Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 February 2019
Sec. Educational Psychology

Executive Function and Resilience as Mediators of Adolescents’ Perceived Stressful Life Events and School Adjustment

\r\nYuqing Zhang,&#x;Yuqing Zhang1,2†Xing Zhang&#x;Xing Zhang3†Liwei ZhangLiwei Zhang4Cheng Guo,*Cheng Guo1,2*
  • 1The Lab of Mental Health and Social Adaptation, Faulty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
  • 2Research Center of Mental Health Education, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
  • 3Faulty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
  • 4Physical Education Department, Luoyang Vocational & Technical College, Luoyang, China

This study investigated psychological mechanisms underlying the relationship between stressful life events and school adjustment in Chinese adolescents. The Adolescent Self-rating Life Events Checklist, the Adolescent Executive Function Scale, the Chinese version of the Resilience Scale, and the School-adjustment Scale were administered to 1101 Chinese adolescents (465 males, 636 females), aged 11–19 years, from three secondary schools. Results from serial mediation analysis revealed that perceived stressful life events could affect school adjustment through the mediation of executive function and resilience. The mediation effect contained three paths, the separate mediation effect of executive function, the separate mediation effect of resilience, and the serial mediation effect of executive function and resilience. These findings provide valuable insights into the effect of perceived stressful life events on school adjustment of Chinese adolescents, and suggest that, the researchers and educators could enhance school adjustment in vulnerable groups by improving their executive function and resilience.

Introduction

We will encounter various stressful events in our daily life. Indeed, stressors begin to affect individuals from birth (Kramer et al., 2009), and occur in the daily life of virtually every individual (Compas et al., 1993). Stressful life events refer to life experiences that result in changes in an individual’s life and those that necessitate coping and adjustment strategies (Compas, 1987). Adolescence is an important transition in academic, cognitive, social, physiological, and physical change (Arnett, 1999), and is a fragile developmental stage characterized by exposure to stressful life events and their debilitating mental health effects (Byrne et al., 2007). The stressful life events among adolescents have attracted the attention of many researchers (Dupéré et al., 2018; Han et al., 2018; Humphreys et al., 2018). Furthermore, a large amount of studies showed that stressful life events can exert profound influence on an individual’s maladjustment (Sandler et al., 1994; Rod et al., 2009), and stressful life events in adolescence are correlated with many negative outcomes, such as decreased well-being, impaired mental health, anxiety and depression (Troy and Mauss, 2011). Other studies further pointed that the stressful life events played a role in triggering high school dropout (Dupéré et al., 2018), and higher levels of perceived stress were associated with decreased hippocampal volume in adolescent (Piccolo and Noble, 2017). Thus, the stressful life events have negative effects on the physical and mental health of adolescents. Adolescence period is in school-age, and the adolescents spend most of their time in school activities. Thus, school adjustment of adolescents is an important problem that the whole society need pay special attention to (Maclean et al., 2016; Chen et al., 2019). However, the mediating processes that account for the association between perceived stressful life events and school adjustment are still being identified. Therefore, it is urgent to explore the association between perceived stressful life events and school adjustment and the psychological mechanisms behind this association among adolescence.

Perceived Stressful Life Events, Executive Function, and School Adjustment

Executive function refers to a family of top-down mental processes that are essential for tasks requiring concentration and attention, performed in such instances when relying on instinct or intuition would be ill advised or insufficient. Core executive functions include inhibition control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility (Diamond, 2013). Prior studies provide indirect support for the prediction that stressful life events impair executive function in adolescence. For example, many studies showed that early life stress exposure was associated with poorer adult cognitive function in memory domains and executive function in healthy and psychopathology populations (Navalta et al., 2006), and individuals who reporting predictive early life stress exhibited poorer processing speed and working memory performance (Saleh et al., 2017). In addition, Pukay-Martin et al. (2003) found that, for HIV-positive subjects, stressful life events were related to poor performance on measures of executive function, attention, and processing speed, while positive life events were related to better performance. In addition to behavioral evidence, evidence from neuroimaging study also suggested that, greater activation in brain regions responsible for executive functions accounted for the association between exposure to chronic stress and less use of adaptive coping among adolescents (Reising et al., 2018). It’s not hard to come to conclusion that, stressful life events may have a negative effect on executive function, the more stressful life events the individuals perceived, the individuals may show worse executive function performance. In general, impaired executive function is viewed as maladjusted cognitive activity and has been shown to be related to anxiety and depression (Brosschot et al., 2006; Greenaway and Howlin, 2010). In addition, other studies pointed to the effects of executive function on children’s academic performance, specifically in reading, mathematics, and interpersonal relationships (Clark et al., 2002; Blair and Razza, 2007; Fitzpatrick et al., 2014), as well as late adolescents’ academic performance (Baars et al., 2015). More importantly, the prevailing argument is that impairments in executive function can lead to significant and lasting adjustment difficulties (Biederman et al., 2004; Ellis et al., 2004). Consistent with this argument, several longitudinal studies suggested that executive function was a predictor of children’s school adjustment (Jacobson et al., 2011; Masten and Tellegen, 2012) and was regard as a determinant of academic performance in late adolescents university students (Baars et al., 2015). Accordingly, perceived more stressful life events may lead to impairments in execution function, which in turn may affect the school adjustment of the adolescents. However, most of studies in the domain of executive function were conducted by experimental paradigm. Laboratory studies on executive function have been developed comprehensively, but executive function are less measured by self-reported questionnaires (Gioia et al., 2000; Baars et al., 2015). Furthermore, because of its practicality and flexibility, the self-reported executive function scale can get more observable and self-insight data than laboratory tests (Barkley and Murphy, 2011; Baars et al., 2015). Self-reported executive function scale provides practitioners a reliable and effective assessment tool to assess the level of executive function. Accordingly, executive function, being measured by questionnaires and being considered as a fundamental human cognitive factor, might play a significant role in the influence between perceived stressful life events and school adjustment among adolescents.

