Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 16 July 2019
Sec. Organizational Psychology

Are Effect Sizes in Emotional Intelligence Field Declining? A Meta-Meta Analysis

  • Department of Psychology, Normal College, Qingdao University, Qingdao, China

Since Salovey and Mayer (1990) first proposed the emotional intelligence (EI) as an independent intellectual component, research on the field of EI has developed rapidly. A large number of studies have shown that emotional intelligence is an important predictor that affects our lives, such as job performance, mental health, and so on. However, we observed that some effect sizes in the field of emotional intelligence decreased over time. Is this kind of decline simply due to random errors, or is emotional intelligence field undergoing decline effects? The present study analyzed 484 effect sizes based on the responses of 102,579 participants from nine meta-analyses in emotional intelligence field to estimate the average effect size, and evidence for decline effects in this field. This study finds that the average effect size of EI is 0.244 (p < 0.001), and the average effect size of mixed EI (r = 0.272, p < 0.001) is significantly higher than that of ability EI (r = 0.160, p < 0.001). Effect sizes in the field of EI decrease with time, there are decline effects in emotional intelligence field. Furthermore, there are also decline effects in mixed EI field. However, we find no evidence that there are decline effects in ability EI field. Base on the significant average effect size of mixed EI, the most likely explanation for the decline in effect sizes is that effect sizes of mixed EI in the original studies were overestimate. This study considers decline effects in mixed EI research as inflated decline effects. To sum up, decline effects in the field of emotional intelligence are mainly due to the choice of emotional intelligence model and measurement method.

Introduction

Can results of psychological research be replicated? With the discovery that a large number of published psychological research results could not be successfully replicated in new samples, the problem has received increasing attention, and researchers have found that many results were overly optimistic and effect sizes were overestimated (Francis, 2014). Based on this, COS organization repeated 100 psychological studies (Hartgerink and Pernet, 2015), they found that the average effect size in repetitive studies was half of that in the original studies. The original studies' results in 97% were statistically significant, but only 36% of repetitive studies had significant results. Therefore, the researchers began to doubt the credibility of psychological research results and discuss “replicability crisis” (Baker, 2016). The replicability crisis is a term used by psychologists in the current introspection phase, and the psychology community is undergoing a revolution (Spellman, 2015).

Because of the increasing emphasis on the role of emotional intelligence in today's society, it is generally accepted that the predictive power of emotional intelligence measurement to individual success transcends the predictive power of traditional cognitive measurement (Zeidner et al., 1998). Therefore, since the first systematic study of emotional intelligence by Mayer and Salovey in the 1990s, emotional intelligence has quickly aroused widespread concern and scientific interest among many scholars. Research on emotional intelligence has been in full swing for the past 30 years. So, does emotional intelligence field also experience a replicability crisis?

The decline effect is the phenomenon of decreasing effect sizes in repeated studies over time (Schooler, 2011). Decline effect is an important indicator to explore the replicability crisis. Therefore, the present study examines whether emotional intelligence experiences a replicability crisis by studying decline effects in the field of emotional intelligence.

Emotional Intelligence

Psychologists give many different definitions of emotional intelligence, represented by three genres, which are more popular in psychology:

The first genre is ability model. They defined emotional intelligence as “the ability to perceive accurately, appraise, and express emotion; the ability to access and/or generate feelings when they facilitate thought; the ability to understand emotion and emotional knowledge; and the ability to regulate emotions to promote emotional and intellectual growth” (Mayer and Salovey, 1997). They separated personality traits from emotional intelligence through a multi-factor intelligence scale and claimed that emotional intelligence was independent of personality traits (Mayer et al., 2003).

The second genre is Goleman's emotional intelligence model. Unlike Salovey and Mayer's views, Goleman considered that emotional intelligence is based on both cognitive ability and personality trait. He defined emotional intelligence as a universal ability that every normal person has, and a quantifiable dimension that embodies individual differences, and that individuals can be sorted by emotional measurements within a certain range (Goleman, 1995). Moreover, the four important components that make up emotional intelligence were: self-awareness, social awareness, self-management and interpersonal management (Goleman, 1996). In 1998, Goleman improved his model and proposed the concept of emotional competence, which he defined as “an ability to recognize, understand and use emotional information about oneself or others that leads to or causes effective or superior performance” (Goleman, 1998).

The third genre is Bar-On model. Bar-On defined emotional intelligence as “a cross-section of interrelated emotional and social competencies, skills and facilitators that determine how effectively we understand and express ourselves, understand others and relate with them, and cope with daily demands.” (Bar-On, 1997). And there were the five key components of emotional intelligence included: Intrapersonal, Interpersonal, Stress Management, Adaptability and General Mood (Bar-On, 2004). Based on these five components, he designed the first standardized emotional intelligence scale (Bar-On Emotion Quotient Inventory, EQ-i) (Bar-On, 2006).

