Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 02 August 2019
Sec. Comparative Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Community Series: The Science and Practice of Captive Animal Welfare, volume I View all 10 articles

Providing Belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) in Controlled Environments Opportunities to Thrive: Health, Self-Maintenance, Species-Specific Behavior, and Choice and Control

\nHeather M. Hill
Heather M. Hill1*Hendrik NollensHendrik Nollens2
  • 1Psychology Department, St. Mary's University, San Antonio, TX, United States
  • 2SeaWorld Parks and Entertainment, Orlando, FL, United States

The health and welfare of animals in controlled environments continues to improve as evidence-based practices inform best care processes (Ward et al., 2018; Wolfensohn et al., 2018). Zoos and aquariums play an integral role in educating the public about Earth's fauna and flora (Maple and Perdue, 2018). Zoo and aquariums are learning environments for visitors and scientists, who come because of their appreciation for the animals and who trust that they are provided with the best possible welfare. Zoological facilities must continue to systematically and empirically measure animal welfare so science can inform decisions ensuring the optimal health and veterinary care practices along with creating environments that promote species-specific behavior and interactions, choice, and variability (Maple and Perdue, 2018; Ward et al., 2018; Wolfensohn et al., 2018). With increasing partnerships between external researchers and zoological facilities, the scientific investigation, and understanding of the components of animal welfare continues to expand (Fernandez and Timberlake, 2008; Ward et al., 2018). For example, academic-zoological partnerships have established that naturalistic exhibits can promote species-specific behaviors (e.g., Finlay et al., 1988; Ogden et al., 1990; Yilmaz et al., 2010; Chih Mun et al., 2013), and the placement of different types of enrichment promotes investigative behavior and greater habitat use (e.g., Vick et al., 2000; Clark, 2017; Makecha and Highfill, 2018). Research has also indicated that the needs of a given species and the individuals making up the facility's population must be considered when utilizing different types of enrichment, social groupings, or habitat configurations (Rose and Croft, 2015; Wolfensohn et al., 2018; Nollens et al., 2019).

The behavior and social interactions of a relatively stable white whale, or beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) population at a North American facility has been studied systematically since 2007. From long-term weekly documentation calf behavioral development and maternal care, behavioral milestones, such as nursing, swim positions, locomotor development, social interactions, and play behaviors have been established and validated with belugas from other facilities and in their natural habitat (Krasnova et al., 2006, 2009; Hill, 2009; Karenina et al., 2010, 2013; Hill et al., 2013, 2018a; Hill and Campbell, 2014; Hill and Ramirez, 2014). Maternal care behaviors are conserved across both habitats (Krasnova et al., 2006, 2009; Hill, 2009; Karenina et al., 2010, 2013; Hill et al., 2013, 2017), but show individual variation much like bottlenose dolphins in human care or the wild (Hill et al., 2007; Gibson and Mann, 2008; Stanton and Mann, 2012).

Results illustrated that dynamic social groupings decreased time spent swimming alone and provided opportunities for species-specific behavior, such as engaging with the environment whether it was interacting with different objects (e.g., enrichment devices, permanent fixtures, organic materials, or water) or with each other (Hill and Ramirez, 2014; Hill et al., 2015b, 2018b). Pool configuration also influenced the actions of the belugas (unpublished data). When multiple pools were available, the belugas engaged in more dynamic and variable swim patterns. Moreover, access to multiple pools provided opportunities for choice and self-maintenance: individual belugas could move out of sight of conspecifics or choose a specific area in which to swim. More recently, based on data from three different facilities, male-male interactions (whether interspecific or intraspecific) have emerged as a significant element in beluga socialization, appearing early in the juvenile years and solidifying as the males age (Glabicky et al., 2010; Hill et al., 2015a, 2018b; Mazokowski et al., 2018). These socialization patterns appear to be conserved as well based on genetic work on beluga distributions in several beluga stocks (reviewed by Colbeck et al., 2013; O'Corry-Crowe et al., 2018).