Perceived Stressful Life Events, Resilience and School Adjustment

Resilience can be broadly defined as the potential or manifested capacity of a dynamic system to adjust successfully to disturbances that threaten the function, survival, or development of the system (Masten, 2015). As Masten (2015) has suggested, the research in the domain of resilience mainly focuses on adjustment under challenging circumstances. Although everyone will encounter various life stresses, not everyone will show negative outcomes in the face of stress, and some exhibit resilience even in the face of high levels of stress exposure. Many studies found the negative association between stressful life events and resilience. For example, Cole et al. (2015) found a negative relationship between stress of academic performance and ego resilience, which provides preliminary evidence for this relationship; other study showed that the resilience level of Sichuan earthquake survivors was lower than that of those who didn’t experience the earthquake (Ni et al., 2015). More importantly, many recent studies from adolescent population also indicated that, the stressful life events were negatively associated with resilience (Anyan and Hjemdal, 2016). Thus, stress may have an adverse impact on an individual’s resilience.

In addition, a large scale of studies showed that, resilience was positively relate to people’s mental health (Catalano et al., 2011; Li et al., 2012) and quality of life (Migerode et al., 2012), while was negatively associated with depression or anxiety (Anyan and Hjemdal, 2016). As an important stress coping resource, resilience also contributes to psychological and behavioral adjustment. For example, Luthar et al. (2000) found that, resilience was particularly important in forming psychosocial adjustment (e.g., at-risk children who appeared resilient, based on high academic grades, also showed positive adjustment based on persevering classroom behaviors as perceived by others). Other studies further found that, resilience had a predictive role in school adjustment among elementary school students (Lee et al., 2014) and middle school students (Zhu et al., 2012). More importantly, a recent study found resilience played a mediator role of the association between academic stress and school life adjustment among adolescent (Kim et al., 2018), which provides more direct evidence for the possible mediating role of resilience on the association between perceived stressful life events and school adjustment in adolescents. Thus, stressful life events affect resilience, and resilience has an influence on school adjustment, it is reasonable to predict that, resilience plays a mediator in the relationship between perceived stressful life events and school adjustment in adolescents.

Perceived Stressful Life Events, Executive Function, Resilience and School Adjustment

Although perceived stress has a detrimental effect on resilience, Masten and Tellegen (2012) argued that, executive function provided much of the capacity for resilience in humans. Benight and Cieslak (2011) argued that, the cognitive determinants played important role in successful adjustment, and proposed a self-efficacy based model of resilience. According to the self-efficacy based model of resilience, the focus on cognitive factors could easily apply in theoretical models to enhance our understanding of resilience. Due to executive function’s effect on goal-oriented behavior, having effective executive function may enhance individuals’ sense of self-efficacy, or the feeling that one has abilities to accomplish tasks. For example, executive function had been found to be related to self-efficacy beliefs regarding exercise (McAuley et al., 2011) in older adults, and driving ability (Rike et al., 2015) in individuals with brain injury. As a part of broader motivational system, the self-efficacy may be a key component of resilience (Masten, 2014). These studies provide preliminary evidence for the relationship between executive function and resilience from theoretical and empirical aspects. Additionally, Greenberg (2006) suggested that the interventions aimed at improving children’s executive function could promote resilience, implying the important role of executive function ability in resilience. More importantly, executive function was found to be associated with resilience in adolescents with depressed mothers (Davidovich et al., 2016), as well as in first-year undergraduate students (McKee, 2017). Thus, the concrete theoretical and empirical bases support the close association between executive function and resilience. Further, as discussed previously, stressful life events may lead to impaired executive function and resilience may contribute to better school adjustment in adolescence. Therefore, it is reasonable to predict that, the less stressful life events the adolescents perceived may be associated with better executive function, executive function has positive effect on adolescents’ resilience, and resilience may contribute to better school adjustment in adolescence.

