Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 14 March 2019
Sec. Psychology of Language

Commentary: Titi semantics: Context and meaning in Titi monkey call sequences

\r\nSwan CommierSwan CommierMlissa Berthet*Mélissa Berthet*
  • Institut Jean Nicod, Département d'études Cognitives, ENS, EHESS, CNRS, PSL Research University, Paris, France

A Commentary on
Titi semantics: Context and meaning in Titi monkey call sequences

by Schlenker, P., Chemla, E., Cäsar, C., Ryder, R., and Zuberbühler, K. (2017). Nat. Lang. Linguist. Theory 35, 271–298. doi: 10.1007/s11049-016-9337-9

Analyses of non-human vocal systems inspired by formal linguistics have recently emerged and appear fruitful to help understand the semantic properties of animal vocalizations (Schlenker et al., 2016b). The vocal system of one primate species has been particularly illuminated by such a perspective, the Black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons). This species uses two alarm calls, the A-call and the B-call, and arranges them into diverse sequences. The composition of those sequences differs depending on the type and location of the predator (Cäsar et al., 2013), as shown in (1):

(1) a. A raptor in a non-ground position (flying or perched in the canopy) elicits a sequence of A-calls, (hereafter A+)

b. A raptor on the ground elicits a sequence of A-calls followed by B-calls (A+B+)

c. A cat in the canopy elicits a sequence a single A-call followed by B-calls (AB+)

d. A cat on the ground /the presence of a terrestrial animal/ a monkey descending near the ground elicits a sequence of B-calls only (B+).

Schlenker et al. (2017) proposed that (i) an A-call refers to a serious non-ground threats; (ii) a B-call is given to noteworthy events; (iii) an Informativity Principle mandates that the more informative A-call should be used instead of the less informative B-calls whenever it applies to the situation—a principle independently motivated in similar investigations in other species, such as Campbell's monkeys and Putty-nosed monkeys (Schlenker et al., 2016b). Moreover, Schlenker et al. (2017) stated that each call that is produced constitutes an independent utterance and reflects the state of the environment at the exact time of production. In that sense, a combination of calls is viewed as a sequence of calls that each have their independent meaning, regardless of their position and the other calls composing the sequence. Thus, Schlenker et al. (2017) derived the following analysis (2) for the generalizations in (1):

(2) a. A+ is given to a serious non-ground threat

b. a raptor on the ground attacks by flying, hence it is treated as a non-ground threat (A+) that stops being serious at some point because it is not in a hunting position (hence B+)

c. a cat in the canopy is a serious non-ground threat (hence A). However, the cat's hunting strategy relies on surprise, and spotting a felid may deter it from attacking (Zuberbühler et al., 1999). Thus, after the emission of one A-call, the cat stops being a serious threat for it has been spotted and may stop hunting: A-calls cannot be used anymore, and B+ is therefore uttered

d. in ground and/or non-serious alerts only B can be used.

However, Cäsar (2011) argued that aerial predators represent the major danger to Titi monkeys, which suggests that the meaning “serious non-ground threat” of A-calls is redundant. We argue that the specification of the location can be eliminated from the meaning of A-call, resulting in a simplification such as A-calls refer to “serious threat.” The generalizations in (1) can thus be analyzed as follow (3):

(3) a. A+ is given to a serious threat

b. A raptor on the ground is still a dangerous threat (A+) but stops being serious at some point for it is not in a hunting position (hence B+)

c. a cat in the canopy is a serious threat (A) that stops being serious once spotted (B+)

d. B are used in non-serious alerts.

In Schlenker et al. (2017), A-calls referred to the level of alarm (“serious”) and the location of the threat (“non-ground”) while we argue that A-calls only refer to the level of alarm. Our analysis is therefore more parsimonious, and in line with the animal communication literature that described several animal communication systems as urgency-based (e.g., Rauber and Manser, 2017).