These three genres can be divided into two models, namely, mixed model and ability model. Mayer and Salovey's ability model belongs to ability EI, whereas, Goleman's model and Bar-On model belong to mixed EI. Mixed EI and ability EI are completely different conceptual frameworks, rather than different means of measuring the same concept. Ability model considers emotional intelligence to be an independent intellectual component and uses objective measurement methods. Whereas mixed model considers that emotional intelligence is distinct from personality and general cognitive ability, and uses the self-report scale (Bar-On, 2000; Byrne et al., 2007). Based on differences between two models, the present study will be discussed separately decline effects in mixed EI and ability EI.

Since Salovey and Mayer (1990) first proposed EI as an independent intellectual component, research on the field of EI has developed rapidly. Many studies have shown that EI is not only an important factor affecting individual success, but also a direct factor affecting individual mental health (Stewart-Brown, 1998; Bar-On, 2016).

Many studies carried out in recent years have linked EI with a number of variables. some studies have reported that individuals with high EI were more likely to have strong social adaptability and more emotional skills, thus individuals with high EI were more likely to report positive relationships with others and more parental support (Fabio, 2015). Some studies showed that EI influenced life satisfaction (Extremera and Rey, 2016). Some studies have also found that EI could positively predict job performance and academic performance (Bar-On et al., 2006; Newman et al., 2010; Sharma, 2011; Bar-On, 2018). And EI could negatively predict drug and alcohol involvement (Peterson et al., 2011). In addition, EI was negatively correlated with suicidal behavior (Domínguez-García and Fernández-Berrocal, 2018). To sum up, EI is an important topic in psychological research, and the research on EI is developing rapidly (Sharma, 2008).

Decline Effect

The decline effect is a phenomenon in which effect sizes decrease with time, and it is a pattern of bias. Protzko and Schooler (2015) divided decline effects into 4 types: False positive decline effects, Inflated decline effects, Under-specified decline effects and Genuinely decreasing decline effects.

False positive decline effects mean that effect sizes of subsequent studies decrease over time, because there is no true effect. Significant results in the original studies originated from the errors of statistics or methods. For example, Mozart's music has non-significant effect on cognition, the original result was a statistical fluke (Pietschnig et al., 2010).

Inflated decline effects mean that effect sizes of subsequent studies decrease over time, because original studies overestimated effect sizes (Protzko and Schooler, 2015). For instance, effect sizes that men with symmetrical secondary sexual characteristics had advantage in selecting mates decreased, because for reducing measurement errors subsequent studies no longer used single-exposure methods (Swaddle et al., 1994).

Under-specified decline effects mean that effect sizes of subsequent studies decrease over time, because a necessary condition in original studies was under-specified. For example, in the economic game of intuition promoting cooperation, the original study did not report that all the participates were new to this game, which led to a decline in effect sizes of subsequent studies (Rand et al., 2012).

Genuinely decreasing decline effects mean that effect sizes of subsequent studies decrease over time, due to social developments. For instance, with the cultural development, the prejudice of white students toward African Americans decreased (Dovidio and Gaertner, 1986).

Therefore, are there decline effects in emotional intelligence field? If so, what type of decline effects are they?

Meta-Meta Analysis

Meta-Meta analysis is a quantitative re-analysis on a series of meta-analyses, and it is an important way to examine whether there are biases in a certain field (Cumming, 2012, 2014; Fox et al., 2014). The rationales of Meta-Meta analysis are the same as that of meta-analysis, and there are also strict screening criteria and coding system. However, there are still differences, for example, the research object of meta-analysis is primary study, and the research object of Meta-Meta analysis is meta-analysis. Additionally, Meta-analysis is mainly applicable to study relationship between variables or whether interventions are effective, whereas Meta-Meta analysis is mainly used to study biases in a certain field. Biases affect the development of science, and they are important causes of replicability crises. Therefore, Metascience could rescue replicability crises (Schooler, 2014).

Fanelli and Ioannidis (2013) used Meta-Meta analysis to re-analyze 82 meta-analyses in softer research, and found evidences of “US effect.” US effect means that “US studies may overestimate effect sizes in softer research” (Fanelli and Ioannidis, 2013). Then, Fanelli et al. (2017) using Meta-Meta analysis to explore biases in the whole field of science (including Small-study effects, Gray literature bias, Decline effect, Early-extreme, Citation bias, US effect and Industry bias). Following Fanelli et al. (2017), we will look at decline effects in the field of emotional intelligence.

Objectives and Hypotheses

By reading the literature, we find that predictive effects of emotional intelligence seem to be declining. For example, the predictive effect of mixed EI on academic performance has decreased over time. In 1998, Schutte reported the effect size (r) between mixed EI and academic performance was 0.374 (Schutte et al., 1998). In 2005, Austin reported the effect size (r) between those was 0.283 (Austin et al., 2005). And in 2010, Radford reported the effect size (r) of that was 0.112 (Radford, 2012).