The Current Study

In sum, the purpose of this study is to examine the influence of stressful life events on the school adjustment of adolescents and the potential mediators (e.g., execution function and resilience) behind this association. Adolescence is a fragile developmental stage characterized by exposure to stressful life events and the adolescents’ school adjustment is an important problem that the whole society need pay special attention to (Lan et al., 2019), exploring the potential psychology mechanisms behind the association between stressful life events and school adjustment in adolescence may contribute to reduce the negative impact of stressful life events on their school adjustment, thus having important theoretical and practical significance (Benight and Cieslak, 2011). Based on the literature review, we hypothesized that:

H1: Executive function would mediate the impact of stressful life events on school adjustment.

H2: Resilience would mediate the impact of stressful life events on school adjustment.

H3: Executive function and resilience would serially mediate the impact of stressful life events and school adjustment. This mediation model contains three paths.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were 1175 adolescents randomly recruited from three secondary schools in the southwest of China. Because the invalid questionnaires of some participants, a total of 74 adolescents were excluded from analyses. Thus, data from 1101 adolescents were used after data screening procedures (465 males, 636 females, aged 11–19 years, M = 14.77). All of adolescents and their parents had provided written informed consent before data collection, and then all of adolescents filled out all the paper questionnaires in pen. This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Human Ethics Committee of the Southwest University. All the adolescents gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of the Southwest University.

Measures

Perceived Stress

The Adolescent Self-rating Life Events Checklist (ASLEC; Xin and Yao, 2015) was originally used to assess the severity of recent negative life events during the past 6 months. The respondents rated the impact of each negative life event on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not at all to 5 = extremely severe). This measure incorporates 26 items, including five dimensions: being punished, loss, relationship pressure, learning pressure, and adaptation problems (Xin and Yao, 2015). The higher the score, the more stressful life events were perceived. This scale has been found to be reliable and valid among Chinese adolescents (Xin et al., 2016). In current study, the Cronbach’s alpha was 0.934, the composite reliability was 0.896, the average variance extracted was 0.633 and the MacDonald’s omega was 0.855, 95% CI = 0.837 to 0.869.

Executive Function

The Adolescent Executive Function Scale (Huang et al., 2014) was originally used to assess executive function status in adolescents. This scale contains 21 items that consist of three factors, namely, inhibition control, cognitive flexibility, and working memory. The respondents rated how often the specific behaviors happen in the past 6 months on a 3-point Likert scale (1 = not at all to 3 = often). Higher scores on this metric typically indicate worse executive function performance. However, in the current study, all items were reverse-scored so that high scores indicated better executive function performance. The Cronbach’s alpha in the current study was 0.852, the composite reliability was 0.793, the average variance extracted was 0.561, and the MacDonald’s omega was 0.850, 95% CI = 0.832 to 0.864.

Resilience

Resilience was originally assessed with the Chinese version of the Resilience Scale (RS; Wagnild and Young, 1993), which has been widely used in China and has been shown to be a reliable and valid measure in Chinese adolescents (Zheng et al., 2011). This scale consists of 25 items arranged in two subscales: personal competence and acceptance of the self and life. The higher the score, the higher the resilience level of the respondents. Participants rated the extent to which each item reflected them on a 7 - point Likert scale (1 = disagree to 7 = agree). In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient was 0.880, the composite reliability was 0.908, the average variance extracted was 0.831, and the MacDonald’s omega was 0.855, 95% CI = 0.841 to 0.869.

School Adjustment

School adjustment was originally assessed with the School-adjustment Scale (Sung et al., 2014) and contains 21 statements. The questionnaire includes three dimensions: teacher-student relationships, peer relationships, and self-adjustment. A 6-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 6 strongly agree) was used to indicate the agreement level of each statement with regard to school adjustment. The higher the score, the higher the school adjustment level of the respondents. In the current study, the Cronbach’s alpha value is 0.852, the composite reliability was 0.837, the average variance extracted was 0.631, and the MacDonald’s omega was 0.855, 95% CI = 0.838–0.869.

Analytic Strategy

Our analytic approach involved two steps. Prior to the examination of the hypothesized mediational model regarding the mediating effects of execution function and resilience on the relation between perceived stressful life events and school adjustment, descriptive statistics and correlations were conducted with for all variables in the total sample by SPSS 21.0. Then, a two-step procedure was applied to analyze the mediation effect (Anderson and Gerbing, 1988; Zhang et al., 2016). Firstly, the measurement model, which involved four latent variables, was tested to assess the goodness of fit represented by its explicit indicators. Secondly, if the index of measurement model met the requirements, the maximum likelihood estimation examined the structural equation modeling (SEM). Mplus 7.4 was used to evaluate the hypothetical model’s data fit (Muthén and Muthén, 1998/2012). According to previous studies (Schafer and Graham, 2002; Graham, 2009; Curtis et al., 2016), missing data were handled using the full information maximum likelihood (FIML) procedure. SEM was performed using the robust maximum likelihood (MLR) estimator to account for identified data non-normality. Indirect effects were tested using bootstrapping procedures (Preacher and Hayes, 2008).

