space/function on air travelers’ decision to select a low-cost airline. Tour. Manag. 37, 125–135. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2013.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, H., and Jeong, C. (2013). Multi-dimensions of patrons’ emotional experiences in upscale restaurants and their role in loyalty formation: emotion scale improvement. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 32, 59–70. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2012.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., and Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional Contagion: Studies in Emotion and Social Interaction. Editions De La Maison Des Sciences De L’homme. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Hennig-Thurau, T., Groth, M., Paul, M., and Gremler, D. D. (2006). Are all smiles created equal? How emotional contagion and emotional labor affect service relationships. J. Mark. 70, 58–73. doi: 10.1509/jmkg.70.3.058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heskett, J. L., Jones, T. O., Loveman, G. W., Sasser, W. E., and Schlesinger, L. A. (1994). Putting the service-profit chain to work. Harv. Bus. Rev. 72, 164–174.

Google Scholar

Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The Managed Heart: Commercialization of Human Feeling. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Homburg, C., and Stock, R. M. (2004). The link between salespeople’s job satisfaction and customer satisfaction in a business-to-business context: a dyadic analysis. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 32:144. doi: 10.1177/0092070303261415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hülsheger, U. R., Lang, J. W., and Maier, G. W. (2010). Emotional labor, strain, and performance: testing reciprocal relationships in a longitudinal panel study. J. Occup. Health psychol. 15:505. doi: 10.1037/a0021003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphrey, S. E., Nahrgang, J. D., and Morgeson, F. P. (2007). Integrating motivational, social, and contextual work design features: a meta-analytic summary and theoretical extension of the work design literature. J. Appl. Psychol. 92:1332.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hur, W. M., Moon, T. W., and Jung, Y. S. (2015). Customer response to employee emotional labor: the structural relationship between emotional labor, job satisfaction, and customer satisfaction. J. Serv. Mark. 29, 71–80. doi: 10.1108/JSM-07-2013-0161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johanson, M. M., and Woods, R. H. (2008). Recognizing the emotional element in service excellence. Cornell Hosp. Quar. 49, 310–316. doi: 10.1177/1938965508316267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, H. A. M., and Spector, P. E. (2007). Service with a smile: do emotional intelligence, gender, and autonomy moderate the emotional labor process? J. Occup. Health Psychol. 12:319. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.12.4.319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. A. (2014). Measuring Model Fit in Structural Equation Modeling. Avaialable at: www.davidak-enny.net/cm/fit.htm (accessed October 15, 2016).

Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2005). Methodology in the Social Sciences. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling 2nd edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Koenig-Lewis, N., and Palmer, A. (2014). The effects of anticipatory emotions on service satisfaction and behavioral intention. J. Serv. Mark. 28, 437–451. doi: 10.1108/JSM-09-2013-0244

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krumhuber, E., Manstead, A. S., Cosker, D., Marshall, D., Rosin, P. L., and Kappas, A. (2007). Facial dynamics as indicators of trustworthiness and cooperative behavior. Emotion 7:730. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladhari, R. (2009). Service quality, emotional satisfaction, and behavioural intentions: a study in the hotel industry. Manag. Serv. Qual. 19, 308–331. doi: 10.1108/09604520910955320

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, W., Huo, Y., and Chen, Z. (2018). Who is fit to serve? Person–job/organization fit, emotional labor, and customer service performance. Hum. Res. Manag. 57, 483–497. doi: 10.1002/hrm.21871

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, J. S. C., and Liang, H. Y. (2011). The influence of service environments on customer emotion and service outcomes. Manag. Serv. Qual. 21, 350–372. doi: 10.1108/09604521111146243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattila, A. S., and Enz, C. A. (2002). The role of emotions in service encounters. J. Serv. Res. 4, 268–277. doi: 10.1177/1094670502004004004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medrano, N., Olarte-Pascual, C., Pelegrín-Borondo, J., and Sierra-Murillo, Y. (2016). Consumer behavior in shopping streets: the importance of the salesperson’s professional personal attention. Front. Psychol. 7:125. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menguc, B., Auh, S., Fisher, M., and Haddad, A. (2013). To be engaged or not to be engaged: the antecedents and consequences of service employee engagement. J. Bus. Res. 66, 2163–2170. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2012.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moin, M. F. (2018). The link between perceptions of leader emotion regulation and followers’ organizational commitment. J. Manag. Dev. 37, 178–187. doi: 10.1108/JMD-01-2017-0014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliver, R. L. (1980). A cognitive model of the antecedents and consequences of satisfaction decisions. J. Mark. Res. 17, 460–469. doi: 10.1177/002224378001700405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parasuraman, A., Zeithaml, V. A., and Berry, L. L. (1985). A conceptual model of service quality and its implications for future research. J. Mark. 49, 41–50. doi: 10.1177/002224298504900403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parasuraman, A., Zeithaml, V. A., and Berry, L. L. (1988). Servqual: a multiple-item scale for measuring consumer perc. J. Retail. 64:12.