Effect sizes of the ability EI field are also observed decreasing. In 2006, Côté and Miners reported the effect size (r) between ability EI and cognitive ability was 0.47 (Côté and Miners, 2006). Then, in 2011, Fiori and Autonakis reported the effect size (r) between those was 0.36 (Fiori and Antonakis, 2011). In 2014, Grunes reported the effect size of that was 0.26 (Grunes et al., 2014).

Effect sizes in the field of emotional intelligence appear to be declining. Is this kind of decline simply due to random errors, or is emotional intelligence field undergoing decline effects? All above lead to the present study with the following objectives (O) and hypotheses (H):

O1. Determine whether there are decline effects in the field of EI. We hypothesized that there were decline effects in the field of EI, in other words, effect sizes in the field of EI decreased with time (H1).

O2. Determine whether there are decline effects in the field of mixed EI. We hypothesized that there were decline effects in the field of mixed EI, in other words, effect sizes in the field of mixed EI decreased with time (H2).

O3. Determine whether there are decline effects in the field of ability EI. We hypothesized that there were decline effects in the field of ability EI, in other words, effect sizes in the field of ability EI decreased with time (H3).

O4. Discuss the specific type of decline effects in the field of EI. And explore the causes of decline effects in the field of EI.

Methods

Search Strategy and Selection Criteria

We searched for meta-analyses about EQ and emotional intelligence on the 28th of September, 2018, on ISI Web of Knowledge, using the search string “TOPIC: (EQ OR Emotional Intelligence) AND TOPIC: (meta-analysis).” This rendered 258 records. And the present study selection criteria included that meta-analysis should be unique, available, quantitative, and related to emotional intelligence, using at least one specific emotional intelligence measure and providing full data table.

From these 258 records, we excluded seven duplicate ones and 185 records unrelated to emotional intelligence. We then excluded 11 articles that were not a quantitative meta-analysis, and 12 records in which the article was not available in our university library. We excluded 10 articles, because articles need to include at least one specific emotional intelligence measure (EI measures see Table 1). We then excluded 24 articles because of lack of (full) data table, non-standard method, or too few studies. For one primary study, we were not able to calculate the effect size, because information on effect size was missing, so this was excluded from our analyses. Our final sample consisted of nine meta-analyses, included 484 unique primary studies, and based on 102,579 participants (see Figure 1 for a schematic overview of the exclusion criteria and meta-analysis selection).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Emotional intelligence measure.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A schematic overview of the exclusion criteria and meta-analysis selection.

Coding Procedures

After the literature screening, we encoded all the primary studies from collected meta-analyses, in which repetitive primary studies were coded only once. This way, we ensured the independence of the primary studies. For each unique primary study, we recorded the effect size and its standard error. Next, we recorded the primary studies' total sample size and the sample size per condition. Then, we recorded the publication year of the primary study, and we coded the relative order of publication year (studies published in different months of the same year are regarded as published at the same time). Furthermore, we coded the EI model which the primary study was based on. We created a binary variable to indicate if a study was based on mixed model (1) or ability model (0). To ensure reliability of the present study was robust, two independent recorders (i.e., the first author and the second author) double-coded all the primary studies from collected meta-analyses, achieving an intra-class correlation (ICC) of 0.95 of coding comparison, and then two independent recorders discussed and corrected discrepancies in individual coding.

Effect Size Conversion

For meta-analyses, it should convert all the effect sizes to a single type. All the effect size we extracted from nine meta-analyses were correlation (r), so we chose correlation (r) as the type of effect size. We subsequently converted all r's to Fisher's Z values, because the standard error then only depends on the sample size and not on the correlation itself (Sterne and Egger, 2006).

Direction of effect sizes can affect results. To correct for any influence of direction, we used a procedure called “coining,” following Fanelli et al. (2017). This procedure assumed that if the meta-analytic average effect size was negative, the expected direction of primary effect sizes was also negative. And it should multiply all primary effect sizes whose direction was negative by −1. In the present study, we considered the relationship between trait psychopathy and EI as negative, and we also considered the relationship between alcohol involvement and EI as negative.

Results

All meta-analyses in the present study were carried out using CMA 2.0. Publication bias analysis, heterogeneity analysis, average effect size calculation, meta regression were carried out, and results were as follow.

Publication Bias

The analysis of published bias involves study size and effect size. The mechanism for displaying the relationship between study size and effect size is the funnel plot. Traditionally, the funnel plot was plotted with effect size on the X axis and the sample size or variance on the Y axis. In the absence of publication bias, the studies will be distributed symmetrically about the mean effect size, since the sampling error is random. Figure 2 shows that there is little likelihood of publication bias in emotional intelligence field. Therefore, the results of the present study are not affected by publication bias.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Funnel Plot of Standard Error by Fisher's Z.