We used the following indices to evaluate the model’s data fit: the confirmatory fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the self-reunion multiple regression (SRMR). According to previous study, values >0.90 for the CFI and TLI, and <0.6 and <0.8 for the RMSEA and SRMR, respectively, were considered to indicate good model fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999).

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Correlations of Variables

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics, including means, standard deviations, and Pearson’s bivariate correlations for all observed variables. Executive function was positively correlated with resilience, and resilience was positively correlated with school adjustment. Although other variables are statistically significantly interrelated (p < 0.01), their correlation coefficients were less than 0.30, which cannot be considered as a significative correlation.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations between observed variables.

Measurement Model

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to test the measurement model comprising the four interrelated latent variables: stressful life events, executive function, resilience, and school adjustment. The latent stressful life events variables were described as a being punished factor, loss factor, relationship pressure factor, learning pressure factor, and adaptation problem factor. The latent executive function variables were described by inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. The resilience latent variable was described by personal competence, and acceptance of self and life. The school adjustment variable was described by teacher-student relationships, peer relationships, and self-adjustment. The measurement model fit the observed data well: χ2= 420.324, df = 59, χ2/df = 7.124, CFI = 0.925, TLI = 0.901, SRMR = 0.051, and RMSEA = 0.075.

Structural Model

The model examined the associations between stressful life events, executive function, resilience, and school adjustment. Results showed acceptable data fit, χ2= 420.324, df = 59, χ2/df = 7.124, CFI = 0.925, TLI = 0.901, SRMR = 0.051, and RMSEA = 0.075. Specifically, the results provided support for H1 [i.e., executive function mediates the impact of stressful life events on school adjustment (β = -0.200, SE = 0.041, p < 0.001)], H2 [i.e., resilience mediates the impact of stressful life events on school adjustment (β = 0.065, SE = 0.029, p = 0.025)], and H3 [i.e., stressful life events exert a non-significant indirect effect on adolescent’s school adjustment through the three-path mediation effect of executive function and resilience (β = -0.055, SE = 0.042, p = 0.191)] (see Figure 1). We subsequently used Mplus’ model constraint command to create auxiliary variables and used bootstrapping in order to compare the mediation effects (Muthén and Muthén, 1998/2012; Preacher and Hayes, 2008). Executive function affected school adjustment more strongly than resilience (β = -0.265, SE = 0.054, p < 0.001, CI = -0.207 to -0.073; Table 2).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Standardized parameter estimates of the final model. ∗∗∗p < 0.001, p < 0.05.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Standardized indirect effects from stressful life events to school adjustment.

Discussion

The purpose of present study was to explore the role of executive function and resilience in the relationship between perceived stressful life events and school adjustment among adolescents. Consistent with our hypotheses, perceived stressful life events affected school adjustment through the mediation of executive function and resilience. The mediation effect contained three paths: the separate mediation effect of executive function, the separate mediation effect of resilience, and the serial mediation effect of executive function and resilience. The serial mediation model indicated that, the experience of multiple stressful life events impeded the development of executive function, adversely affected the formation of resilience, and led to poor school adjustment among adolescents, which is consistent with results reported in prior studies (Best et al., 2009; Duckworth et al., 2012).

The findings from the current study revealed the mediating effect of executive function on the association between perceived stressful life events and school adjustment among adolescents. The result is consistent with previous studies regarding the negative link between stressful life events and executive function (Pukay-Martin et al., 2003; Saleh et al., 2017), the positive link between executive function and school adjustment (Jacobson et al., 2011; Masten and Tellegen, 2012; Fitzpatrick et al., 2014), and the close link between stressful life events and negative outcomes during adolescence (Nishikawa et al., 2018). A large scale of studies indicated that, executive function contributed to successful adjustment to school during childhood (e.g., school readiness and subsequent academic achievement) (Espy et al., 2004; Bull et al., 2008; Clark et al., 2010; Welsh et al., 2010; Hughes and Ensor, 2011; Neuenschwander et al., 2012), and academic success during early adolescence (Lafavor, 2018). More importantly, other study revealed the mediating role of executive function on the association between poverty and academic delays (Nesbitt et al., 2013). Obviously, poverty could be regarded as stressful life event. The current study confirms previous findings in children and adults are also applicable to the adolescent, and further show that perceived stressful life events have a negative influence on school adjustment of the adolescents via affecting executive function. Thus, the findings from the current study suggest that, the more stressful life events the adolescents perceived, may lead to worse executive function performance, which, in turn, have negative effects on the school adjustment.