When hearing A-calls, titi monkeys mainly look upwards (Cäsar et al., 2012). Schlenker et al. (2017) suggested that the semantics of A-calls comprised a “non-ground” component that could explain the behavioral reaction of the monkeys. However, we think that environmental knowledge actually provides a better explanation to account for the observed behavior: if serious threats usually come from above and titi monkeys know this, then the most adaptive reaction to hearing a conspecific signaling “serious alert” is to look upwards.

We reckon that decisive field experiments are now necessary to verify the meaning of A-calls. One could, for instance, present titi monkeys that are on the ground with a cat model. According to Schlenker et al. (2017), this would elicit B-calls, because the cat is not a serious non-ground threat when on the ground. Our revised theory predicts that it would elicit A-calls, because the cat on the ground becomes a serious threat when the monkeys are not in the trees.

Beyond reflecting on the titi monkeys alarm system, our own analysis proves that the approach introduced by Schlenker et al. (2016a,b) and encouraged elsewhere (e.g., Seyfarth and Cheney, 2016) is likely to be very productive in the future. Their approach allows the building of clear hypotheses, that can be criticized and revised, and that can lead to very clear predictions on the semantics of animal systems that can eventually be tested in the field.

Author Contributions

SC: conception and first draft; MB: further redaction and revision of the manuscript.

Funding

The research leading to these results received funding from the European Research Council under the European Union's Seventh Framework Programme (FP/2007-2013)/ERC Grant Agreement N°324115–FRONTSEM (PI: Schlenker). Institut d'Etudes Cognitives is supported by grants ANR-10-LABX-0087 IEC et ANR-10-IDEX-0001-02 PSL.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Philippe Schlenker, Emmanuel Chemla, and Guillaume Dezecache for their helpful insights on the manuscript and their unfailing encouragements.

References

Cäsar, C. (2011). Anti-predator Behaviour of Black-Fronted Titi Monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons). Doctoral dissertation, University of St. Andrews.

Cäsar, C., Byrne, R. W., Hoppitt, W., Young, R. J., and Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Evidence for semantic communication in titi monkey alarm calls. Anim. Behav. 84, 405–411. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.05.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cäsar, C., Zuberbühler, K., Young, R. W., and Byrne, R. W. (2013). Titi monkey call sequences vary with predator location and type. Biol. Lett. 9:20130535. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauber, R., and Manser, M. B. (2017). Discrete call types referring to predation risk enhance the efficiency of the meerkat sentinel system. Sci. Rep. 7:44436. doi: 10.1038/srep44436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlenker, P., Chemla, E., Cäsar, C., Ryder, R., and Zuberbühler, K. (2017). Titi semantics: context and meaning in Titi monkey call sequences. Nat. Lang. Linguist. Theory 35, 271–298. doi: 10.1007/s11049-016-9337-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlenker, P., Chemla, E., Schel, A. M., Fuller, J., Gautier, J.-P., Kuhn, J., et al. (2016b). Formal monkey linguistics. Theor. Linguist. 42, 1–90. doi: 10.1515/tl-2016-0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlenker, P., Chemla, E., and Zuberbühler, K. (2016a). What do monkey calls mean? Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 894–904. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2016). Schlenker et al.'s informativity principle. Theor. Linguist. 42, 155–158. doi: 10.1515/tl-2016-0008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuberbühler, K., David, J., and Bshary, R. (1999). The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology 105, 477–490.

Google Scholar

Keywords: semantics, formal linguistics, titi monkey, vocalizations, primate, alarm

Citation: Commier S and Berthet M (2019) Commentary: Titi semantics: Context and meaning in Titi monkey call sequences. Front. Psychol. 10:512. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00512

Received: 13 December 2018; Accepted: 20 February 2019;
Published: 14 March 2019.

Edited by:

Sidarta Ribeiro, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Shane Steinert-Threlkeld, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands
Daniel Yasumasa Takahashi, Princeton University, United States

Copyright © 2019 Commier and Berthet. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mélissa Berthet, melissa.berthet.ac@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.