Heterogeneity Analysis

As shown in Table 2, over all Q statistic is 4803.808 and its p-value is less than 0.001, which shows that effect sizes in the field of EI are heterogeneous. And effect sizes in the field of mixed EI are heterogeneous (Q = 3882.462, df = 375, p < 0.001), effect sizes in the field of ability EI are also heterogeneous (Q = 734.104, df = 107, p < 0.001). And the difference between mixed EI and ability EI is significant (Q = 187.241, df = 1, p < 0.001). Therefore, we should choose random-effects model rather than fixed-effects model in following analyses.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Heterogeneity analysis.

Average Effect Size Calculation

As shown in Table 3, the average effect size of EI is 0.244 (p < 0.001). The average effect size of mixed EI is 0.272 (p < 0.001), and the average effect size of ability EI is 0.160 (p < 0.001). The average effect size of mixed EI is significantly higher than that of ability EI.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Average effect size calculation.

Meta Regression

As shown in Table 4, the relationship between effect sizes of overall EI and time is stronger than we would except by chance (Qmodel = 17.09059, df = 1, p < 0.001). With time in the model, the between-studies variance can be explained (Qresidual = 480.98990, df = 482, p > 0.05). And the slope is less than zero significantly (Z = −4.13408, p < 0.001). Therefore, the relationship of time to effect is y = −0.01133x+ 0.38726, where x is the order of publication year. We can plot this in Figure 3. It illustrates that effect sizes in the field of EI decreases with time, there are decline effects in emotional intelligence field. This indicates that hypothesis 1 is acceptable.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Mixed effects regression of time (EI).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Regression of Time on Fisher's Z (EI).

In Table 5, the relationship between effect sizes of mixed EI and time is significant (Qmodel = 14.72518, df = 1, p < 0.001). With time in the model, the between-studies variance can be explained (Qresidual = 369.30960, df = 374, p > 0.05). And the slope is less than zero significantly (Z = −3.83734, p < 0.001). Therefore, the relationship of time to effect is y = −0.01247x+ 0.42108, where x is the order of publication year. We can plot this in Figure 4. It shows that effect sizes in the field of mixed EI decreases with time, there are decline effects in mixed EI field. This indicates that hypothesis 2 is acceptable. In Table 6, the relationship between effect sizes of ability EI and time is non-significant (Qmodel = 0.00218, df = 1, p > 0.05). We can plot this in Figure 5. It illustrates that effect sizes in the field of ability EI do not decrease with time, we find no evidence that there are decline effects in ability EI field. This indicates that hypothesis 3 is rejected.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Mixed effects regression of time (Mixed EI).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Regression of Time on Fisher's Z (Mixed EI).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Mixed effects regression of time (Ability EI).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Regression of Time on Fisher's Z (Ability EI).

Discussion

The present study is the first to our knowledge that provides evidences that there are decline effects in the field of EI. This finding is consistent with hypothesis H1 and with previous studies showing that psychology is facing replicability crises. There are two possible explanations for this. One is that effect sizes of mixed EI field decrease, because there are 77.7% primary studies based on the mixed model. The other is that the number of primary studies in mixed EI decrease sharply after 2008 (see Figure 6). Though the average effect size of mixed EI is significantly higher than that of ability EI, the number of primary studies in mixed EI decrease, leading to the decrease of effect sizes in entire EI. However, the latter explanation is limited by sample size of the present study, the total sample size before 2008 was twice as big as that after 2008, so the number of primary studies in mixed EI decrease, possibly due to smaller total sample size.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Line chart.

Our finding is also consistent with hypothesis H2, suggesting that effect sizes in mixed EI decline over time, which is the most important finding in this study. According to Protzko and Schooler's classification of decline effects (see section Decline Effect), there are 4 types of decline effects: False positive decline effects, Inflated decline effects, Under-specified decline effects and Genuinely decreasing decline effects. And here is a discussion about the type of decline effects in mixed EI. This kind of decline effects does not belong to false positive decline effects, because of the significant average effect size of mixed EI. Various effects in the entire mixed EI field decline rather than a particular effect, and there are no unclear specific conditions, therefore this kind of decline effect is not under-specified decline effect. The emphasis on emotional intelligence does not decline with society development, so this does not belong to genuinely decreasing decline effects. Base on the significant average effect size of mixed EI, the most likely explanation for this decline is that effect sizes of mixed EI in the original studies were overestimated. Therefore, we consider decline effects in mixed EI research as inflated decline effects.