The findings from the current study also indicate the mediating effect of resilience on the association between perceived stressful life events and school adjustment among adolescents. Some previous studies found that, the more stressful life events could lead to poorer resilience (Anyan and Hjemdal, 2016). Contrary to previous studies, the findings from current study show that, stressful life events could positively predict resilience among adolescents. Inconsistency in findings may be due to differences in measuring tools for variables and heterogeneity of participants with regard to age. In general, the stressful life events that individuals perceived in daily life belong to appropriate stress, and these appropriate stresses may help to develop adolescents’ resilience to stress. According to the challenge model of resilience, when appropriately exposed to risk that can be overcome, the individual, could learn to deploy resources to overcome future stress (Fergus and Zimmerman, 2004), which implies the special significance of appropriate stress on developing individuals’ resilience. The findings from present study seem to support the challenge model of resilience. Indeed, recent adversity may negatively affect individuals in the short term, while prior experience may yield a greater propensity for resilience over a prolonged period (Seery, 2011). In addition, previous research found a mediator role of resilience in the association between academic stress on school life adjustment among adolescent (Kim et al., 2018), and a moderating role of resilience on the relationship between perceived stress and binge eating symptoms among young adult women (Thurston et al., 2018). These studies demonstrate the important role of resilience on the relationship between stress and mental health. The current study confirms previous findings, and shows that, resilience is another intervening variable between perceived stress and school adjustment.

More interestingly, the current study suggests that, the perceived stressful life events may affect school adjustment of adolescents via the serial mediation effect of executive function and resilience, which emphasizes the serial effect of executive function and resilience on the association between stressful life events and school adjustment. The finding that executive function predicts resilience is consistent with previous research (Davidovich et al., 2016; McKee, 2017), and further supports the notion that the poor executive function may represent a risk factor that leads individuals to be less resilient to life stresses, which can have a negative impact on school adjustment. Additionally, previous work demonstrated that stressful life events could predict poorer executive function (Pukay-Martin et al., 2003), had influence on an individual’s maladjustment (Sandler et al., 1994; Rod et al., 2009), and were correlated with many negative outcomes (e.g., anxiety and depression) (Troy and Mauss, 2011). Furthermore, other study suggested that the association between stressful life events and teenage students’ social adjustment can be explained in part by social problem-solving skill (Yang et al., 2012), implying that stressful life events may have negative effect on cognitive ability. More importantly, many previous researches found the important effect of executive function on resilience (Davidovich et al., 2016; McKee, 2017), and the important role of resilience on the association between perceived stress and mental health (García-Izquierdo et al., 2018; Thurston et al., 2018). The present study extends previous works by demonstrating that, stressful life events are associated with poorer executive function, which in turn predicts resilience and school adjustment in adolescents. Overall, these findings increase our understanding of how stressful life events might be associated with school adjustment among adolescents.

The current study is not without limitations, which also implies suggestions for future research. First, since the study was cross-sectional, it precluded deriving any causal relationship between the variables. To address this issue and facilitate a better understanding of causal mechanisms, future research should conduct longitudinal, experimental or intervention studies. Second, the present study is one of the few studies to utilize a self-reported executive function scale among adolescents. In order to achieve a better understanding of the role of executive function in the relationship between perceived stressful life events and school adjustment among adolescents, future research should compare the differences between experimental and self-report methods in adolescent population. Third, in the present study, participants completed the questionnaires with pen and paper, questionnaire survey was conducted in a physical way may have some limitations, that is, lack of control of the time dedicated to each response and some data encoding errors, etc.,. Some online questionnaire survey App (e.g., SurveyMonkey) could overcome these limitations. In order to obtain more scientific and rigorous research data, online questionnaire survey App should be used more often in the future. Forth, the present study was conducted in the background of Chinese population, the findings cannot provide differences between Chinese population and the population of the West in the psychological mechanisms underlying the relationship between stressful life events and school adjustment. Therefore, cross-cultural studies should be conducted to compare the differences between eastern and western population in the future.

Conclusion

In conclusion, to the best of our knowledge, the current study represents a new attempt to explore executive function and resilience as the underlying mechanisms in the relationship between perceived stressful life events and school adjustment among adolescents. Furthermore, as the significant indirect effects of perceived stressful life events on school adjustment separately via executive function or resilience, and a serial mediation effect of executive function and resilience were found, the current study also sheds light on how adolescents who perceived more stressful life events achieve worse school adjustment. The findings from the current study are significantly meaningful for conducting effective measures to break the cyclical relationship between stressful life events and school adjustment, thus having some practical implications.

Author Contributions

YZ and CG collected the data together. YZ, XZ, and CG analyzed the results and wrote the manuscript. LZ participated in the revision of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the project of Chongqing H&SS Key Research Institute, “The relationship among academic self, academic achievement and life satisfaction of adolescents: A comparison between cross-sectional and longitudinal data” (16SKB029).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Anderson, J. C., and Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 103, 411–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.3.411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anyan, F., and Hjemdal, O. (2016). Adolescent stress and symptoms of anxiety and depression: resilience explains and differentiates the relationships. J. Affect. Disord. 203, 213–220. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnett, J. J. (1999). Adolescent storm and stress, reconsidered. Am. Psychol. 54, 317–326. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.5.317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baars, M. A. E., Marije, N. B., Tonnaer, G. H., and Jelle, J. (2015). Self-report measures of executive functioning are a determinant of academic performance in first-year students at a university of applied sciences. Front. Psychol. 6:1131. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A., and Murphy, K. R. (2011). The nature of executive function (EF) deficits in daily life activities in adults with ADHD and their relationship to performance on EF tests. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 33, 137–158. doi: 10.1007/s10862-011-9217-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benight, C. C., and Cieslak, R. (2011). “Cognitive factors and resilience: How self-efficacy contributes to coping with adversities,” in Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, eds S. M. Southwick, B. T. Litz, and D. Charney (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 45–55. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511994791.005