Social composition, complexity of environment through pool configurations, opportunities for intra- and inter-species interactions, and variable access to different forms of enrichment have produced measurable behavioral and physiological outcomes that are indicative of enhanced welfare of belugas in human care. These findings corroborate those found with a number of terrestrial species in zoological environments (Finlay et al., 1988; Ogden et al., 1990; Vick et al., 2000; Yilmaz et al., 2010; Chih Mun et al., 2013; Clark, 2017; Makecha and Highfill, 2018; Maple and Perdue, 2018; Wolfensohn et al., 2018). Overall, dynamic and variable experiences increased the belugas' activity (i.e., intra- and inter-species interactions, play, engagement with environmental stimuli), reduced periods of solitary swims, and provided opportunities to thrive.

The physiological health of the majority of the belugas present in the population has remained stable and strong, and as such, the population has served as a baseline reference for evaluating free-ranging belugas (Norman et al., 2012, 2013). Belugas are a species of great interest because of their vulnerability to climate change, role as a sentinel species, importance to subsistence, and for understanding threats to at-risk populations. Not only do belugas in controlled environments serve as control populations, but the beluga husbandry expertise provided from aquaria has, in part, allowed for the safe and expedient collection of blood, feces, gastric contents, skin, blubber, blowhole swabs, breath exhalations, as well as morphological, auditory, and ultrasound measurements from 56 belugas in Bristol Bay, AK. Combined, the information gained has advanced our understanding of free-ranging beluga health, physiology, disease exposure, immunology, body condition, hearing, habitat use, and the risks free-ranging belugas face.

After almost 12 years of consistent weekly observations, this academic-zoological collaboration has produced a substantial body of behavioral evidence indicating that beluga welfare is enhanced through variability and complexity with regard to social compositions, enrichment, and habitat configurations. This evidence has been further substantiated by physiological parameters that have been used as controls for assessing the health status of free-ranging belugas. However, as summarized by both Maple and Perdue (2018) and Wolfensohn et al. (2018), individual differences and preferences must be considered in any welfare assessment. Responses to enrichment attempts are variable and subject to the individuals themselves, their current motivational state, and other environmental factors that may or may not be identifiable (e.g., social status change, subclinical illness). Between learning about the needs of the animals themselves and how that knowledge might benefit both captive and wild populations, this long-term study of a reproductively active beluga population that simulates the natural composition of beluga social groupings has demonstrated that variability and choice in different aspects of the daily environment are more central to beluga welfare than the permanent habitat itself. Active collaborations between external researchers (academic researchers in this case) and zoological facilities, such as in the case of this long-term beluga behavioral study, are essential for maintaining and improving animal welfare in human care and educating current and future generations on the need to care for and conserve our planet's resources.

Author Contributions

HH drafted the text regarding the body of behavioral research summarized in the article. HN edited and summarized the research discussing the physiological and biological contributions of research performed with belugas in controlled environments and applied to their wild counterparts.

Funding

Partial funding was received by HH from various internal faculty development grants granted by St. Mary's University over the course of the long-term study. Additional support was received by two undergraduate research assistants through Psi Chi Honor Society summer research program, with complimentary mentor support for HH.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

HH would like to thank SeaWorld Parks and Entertainment for their continued support of the research that is discussed in this article. HH would also like to thank the many research assistants who helped to collect, code, and analyze the data gathered at SeaWorld San Antonio. St. Mary's University provided financial support to HH across the years to initiate and continue the study.