And there are three possible reasons why effect sizes in mixed EI were overestimated. One is that mixed model does not have a high degree of independence. There is a high overlap between mixed EI and general factors of personality (Rooy and Viswesvaran, 2004; Linden et al., 2017; Alegre et al., 2019). And there are overlaps among mixed EI, optimism, and positive emotions (Davies et al., 1998). The second reason is the change of mixed EI measurement method. The original measurement method of mixed EI is self-report scale, and the reliability and objectivity of it has been controversial. Studies have found that the level of intelligence by self-report scale had nothing to do with its actual level of intelligence (Paulhus, 2010). Therefore, psychologists supplemented 360-degree feedback based on the original self-report scale. Three hundred and sixty degree Feedback refers to “different raters—typically a manager (boss), peers, direct reports, and others to rater a specific person” (Adrian et al., 2014). Three hundred and sixty degree feedback effectively improves the accuracy and reliability of measurements (Furnham, 2008). Psychologists are also trying experimental methods and neuroscience methods such as brain mapping (Takeuchi et al., 2013).

Thirdly, with deepening studies on emotional intelligence, “Dark” Side of emotional intelligence are also revealed, which also may lead to the decline of EI effect sizes (Davis and Nichols, 2016). For example, some studies found that emotional attention may make influence of negative emotions bigger, which leads to depression in female. And emotional sensitivity may lead to stress reactivity, which is not conducive to interpersonal relationship (Davis and Nichols, 2016). What's worse, high emotional intelligence may lead to emotional manipulation and deception, which makes interpersonal relationship utilitarian and insincere (Grieve and Panebianco, 2013). In the early stage of the study, psychologists only focused on positive effects of emotional intelligence and ignored the negative effects of it, which may be the reason for overestimating effect sizes in mixed EI.

Therefore, how to improve the independence of mixed EI is an urgent problem. Only by reducing the overlap between mixed EI and other psychological structures will the prediction effects of it not be doubted. In follow-up studies, psychologists should reconsider the definition of mixed EI. And the most important, the attitude toward results should be changed, and scientists should not blindly pursue significant results, and ignore non-significant results. We should pay sufficient attention to the results that are non-significant or negative, and explain the positive results with caution. At present, many scientists advocate preregistration, that is, researchers openly register their studies before research (Miguel et al., 2014). Journals that have accepted a pre-registered study, will publish this study, regardless of its results significant or not, which makes it possible to publish non-significant results.

As for ability EI, this study has found no evidence that there are decline effects in ability EI research. And we consider that there are two possible explanations. One is that ability EI has a good independence, which will not be overestimated by the overlap with other psychological structures. The other explanation is that ability EI is measured by objective method, thus ability EI also has a good reliability and stability, there is little chance that effect sizes fluctuate over time.

This study also has an important finding that the average effect size of ability EI is significantly lower than that of mixed EI. The ability model is independent of other intellectual components, but also independent of the concept of personality. It does not include some psychological attributes, tendencies and characteristics of “non-ability” involved in mixed model, thus the predictive power of the ability model is relatively weak.

Although the present study shows that ability EI has good reliability, but this does not mean that it has a good validity, that is, effectiveness of measurement on emotional intelligence. The low predictive power of ability EI may also because its measurement of ability EI is not effective. This may be related to the definition of emotional intelligence in ability model. Ability model consists of four emotional ability: the ability to perceive, appraise, and express emotion; the ability to access and generate feelings which facilitate thought; the ability to understand emotion; and the ability to regulate emotions (Mayer et al., 1999). These four abilities cannot explain the complexity of emotional intelligence in interpersonal relationship. Therefore, the present study suggests that researchers in ability EI should reconsider the definition of emotional intelligence and expand its connotation in follow-up studies, in order to improve the effectiveness of its measurement. At the same time, studies in emotional intelligence should not be limited to ability model and mixed model, emotional intelligence should be expanded in more directions. At present, many studies begin to explore the evidence of emotional intelligence based on neuroscience (Waldman et al., 2011; Barbey et al., 2014; Boyatzis and Jack, 2018). Emotional intelligence is moving toward more scientific and objective approaches to research.

Conclusion

We find evidences that there are decline effects in emotional intelligence field, and decline effects in the field of emotional intelligence are mainly due to the choice of emotional intelligence model and measurement method. Original studies in mixed EI may overestimate effect sizes, so effect sizes decrease obviously over time, whereas studies in ability EI have better stability and effect sizes in this field are not affected by time. However, the predictive power of the ability model is weaker than that of mixed model. Therefore, the problems to be solved in the field of mixed EI are how to reduce the overlap between mixed EI and other psychological structures, and how to improve the stability of the scale. As for ability EI field, how to improve the predictive power is a problem to be solved at present.