CrossRef Full Text

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., and Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: changes and correlates. Dev. Rev. 29, 180–200. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Doyle, A. E., Seidman, L. J., Wilens, T. E., Ferrero, F., et al. (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 72, 757–766. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C., and Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 78, 647–663. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., and Thayer, J. F. (2006). The perseverative cognition hypothesis: a review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. J. Psychosom. Res. 60, 113–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bull, R., Espy, K. A., and Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Dev. Neuropsychol. 33, 205–228. doi: 10.1080/87565640801982312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, D. G., Davenport, S. C., and Mazanov, J. (2007). Profiles of adolescent stress: the development of the adolescent stress questionnaire (asq). J. Adolesc. 30, 393–416. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catalano, D., Chan, F., Wilson, L., Chiu, C. Y., and Muller, V. R. (2011). The buffering effect of resilience on depression among individuals with spinal cord injury: a structural equation model. Rehabil. Psychol. 56, 200–211. doi: 10.1037/a0024571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Li, D., Xu, X., Liu, J., Fu, R., Cui, L., et al. (2019). School adjustment of children from rural migrant families in urban China. J. Sch. Psychol. 72, 14–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2018.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, C., Prior, M., and Kinsella, G. (2002). The relationship between executive function abilities, adaptive behaviour, and academic achievement in children with externalising behaviour problems. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 43, 785–796. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, C. A., Pritchard, V. E., and Woodward, L. J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Dev. Psychol. 46, 1176–1191. doi: 10.1037/a0019672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, N. N., Nonterah, C. W., Utsey, S. O., Hook, J. N., Hubbard, R. R., Opare-Henaku, A., et al. (2015). Predictor and moderator effects of ego resilience and mindfulness on the relationship between academic stress and psychological well-being in a sample of Ghanaian college students. J. Black Psychol. 41, 340–357. doi: 10.1177/0095798414537939

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Compas, B. E. (1987). Stress and life events during childhood and adolescence. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 7, 275–302. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(87)90037-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Compas, B. E., Orosan, P. G., and Grant, K. E. (1993). Adolescent stress and coping: implications for psychopathology during adolescence. J. Adolesc. 16, 331–349. doi: 10.1006/jado.1993.1028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, J. R., Treece, P. D., Nielsen, E. L., Gold, J., Ciechanowski, P. S., Shannon, S. E., et al. (2016). Randomized trial of communication facilitators to reduce family distress and intensity of end-of-life care. Am. J. Respir. Criti. Care Med. 193, 154–162. doi: 10.1164/rccm.201505-0900OC

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidovich, S., Collishaw, S., Thapar, A. K., Harold, G., Thapar, A., and Rice, F. (2016). Do better executive functions buffer the effect of current parental depression on adolescent depressive symptoms? J. Affect. Disord. 199, 54–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.03.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duckworth, A. L., Kim, B., and Tsukayama, E. (2012). Life stress impairs self-control in early adolescence. Front. Psychol. 3:608. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dupéré, V., Dion, E., Leventhal, T., Archambault, I., Crosnoe, R., and Janosz, M. (2018). High school dropout in proximal context: the triggering role of stressful life events. Child Dev. 89, e107–e122. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, L. K., Rothbart, M. K., and Posner, M. I. (2004). Individual differences in executive attention predict self-regulation and adolescent psychosocial behaviors. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1021, 337–340. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espy, K. A., Mcdiarmid, M. M., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M. M., Hamby, A., and Senn, T. E. (2004). The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in preschool children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 26, 465–486. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2601_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fergus, S., and Zimmerman, M. A. (2004). Adolescent resilience: a framework for understanding healthy development in the face of risk. Ann. Rev. Public Health 26, 399–419. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, C., McKinnon, R. D., Blair, C. B., and Willoughby, M. T. (2014). Do preschool executive function skills explain the school readiness gap between advantaged and disadvantaged children? Learn. Instr. 30, 25–31. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Izquierdo, M., Ríos-Risquez, M. I., Carrillo-García, C., and Sabuco-Tebar, E. D. L. Á (2018). The moderating role of resilience in the relationship between academic burnout and the perception of psychological health in nursing students. Educ. Psychol. 38, 1068–1079. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2017.1383073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., and Kenworthy, L. (2000). Test review behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychol. 6, 235–238. doi: 10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 60, 549–576. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenaway, R., and Howlin, P. (2010). Dysfunctional attitudes and perfectionism and their relationship to anxious and depressive symptoms in boys with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 1179–1187. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0977-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, M. T. (2006). Promoting resilience in children and youth. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1094, 139–150. doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, L., Zhao, S. Y., Pan, X. Y., and Liao, C. J. (2018). The impact of students with left-behind experiences on childhood: the relationship between negative life events and depression among college students in China. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 64, 56–62. doi: 10.1177/0020764017739332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Modeling 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, C., Tang, Y., Wang, L., Xie, D., Fan, C., and Gao, W. (2014). Development of adolescent executive function scale. Chin. J. Behav. Med. Brain Sci. 23, 463–465.