References

Chih Mun, J. S., Kabilan, B., Alagappasamy, S., and Guha, B. (2013). Benefits of naturalistic free-ranging primate displays and implications for increased human–primate interactions. Anthrozoös 26, 13–26. doi: 10.2752/175303713X13534238631353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, F. E. (2017). Cognitive enrichment and welfare: current approaches and future directions. Anim. Behav.Cogn. 4, 52–71. doi: 10.12966/abc.05.02.2017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colbeck, G. J., Duchesne, P., Postma, L. D., Lesage, V., Hammill, M. O., and Turgeon, J. (2013). Groups of related belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) travel together during their seasonal migrations in and around Hudson Bay. Proc. R. Soc. Biol. 280, 2552–2561. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez, E. J., and Timberlake, W. (2008). Mutual benefits of research collaborations between zoos and academic institutions. Zoo Biol. 27, 470–487. doi: 10.1002/zoo.20215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finlay, T., James, L. R., and Maple, T. L. (1988). People's perceptions of animals: the influence of zoo environment. Environ. Behav. 20, 508–528. doi: 10.1177/0013916588204008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, Q. A., and Mann, J. (2008). Early social development in wild bottlenose dolphins: sex differences, individual variation and maternal influence. Anim. Behav. 76, 375–387. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glabicky, N., DuBrava, A., and Noonan, M. (2010). Social–sexual behavior seasonality in captive beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas). Polar Biol. 33, 1145–1147. doi: 10.1007/s00300-010-0790-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, H. (2009). The behavioral development of two beluga calves during the first year of life. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 22, 234–253.

Google Scholar

Hill, H., and Campbell, C. (2014). Allocare depends on social composition for belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) in human care. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 27, 501–514.

Google Scholar

Hill, H., Campbell, C., Dalton, L., and Osborn, S. (2013). The first year of behavioral development and maternal care of beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) calves in human care. Zoo Biol. 32, 565–570. doi: 10.1002/zoo.21093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, H., Dietrich, S., Yeater, D., McKinnon, M., Miller, M., Aibel, S., et al. (2015a). Developing an ethogram of sexual and socio-sexual behaviors of beluga whales in the care of humans. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2, 105–123. doi: 10.12966/abc.05.01.2015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, H., Guarino, S., Crandall, S., Lenhart, E., and Dietrich, S. (2015b). Young belugas diversify adult beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) behavior. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2, 267–284. doi: 10.12966/abc.08.06.2015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, H., and Ramirez, D. (2014). Adults play but not like their young: the frequency and types of play by belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) in human care. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 1, 166–185. doi: 10.12966/abc.05.07.2014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, H. M., Dietrich, S., Guarino, S., Banda, M., and Lacy, K. (2018a). Preliminary observations of an unusual mouth interaction between beluga calves (Delphinapterus leucas). Zoo Biol. 38, 149–156. doi: 10.1002/zoo.21463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, H. M., Garcia de Oliveira Silva-Gruber, D., and Noonan, M. (2018b). Sex-specific social affiliation in captive beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas). Aquat. Mamm. 44, 250–255. doi: 10.1578/AM.44.3.2018.250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, H. M., Greer, T., Solangi, M., and Kuczaj, S. A. II. (2007). Evidence for maternal styles in captive bottlenose dolphins. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 20, 34–53.

Google Scholar

Hill, H. M., Guarino, S., Calvillo, A., Gonzalez, A., Zuniga, K., Bellows, C., et al. (2017). Lateralized swim positions are conserved across environments for beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) mother–calf pairs. Behav. Processes 138, 22–28. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karenina, K., Giljov, A., Baranov, V., Osipova, L., Krasnova, V., and Malashichev, Y. (2010). Visual laterality of calf–mother interactions in wild whales. PLoS ONE 5:e13787. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karenina, K., Giljov, A., Glazov, D., and Malashichev, Y. (2013). Social laterality in wild beluga whale infants: comparisons between locations, escort conditions, and ages. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 1195–1204. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.09.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krasnova, V. V., Bel'kovich, V. M., and Chernetsky, A. D. (2006). Mother-infant spatial relations in wild beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) during postnatal development under natural conditions. Biol. Bull. 33, 53–58. doi: 10.1134/S1062359006010079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krasnova, V. V., Bel'kovich, V. M., and Chernetsky, A. D. (2009). Formation of behavior in the White Sea beluga calf, Delphinapterus leucas, during early postnatal ontogenesis. Russ. J. Mar. Biol. 35, 53–59. doi: 10.1134/S1063074009010088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makecha, R. N., and Highfill, L. E. (2018). Environmental enrichment, marine mammals, and animal welfare: a brief review. Aquat. Mamm. 44, 221–230. doi: 10.1578/AM.44.2.2018.221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maple, T., and Perdue, B. (2018). Zoo Animal Welfare. Heidelberg: Springer.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mazokowski, L., Hill, H., and Noonan, M. (2018). Juvenile belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) exhibit sex-specific social affiliations. Aquat. Mamm. 44, 500–505. doi: 10.1578/AM.44.5.2018.500