Data Availability

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this manuscript will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Author Contributions

ZG and XJ designed, performed, and analyzed the research. XJ wrote up the research. ZG critically reviewed and edited the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the National Social Science Fund of China under Grant No. 14CGL073 awarded to the first author.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adrian, F., Mary-Clare, R., and Adrienne, R. (2014). Emotional intelligence and the occupational personality questionnaire (opq). Front. Psychol. 5:935. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00935

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alegre, A., Pérez-Escoda, N., and López-Cassá, E. (2019). The Relationship between trait emotional intelligence and personality. is trait EI really anchored within the big five, big two and big one frameworks? Front. Psychol. 10:866. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Austin, E. J., Evans, P., Goldwater, R., and Potter, V. (2005). A preliminary study of emotional intelligence, empathy and exam performance in first year medical students. Pers. Individ. Dif. 39, 1395–1405. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.04.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, M. (2016). 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility. Nature 533, 452–454. doi: 10.1038/533452a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., Paul, E. J., Chau, A., Solomon, J., and Grafman, J. H. (2014). Lesion mapping of social problem solving. Brain 137, 2823–2833. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-On, R. (1997). The Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i): Technical Manual. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems.

Bar-On, R. (2000). “Emotional and social intelligence: insights from the Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i),” in Handbook of Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Development, Assessment and Application at Home, School and in the Workplace, eds R. Bar-On and J. D. A. Parker (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass, 363–388.

Bar-On, R. (2004). “The Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i): rationale, description, and summary of psychometric properties,” in Measuring Emotional Intelligence: Common Ground and Controversy, ed G. Geher (Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers, 111–142.

Google Scholar

Bar-On, R. (2006). The bar-on model of emotional-social intelligence. Psicothema 18, 13–25.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bar-On, R. (2016). “Beyond IQ and EQ: the Bar-On multifactor model of performance,” in The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment, ed U. Kumar (London: John Wiley and Sons, 104–118.

Google Scholar

Bar-On, R. (2018). The multifactor measure of performance: its development, norming, and validation. Front. Psychol. 9:140. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-On, R., Handley, R., and Fund, S. (2006). “The impact of emotional intelligence on performance,” in Linking Emotional Intelligence and Performance at Work, eds V. U. Druskat, F. Sala, and G. Mont (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 3–19.

Google Scholar

Boyatzis, R. E., and Jack, A. I. (2018). The neuroscience of coaching. Consult. Psychol. J. Pract. Res. 70, 11–27. doi: 10.1037/cpb0000095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, J. C., Dominick, P. G., Smither, J. W., and Reilly, R. R. (2007). Examination of the discriminant, convergent, and criterion-related validity of self-ratings on the emotional competence inventory. Soc. Sci. Electr. Publ. 15, 341–353. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2389.2007.00393.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Côté, S., and Miners, C. T. H. (2006). Emotional intelligence, cognitive intelligence, and job performance. Adm. Sci. Q. 51, 1–28. doi: 10.2189/asqu.51.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cumming, G. (2012). Understanding the New Statistics: Effect Sizes, Confidence Intervals, and Meta-Analysis. New York, NY: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203807002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cumming, G. (2014). The new statistics: why and how. Psychol. Sci. 25, 7–29. doi: 10.1177/0956797613504966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, M., Stankov, L., and Roberts, R. D. (1998). Emotional intelligence: in search of an elusive construct. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75:989. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, S. K., and Nichols, R. (2016). Does Emotional Intelligence have a “Dark” Side? A review of the literature. Front. Psychol. 7:1316. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domínguez-García, E., and Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2018). The association between emotional intelligence and suicidal behavior: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 9:2380. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dovidio, J. F., and Gaertner, S. L. (1986). “Prejudice, discrimination, and racism: historical trends and contemporary approaches,” in Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism, eds J. F. Dovidio and S. L. Gaertner (San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 1–34.

Dulewicz, V., Higgs, M., and Slaski, M. (2003). Measuring emotional intelligence, content, construct and criterion related validity. J. Manage. Psychol. 18, 405–420. doi: 10.1108/02683940310484017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Extremera, N., and Rey, L. (2016). Ability emotional intelligence and life satisfaction: positive and negative affect as mediators. Pers. Individ. Dif. 102, 98–101. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabio, A. D. (2015). Beyond fluid intelligence and personality traits in social support: the role of ability based emotional intelligence. Front Psychol. 6:395. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanelli, D., Costas, R., and Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2017). Meta-assessment of bias in science. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 3714–3719. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1618569114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanelli, D., and Ioannidis, J. P. (2013). US studies may overestimate effect sizes in softer research. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 15031–15036. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302997110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiori, M., and Antonakis, J. (2011). The ability model of emotional intelligence: searching for valid measures. Pers. Individ. Dif. 50, 329–334. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, P. T., Lancaster, J. L., Laird, A. R., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2014). Meta-analysis in human neuroimaging: computational modeling of large-scale databases. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 37, 409–434. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francis, G. (2014). The frequency of excess success for articles in psychological science. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21, 1180–1187. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0601-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A. (2008). Personality and Intelligence at Work. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional Intelligence. New York, NY: Bantam Books.