Google Scholar

Hughes, C., and Ensor, R. (2011). Individual differences in growth in executive function across the transition to school predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors and self-perceived academic success at 6 years of age. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 663–676. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphreys, K. L., Watts, E. L., Dennis, E. L., King, L. S., Thompson, P. M., and Gotlib, I. H. (2018). Stressful life events, ADHD symptoms, and brain structure in early adolescence. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. doi: 10.1007/s10802-018-0443-5 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, L. A., Williford, A. P., and Pianta, R. C. (2011). The role of executive function in children’s competent adjustment to middle school. Child Neuropsychol. 17, 255–280. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2010.535654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, I., Kim, W. S., and Bae, S. C. (2018). Effects of adolescents’ academic stress on school life adjustment: focusing on mediator effect of resilience. Medico Legal Update 18, 290–295. doi: 10.5958/0974-1283.2018.00060.9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, M. S., Lydon, J., Goulet, L., Kahn, S. R., Mcnamara, H., Genest, J., et al. (2009). Stress pathways to spontaneous preterm birth: the role of stressors, psychological distress, and stress hormones. Am. J. Epidemiol. 169, 1319–1326. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lafavor, T. (2018). Predictors of academic success in 9-to 11-year-old homeless children: the role of executive function, social competence, and emotional control. J. Early Adolesc. 38, 1236–1264. doi: 10.1177/0272431616678989

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lan, X., Scrimin, S., and Moscardino, U. (2019). Perceived parental guan and school adjustment among chinese early adolescents: the moderating role of interdependent self-construal. J. Adolesc. 71, 18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S., Hwang, S., Song, Y., and Lee, H. (2014). Mediating effects of inter-organizational relation on the relation between resilience and school adjustment in elementary school students. J. Fish. Mar. Sci. Educ. 26, 1217–1230.

Google Scholar

Li, P., Zhang, J., Li, M., Li, P., Zhang, Y., Xin, Z., et al. (2012). Negative life events and mental health of chinese medical students: the effect of resilience, personality and social support. Psychiatry Res. 196, 138–141. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., and Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: a critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Dev. 71, 543–562. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maclean, M. J., Taylor, C. L., and O’Donnell, M. (2016). Pre-existing adversity, level of child protection involvement, and school attendance predict educational outcomes in a longitudinal study. Child Abuse Negl. 51, 120–131. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.10.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, A. S. (2014). Global perspectives on resilience in children and youth. Child Dev. 85, 6–20. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, A. S. (2015). Pathways to integrated resilience science. Psychol. Inq. 26, 187–196. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2015.1012041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, A. S., and Tellegen, A. (2012). Resilience in developmental psychopathology: contributions of the project competence longitudinal study. Dev. Psychopathol. 24, 345–361. doi: 10.1017/S095457941200003X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAuley, E., Mullen, S. P., Szabo, A. N., White, S. M., Wójcicki, T. R., Mailey, E. L., et al. (2011). Self-regulatory processes and exercise adherence in older adults: executive function and self-efficacy effects. Am. J. Preven. Med. 41, 284–290. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2011.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKee, J. (2017). Executive Functions and Resilience in First-Year Undergraduate Students. Doctoral dissertation, University of Calgary, Calgary.

Google Scholar

Migerode, F., Maes, B., Buysse, A., and Brondeel, R. (2012). Quality of life in adolescents with a disability and their parents: the mediating role of social support and resilience. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 24, 487–503. doi: 10.1007/s10882-012-9285-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (1998/2012). Mplus User’s Guide, 7th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Google Scholar