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nollens, B. A., Burtis, K., Price, M., Felice, M., and Nollens, H. H. (2019). “Transitioning to a mixed species habitat influences killer whale (Orcinus orca) time budgets and enhances killer whale welfare,” in Proceedings of the Association of Zoos & Aquariums and International Marine Animal Trainer Association Joint Conference (New Orleans, LA).

Google Scholar

Norman, S. A., Beckett, L. A., Miller, W. A., Leger, J. S., and Hobbs, R. C. (2013). Variation in hematologic and serum biochemical values of belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) under managed care. J. Zoo Wildl. Med. 44, 376–388. doi: 10.1638/2012-0172R

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, S. A., Goertz, C. E., Burek, K. A., Quakenbush, L. T., Cornick, L. A., Romano, T. A., et al. (2012). Seasonal hematology and serum chemistry of wild beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas) in Bristol Bay, Alaska, USA. J. Wildl. Dis. 48, 21–32. doi: 10.7589/0090-3558-48.1.21

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Corry-Crowe, G., Suydam, R., Quakenbush, L., Potgieter, B., Harwood, L., Litovka, D., et al. (2018). Migratory culture, population structure and stock identity in North Pacific beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas). PLoS ONE 13:e0194201. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0194201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogden, J. J., Finlay, T. W., and Maple, T. L. (1990). Gorilla adaptations to naturalistic environments. Zoo Biol. 9, 107–121. doi: 10.1002/zoo.1430090205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, P. E., and Croft, D. P. (2015). The potential of Social Network Analysis as a tool for the management of zoo animals. Anim. Welfare 24, 123–138. doi: 10.7120/09627286.24.2.123

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanton, M. A., and Mann, J. (2012). Early social networks predict survival in wild bottlenose dolphins. PLoS ONE 7:e47508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vick, S. J., Anderson, J. R., and Young, R. (2000). Maracas for Macaca? Evaluation of three potential enrichment objects in two species of zoo-housed macaques. Zoo Biol. 19, 181–191. doi: 10.1002/1098-2361(2000)19:3<181::AID-ZOO2>3.0.CO;2-W

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, S. J., Sherwen, S., and Clark, F. E. (2018). Advances in applied zoo animal welfare science. J. Appl. Anim. Welfare Sci. 21, 23–33. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2018.1513842

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfensohn, S., Shotton, J., Bowley, H., Davies, S., Thompson, S., and Justice, W. (2018). Assessment of welfare in zoo animals: towards optimum quality of life. Animals 8, 1–16. doi: 10.3390/ani8070110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yilmaz, S., Mumcu, S., and Özbilen, A. (2010). Effects of spatial differences on visitor perceptions at zoo exhibits. Sci. Res. Essays 5, 2327–2340.

Google Scholar

Keywords: beluga (Delphinapterus leucas), social composition, welfare, variability, choice

Citation: Hill HM and Nollens H (2019) Providing Belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) in Controlled Environments Opportunities to Thrive: Health, Self-Maintenance, Species-Specific Behavior, and Choice and Control. Front. Psychol. 10:1776. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01776

Received: 01 May 2019; Accepted: 17 July 2019;
Published: 02 August 2019.

Edited by:

Bonnie Perdue, Georgia State University, United States

Reviewed by:

Eduardo J. Fernandez, Florida Institute of Technology, United States

Copyright © 2019 Hill and Nollens. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Heather M. Hill, hhill1@stmarytx.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.