Goleman, D. (1996). Emotional intelligence: why it can matter more than IQ. Learning 24, 217–236.

Goleman, D. (1998). Working with Emotional Intelligence. New York, NY: Bantam Books.

Grieve, R., and Panebianco, L. (2013). Assessing the role of aggression, empathy, and self-serving cognitive distortions in trait emotional manipulation. Aust. J. Psychol. 65, 79–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-9536.2012.00059.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grunes, P., Gudmundsson, A., and Irmer, B. (2014). To what extent is the Mayer and Salovey (1997) model of emotional intelligence a useful predictor of leadership style and perceived leadership outcomes in Australian educational institutions? Educ. Manage. Admin. Leadersh. 42, 112–135. doi: 10.1177/1741143213499255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartgerink, C., and Pernet, C. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349:aac4716. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4716

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Joseph, D. L., Jing, J., Newman, D. A., and O'Boyle, E. H. (2015). Why does self-reported emotional intelligence predict job performance? A meta-analytic investigation of mixed EI. J. Appl. Psychol. 100, 298–342. doi: 10.1037/a0037681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemp, A., Stephan, B., Hopkinson, P., Sumich, A., Paul, R., Richard Clark, C., et al. (2005). Toward an integrated profile of depression: evidence from the brain resource international database. J. Integr. Neurosci. 4, 95–106. doi: 10.1142/S0219635205000665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Law, K. S., Wong, C. S., and Song, L. J. (2004). The construct and criterion validity of emotional intelligence and its potential utility for management studies. J. Appl. Psychol. 89, 483–496. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.89.3.483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linden, D. V. D., Schermer, J. A., Zeeuw, E. D., Dunkel, C. S., Pekaar, K. A., Bakker, A. B., et al. (2017). Overlap between the general factor of personality and trait emotional intelligence: a genetic correlation study. Behav. Genet. 48, 1–8. doi: 10.1007/s10519-017-9885-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Malouff, J. M., Schutte, N. S., and Thorsteinsson, E. B. (2014). Trait emotional intelligence and romantic relationship satisfaction: a meta-analysis. Am. J. Family Therap. 42, 53–66. doi: 10.1080/01926187.2012.748549

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinez-Pons, M. (1999–2000). Emotional intelligence as a self-regulatory process: a social cognitive view. Imagination Cognit. Per. 19, 331–350. doi: 10.2190/WVMC-AEF1-T3XX-8P7A.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Martins, A., Ramalho, N., and Morin, E. (2010). A comprehensive meta-analysis of the relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Pers. Individ. Dif. 49, 554–564. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., and Solovey, P. (1999). Emotional intelligence meets traditional standard for an intelligence. Intelligence 27, 267–298. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00016-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., and Salovey, P. (1997). “What is emotional intelligence?” in Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence: Implications for Educators, eds P. Salovey and D. Sluyter (New York, NY: Basic Books, 3–31.

Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., and Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) User' s Manual. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems.

Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D. R., and Sitarenios, G. (2003). Measuring emotional intelligence with the MSCEIT V 2.0. Emotion 3, 97–105. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.97

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Megias, A., Gomez-Leal, R., Gutierrez-Cobo, M. J., Cabello, R., and Fernandez-Berrocal, P. (2018). The relationship between trait psychopathy and emotional intelligence: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 84, 198–203. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miguel, E., Camerer, C., Casey, K., Cohen, J., Esterling, K. M., Gerber, A., et al. (2014). Promoting transparency in social science research. Science 343, 30–31. doi: 10.1126/science.1245317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, D. A., Joseph, D. L., and Maccann, C. (2010). Emotional intelligence and job performance: the importance of emotion regulation and emotional labor context. Ind. Organ. Psychol. 3, 159–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1754-9434.2010.01218.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulhus, D. L. (2010). Self-report measures of intelligence: are they useful as proxy IQ tests? J. Pers. 66, 525–554. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Peña-Sarrionandia, A., Mikolajczak, M., and Gross, J. J. (2015). Integrating emotion regulation and emotional intelligence traditions: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 6:160. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Perera, H. N., and DiGiacomo, M. (2013). The relationship of trait emotional intelligence with academic performance: a meta-analytic review. Learn. Individ. Diff. 28, 20–33. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Peterson, K., Malouff, J., and Thorsteinsson, E. B. (2011). A meta-analytic investigation of emotional intelligence and alcohol involvement. Substance Use Misuse 46, 1726–1733. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2011.618962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V., Pérez-González, J. C., and Furnham, A. (2007). On the criterion and incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence. Cognit. Emot. 21, 26–55. doi: 10.1080/02699930601038912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Pietschnig, J., and Gittler, G. (2016). Is ability-based emotional intelligence impervious to the Flynn effect? A cross-temporal meta-analysis (2001–2015). Intelligence. 61, 37–45. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2016.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pietschnig, J., Voracek, M., and Formann, A. K. (2010). Mozart effect–shmozart effect: a meta-analysis. Intelligence 38, 314–323. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2010.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Protzko, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2015). “Decline effects: types, mechanisms, and personal reflections,” in Psychological Science Under Scrutiny, eds Scott O. Lilienfeld and Irwin D. Waldman (Chichester, UK: Wiley, 87–109.