Navalta, C. P., Polcari, A., Webster, D. M., Boghossian, A., and Teicher, M. H. (2006). Effects of childhood sexual abuse on neuropsychological and cognitive function in college women. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 18, 45–53. doi: 10.1176/jnp.18.1.45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nesbitt, K. T., Baker-Ward, L., and Willoughby, M. T. (2013). Executive function mediates socio-economic and racial differences in early academic achievement. Early Childhood Res. Q. 28, 774–783. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuenschwander, R., Röthlisberger, M., Cimeli, P., and Roebers, C. M. (2012). How do different aspects of self-regulation predict successful adaptation to school? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 113, 353–371. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ni, C., Chow, M. C., Jiang, X., Li, S., and Pang, S. M. (2015). Factors associated with resilience of adult survivors five years after the 2008 Sichuan earthquake in China. PLoS One 10:e0121033. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishikawa, S., Fujisawa, T. X., Kojima, M., and Tomoda, A. (2018). Type and timing of negative life events are associated with adolescent Depression. Front. Psychiatry 9:41. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piccolo, L. R., and Noble, K. G. (2017). Perceived stress is associated with smaller hippocampal volume in adolescence. Psychophysiology 55:e13025. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 879–891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pukay-Martin, N. D., Cristiani, S. A., Saveanu, R., and Bornstein, R. A. (2003). The relationship between stressful life events and cognitive function in HIV-infected men. J. Neuropsychiatry 15, 436–441. doi: 10.1176/jnp.15.4.436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reising, M. M., Bettis, A. H., Dunbar, J. P., Watson, K. H., Gruhn, M., Hoskinson, K. R., et al. (2018). Stress, coping, executive function, and brain activation in adolescent offspring of depressed and nondepressed mothers. Child Neuropsychol. 24, 638–656. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2017.1307950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rike, P. O., Johansen, H. J., Ulleberg, P., Lundqvist, A., and Schanke, A. K. (2015). Exploring associations between self-reported executive functions, impulsive personality traits, driving self-efficacy, and functional abilities in driver behaviour after brain injury. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 29, 34–47. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2015.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rod, N. H., Grønbaek, M., Schnohr, P., Prescott, E., and Kristensen, T. S. (2009). Perceived stress as a risk factor for changes in health behaviour and cardiac risk profile: a longitudinal study. J. Intern. Med. 266, 467–475. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2009.02124.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saleh, A., Potter, G. G., McQuoid, D. R., Boyd, B., Turner, R., MacFall, J. R., et al. (2017). Effects of early life stress on depression, cognitive performance and brain morphology. Psychol. Med. 47, 171–181. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716002403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandler, I. N., Tein, J. Y., and West, S. G. (1994). Coping, stress, and the psychological symptoms of children of divorce: a cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Child Dev. 65, 1744–1763. doi: 10.2307/1131291

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schafer, J. L., and Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: our view of the state of the art. Psychol. Methods 7, 147–177. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seery, M. D. (2011). Resilience: a silver lining to experiencing adverse life events? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 390–394. doi: 10.1177/0963721411424740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sung, Y. T., Huang, L. Y., Tseng, F. L., and Chang, K. E. (2014). The aspects and ability groups in which little fish perform worse than big fish: examining the big-fish-little-pond effect in the context of school tracking. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 39, 220–232. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thurston, I. B., Hardin, R., Kamody, R. C., Herbozo, S., and Kaufman, C. (2018). The moderating role of resilience on the relationship between perceived stress and binge eating symptoms among young adult women. Eat. Behav. 29, 114–119. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2018.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troy, A. S., and Mauss, I. B. (2011). Resilience in the Face of Stress: Emotion Regulation as a Protective Factor. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 30–44.

Google Scholar

Wagnild, G. M., and Young, H. M. (1993). Development and psychometric evaluation of the resilience scale. J. Nurs. Measur. 1, 165–178.

Google Scholar

Welsh, J. A., Nix, R. L., Blair, C., Bierman, K. L., and Nelson, K. E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. J. Educ. Psychol. 102, 43–53. doi: 10.1037/a0016738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xin, X., Ming, Q., Zhang, J., Wang, Y., Liu, M., and Yao, S. (2016). Four distinct subgroups of self-injurious behavior among Chinese adolescents: findings from a latent class analysis. PLoS One 11:e0158609. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0158609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xin, X. H., and Yao, S. Q. (2015). Validity and reliability of the adolescent self-rating life events checklist in middle school students. Chin. Ment. Health J. 29, 355–360.

Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Chen, Y., Zhou, H., and Wang, L. (2012). The influence of daily stressful life events on teenage students’ social adjustment: social problem-solving ability as a mediator. J. Psychol. Sci. 35, 1376–1382. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2012.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Chen, X., Ran, G., and Ma, Y. (2016). Adult children’s support and self-esteem as mediators in the relationship between attachment and subjective well-being in older adults. Personal. Individ. Dif. 97, 229–233. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, Y.-H., Fan, F., Yu, C.-F., and Luo, T.-C. (2011). Relationship between gratitude and symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder among adolescents: mediation of social support and resilience. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 27, 522–528. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2011.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, H., Lu, G., and Liu, H. (2012). Explore the correlations among resilience, stress and school adjustment of middle school students. Med. Soc. 25, 71–73. doi: 10.3870/YXYSH.2012.05.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stressful life events, school adjustment, executive function, resilience, adolescents

Citation: Zhang Y, Zhang X, Zhang L and Guo C (2019) Executive Function and Resilience as Mediators of Adolescents’ Perceived Stressful Life Events and School Adjustment. Front. Psychol. 10:446. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00446

Received: 05 October 2018; Accepted: 13 February 2019;
Published: 28 February 2019.

Edited by:

Laura Elvira Prino, University of Turin, Italy

Reviewed by:

Martina Smorti, University of Pisa, Italy
Jesús Nicasio García Sánchez, Universidad de León, Spain

Copyright © 2019 Zhang, Zhang, Zhang and Guo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cheng Guo, guochen@swu.edu.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work as co-first authors

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.