Google Scholar

Park, S., Hahn, S., Lee, T., and Jun, M. (2018). Two factor model of consumer satisfaction: international tourism research. Tour. Manag. 67, 82–88. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2018.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, S. K., and Axtell, C. M. (2001). Seeing another viewpoint: antecedents and outcomes of employee perspective taking. Acad. Manag. J. 44, 1085–1100.

Google Scholar

Pelegrín-Borondo, J., Juaneda-Ayensa, E., González-Menorca, L., and González-Menorca, C. (2015). Dimensions and basic emotions: a complementary approach to the emotions produced to tourists by the hotel. J. Vacat. Mark. 21, 351–365. doi: 10.1177/1356766715580869

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penz, E., and Hogg, M. K. (2011). The role of mixed emotions in consumer behaviour: investigating ambivalence in consumers’ experiences of approach-avoidance conflicts in online and offline settings. Eur. J. Mark. 45, 104–132. doi: 10.1108/03090561111095612

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roch, C. H., and Poister, T. H. (2006). Citizens, accountability, and service satisfaction: the influence of expectations. Urban Aff. Rev. 41, 292–308. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandström, S., Edvardsson, B., Kristensson, P., and Magnusson, P. (2008). Value in use through service experience. Manag. Serv. Qual. 18, 112–126. doi: 10.1108/09604520810859184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schepers, J. J., Nijssen, E. J., and van der Heijden, G. A. (2016). Innovation in the frontline: exploring the relationship between role conflict, ideas for improvement, and employee service performance. Int. J. Res. Mark. 33, 797–817. doi: 10.1016/j.ijresmar.2016.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schminke, M., Caldwell, J., Ambrose, M. L., and McMahon, S. R. (2014). Better than ever? Employee reactions to ethical failures in organizations, and the ethical recovery paradox. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 123, 206–219. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenkamp, J. B. E., and Baumgartner, H. (1998). Assessing measurement invariance in cross-national consumer research. J. Cons. Res. 25, 78–90. doi: 10.1086/209528

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutton, A. W., Baldwin, S. P., Wood, L., and Hoffman, B. J. (2013). A meta-analysis of the relationship between rater liking and performance ratings. Hum. Perform. 26, 409–429. doi: 10.1080/08959285.2013.836523

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teas, R. K. (1993). Consumer expectations and the measurement of perceived service quality. J. Profess. Serv. Mark. 8, 33–54. doi: 10.1300/J090v08n02_05

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychol. Rev. 96:506. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, W. (2001). Knowledge transfer in intraorganizational networks: effects of network position and absorptive capacity on business unit innovation and performance. Acad. Manag. J. 44, 996–1004.

Google Scholar

Uy, M. A., Lin, K. J., and Ilies, R. (2017). Is it better to give or receive? The role of help in buffering the depleting effects of surface acting. Acad. Manag. J. 60, 1442–1461. doi: 10.5465/amj.2015.0611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, K. L., and Groth, M. (2014). Buffering the negative effects of employee surface acting: the moderating role of employee–customer relationship strength and personalized services. J. Appl. Psychol. 99:341. doi: 10.1037/a0034428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westbrook, R. A., and Oliver, R. L. (1991). The dimensionality of consumption emotion patterns and consumer satisfaction. J. Cons. Res. 49, 41–50. doi: 10.1086/209243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, T. D., and Klaaren, K. J. (1992). Expectation whirls me round: The role of affective expectations on affective experiences. Rev. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 14, 1–31.

Google Scholar

Wong, A. (2004). The role of emotional satisfaction in service encounters. Manag. Serv. Qual. 14, 365–376. doi: 10.1108/09604520410557976

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotional service, customer expectation, satisfaction, emotional labor, deep/surface acting

Citation: Jeong JY, Park J and Hyun H (2019) The Role of Emotional Service Expectation Toward Perceived Quality and Satisfaction: Moderating Effects of Deep Acting and Surface Acting. Front. Psychol. 10:321. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00321

Received: 11 September 2018; Accepted: 01 February 2019;
Published: 12 March 2019.

Edited by:

Monica Gomez-Suárez, Autonomous University of Madrid, Spain

Reviewed by:

Teresa Villacé-Molinero, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, Spain
Jorge Pelegrín-Borondo, University of La Rioja, Spain

Copyright © 2019 Jeong, Park and Hyun. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jungkun Park, viroid2007@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.