Google Scholar

Radford, M. (2012). Emotional intelligence as a predictor of nursing student success. J. Nurs. 2, 23–29. doi: 10.26634/jnur.2.3.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rand, D. G., Greene, J. D., and Nowak, M. A. (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature 489, 427–430. doi: 10.1038/nature11467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rooy, D. L. V., and Viswesvaran, C. (2004). Emotional intelligence: a meta-analytic investigation of predictive validity and nomological net. J. Vocat. Behav. 65, 71–95. doi: 10.1016/S0001-8791(03)00076-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sala, F. (2002). Emotional Competence Inventory (ECI): Technical manual. Boston, MA: Hay Group.

Google Scholar

Salovey, P., and Mayer, J. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 9, 185–211. doi: 10.2190/DUGG-P24E-52WK-6CDG

CrossRef Full Text

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S., Turvey, C., and Palfai, T. (1995). “Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: exploring emotional intelligence using the trait meta-mood scale,” in Emotion, Disclosure, and Health, ed J. W. Pennebaker (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 125–154.

Google Scholar

Schooler, J. (2011). Unpublished results hide the decline effect. Nature 470:437. doi: 10.1038/470437a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schooler, J. W. (2014). Metascience could rescue the ‘replication crisis’. Nature 515:9. doi: 10.1038/515009a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., and Bhullar, N. (2009). “The assessing emotions scale,” in eds Assessing Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Research, and Applications, eds C. Stough, D. H. Saklofske, and J. D. A. Parker (New York, NY: Springer), 119–134. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-88370-0_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., and Golden, C. J. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Pers. Individ. Dif. 25, 167–177. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00001-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., Bhullar, N., and Rooke, S. E. (2007). A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Pers. Individ. Dif. 42, 921–933. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, R. R. (2008). Emotional Intelligence from 17th Century to 21st Century: perspectives and directions for future research. Vision J. Business Perspect. 12, 59–66. doi: 10.1177/097226290801200108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, R. R. (2011). An empirical investigation into the role of eq/emotional intelligence competencies in mental well-being. Vision J. Business Perspect. 15, 177–191. doi: 10.1177/097226291101500208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spellman, B. A. (2015). A Short (Personal) future history of revolution 2.0. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. J. Assoc. Psychol. Sci. 10:886. doi: 10.1177/1745691615609918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterne, J., and Egger, M. (2006). Regression methods to detect publication and other bias in meta-analysis. Publi. Bias Meta Anal. Prevent. Assess. Adjust. 99–110. doi: 10.1002/0470870168.ch6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart-Brown, S. (1998). Emotional wellbeing and its relation to health. BMJ, 317, 1608–1609. doi: 10.1136/bmj.317.7173.1608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swaddle, J. P., Witter, M. S., and Cuthill, I. C. (1994). The analysis of fluctuating asymmetry. Anim. Behav. 48, 986–989. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1994.1327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Nagase, T., et al. (2013). White matter structures associated with emotional intelligence: evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1025–1034. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tapia, M. (2001). Measuring emotional intelligence. Psychol. Rep. 88, 353–364. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2001.88.2.353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsaousis, I., and Nikolaou, I. (2005). Exploring the relationship of emotional intelligence with physical and psychological health functioning. Stress Health 21, 77–86. doi: 10.1002/smi.1042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldman, D. A., Balthazard, P. A., and Peterson, S. J. (2011). Leadership and neuroscience: can we revolutionize the way that inspirational leaders are identified and developed? Acad. Manage. Perspect. 25, 60–74. doi: 10.5465/amp.25.1.60

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeidner, M., Roberts, R. D., and Matthews, G. (1998). The science of emotional intelligence. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 14, 281–285. 1

Google Scholar

*An asterisk denotes a study included in the meta-analysis.

Keywords: emotional intelligence, decline effects, ability emotional intelligence, mixed emotional intelligence, replicability crisis

Citation: Gong Z and Jiao X (2019) Are Effect Sizes in Emotional Intelligence Field Declining? A Meta-Meta Analysis. Front. Psychol. 10:1655. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01655

Received: 17 February 2019; Accepted: 01 July 2019;
Published: 16 July 2019.

Edited by:

Radha R. Sharma, Management Development Institute, India

Reviewed by:

María del Mar Molero, University of Almería, Spain
Xavier Fernández-i-Marín, Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany

Copyright © 2019 Gong and Jiao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Zhun Gong, gongzhun2001@163.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.